<<

bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

1

2

3 shapes biological rhythms and promotes temporal niche

4 differentiation in a simulation

5

6 Resource competition, biological rhythms, and temporal niches

7

8 Vance Difan Gao1,2*, Sara Morley-Fletcher1,4, Stefania Maccari1,3,4, Martha Hotz Vitaterna2, Fred W. Turek2

9

10 1UMR 8576 Unité de Glycobiologie Structurale et Fonctionnelle, Campus Cité Scientifique, CNRS, University of 11 Lille, Lille, France

12 2 Center for Sleep and Circadian Biology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States of America

13 3Department of Medico-Surgical Sciences and Biotechnologies, University Sapienza of Rome, Rome, Italy

14 4International Associated Laboratory (LIA) “Perinatal Stress and Neurodegenerative Diseases”: University of Lille, 15 Lille, France; CNRS-UMR 8576, Lille, France; Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy; IRCCS Neuromed, Pozzilli, 16 Italy

17

18

19 * Corresponding author

20 E-mail: [email protected]

21

1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

22 Abstract

23 Competition for resources often contributes strongly to defining an organism’s . Biological

24 rhythms are important to the temporal dimension of niches, but the role of other organisms in

25 determining such temporal niches have not been much studied, and the role specifically of competition even

26 less so. We investigate how interspecific and intraspecific competition for resources shapes an organism’s

27 activity rhythms. For this, communities of one or two species in an environment with limited resource input

28 were simulated. We demonstrate that when organisms are arrhythmic, one species will always be competitively

29 excluded from the environment, but the existence of activity rhythms allows and indefinite

30 coexistence of the two species. Two species which are initially active at the same phase will differentiate their

31 phase angle of entrainment over time to avoid each other. When only one species is present in an environment,

32 competition within individuals of the species strongly selects for niche expansion through arrhythmicity, but the

33 addition of an interspecific competitor facilitates evolution of increased rhythmic amplitude when combined

34 with additional adaptations for temporal specialization. Finally, if individuals preferentially mate with others

35 who are active at similar times of day, then by intraspecific competition can split one

36 population into two reproductively isolated groups. In summary, these simulations suggest that biological

37 rhythms are an effective method to temporally differentiate ecological niches, and that competition is an

38 important ecological pressure promoting the evolution of rhythms and sleep.

39

40 Author summary

41 Why do we sleep? We are interested in the ecological factors which promote the evolution of biological

42 rhythms like the sleep-wake cycle, focusing especially on competition. When animals compete with each other

43 for resources, they often evolve to avoid each other by specializing to use different resources or separating their

2 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

44 activity in other ways. To test hypotheses about how competition shapes rest-activity rhythms, we performed

45 computer simulations of a community of animals who move, reproduce, and compete for resources. We show

46 that biological rhythms let two species divide time so that one species is active while its competitor is resting,

47 thus avoiding depleting shared resources. When a species has no competitors in the simulation, competition

48 between members of the same species cause population and individual rhythms to decrease, since resource

49 availability is low when everybody is active at the same time. However, having competitors allows strong

50 rhythms to evolve from originally arrhythmic organisms. Competition can even cause a single population to split

51 into two species which are separated in time. In summary, these results suggest that competition is a strong

52 factor promoting rest-activity rhythms.

53

54 Introduction

55 An ecological niche is the particular “space” a species occupies in an , comprising a diversity of

56 concepts including the physical environment where it lives, the resources it uses, its interactions with other

57 species, and the adaptations it has evolved to exploit the niche. Both theory and evidence indicate that

58 competition is a major factor which determines species’ niches [1,2]. Competitors for a resource generally will

59 adjust to avoid each other, differentiating their niches by adapting to utilize different types of resources, thus

60 avoiding the depletion of shared resources (exploitative competition) [3] as well as costly acts of aggression and

61 territorial exclusion (interference competition) [4].

62 Time is one dimension which defines a niche. Most species live in environments dominated by a day-night

63 cycle and a seasonal cycle, though other cycles such as tides can also strongly define an ecosystem [5]. A

64 temporal niche is defined by both abiotic elements such as light intensity and temperature [6,7], as well the as

65 by predictable temporal patterns of other organisms [8]. Adaptions for a specific temporal niche include for

66 example specializations in vision, coat color, temperature regulation, and importantly, biological rhythms, i.e.

3 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

67 physiological and behavioral traits which cycle regularly according to an internal biological clock . In this article,

68 we consider activity rhythms through the lens of ecological niche theory, viewing them as a way partition niches

69 among competitors and examining how competition shapes the expression of these biological clocks. Time can

70 be thus viewed as a sort of resource or space which is partitioned by organisms in response to competition [9],

71 both interspecific (between different species) and intraspecific (between members of the same species) [10].

72 While some work has examined ’s effect on animals’ activity rhythms [11–13], the role of competition

73 has been less examined.

74 A variety of field studies have observed evidence of temporal partitioning between putative competitors.

75 For example, a community of three sympatric species of insectivorous were observed to have activity

76 profiles which are displaced from each other, suggesting that they avoid each other [14]. While Argentinian

77 pampas foxes are normally nocturnal, they show a modified half-nocturnal half-diurnal profile in a region where

78 they overlap with crab-eating foxes [15]. Other examples of apparent temporal partitioning have been seen at

79 watering holes [16,17], in gerbils [18], carabid beetles [19], and large African [20]. Stronger

80 evidence comes from semi-natural manipulation experiments. Common spiny mice are nocturnal and golden

81 spiny mice are diurnal in areas where the two species coexist, but when the common spiny mice are

82 experimentally removed, the originally diurnal golden spiny mice shift into , suggesting that this

83 species was pushed into by the competing species [9]. It should be recognized that these examples all

84 occur at the behavioral and ecological time scale, i.e. they are “temporary” displacements from the species’

85 genetically determined preferred activity time. Although certain pairs of taxa are suggestive of evolutionary-

86 level temporal partitioning—nocturnal and diurnal hawks, for instance, or the hypothesized nocturnal

87 origin of mammals [21]—the role of competition in causing these evolutionary divergences is near impossible to

88 establish conclusively. Nonetheless, it is probable that temporal shifts at the behavioral/ecological level cause

89 selection for alleles suited for the new time period, eventually translating into long-term changes at the

90 genetic/evolutionary level.

4 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

91 In this work, we explore ways which competition in ecological niche theory can be applied to biological

92 rhythms, using simulation of a community of two species who compete for limited resources in an environment.

93 We first demonstrate that circadian rhythms are a form of niche differentiation which allows two species to

94 stably coexist without competitive exclusion, and that two rhythmic species will quickly differentiate their

95 circadian phases to avoid competition. We show that arrhythmicity is preferred when there is no interspecific

96 competitor present, but the presence of interspecific competition greatly facilitates the development of strong

97 activity rhythms. Finally, we show that if organisms have assortative mating based on circadian timing, then

98 intraspecific competition can drive the division of one population into two mating groups, contributing to

99 speciation.

100

101 Results

102 Briefly, we simulated a community of two species in an environment using Python. Each individual organism

103 has a circadian activity rhythm which is modeled as a cosine wave with two parameters: amplitude ranging from

104 0 to 1, and phase angle ranging from 0° to 360° (Fig 1). Resource input into the environment occurs at a constant

105 rate. If an individual encounters a resource, it consumes that resource and gains energy, which is in turn used for

106 movement and basal metabolism. Death occurs if an individual’s energy drops to zero or if the individual reaches

107 an age limit. Organisms reproduce sexually, and the probability of an individual reproducing is proportional to its

108 energy. During procreation, an offspring’s amplitude and phase are the means of the parents’ traits. When

109 testing the evolution of a trait, the offspring’s trait is the parents’ mean plus or minus a small random variance,

110 which naturally results in and evolution.

111

5 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

112 Fig 1. Example activity rhythm profiles. Activity profiles are modeled as cosine waves with two properties,

113 amplitude and phase angle. Two example profiles are shown. In blue, the activity rhythm has amplitude = 1 and

114 phase angle = 0°. In orange, amplitude = 0.4 and phase angle = 90°. Regardless of changes in circadian

115 parameters, the number of moves per day is fixed at 20 (equivalent to the area under each curve).

116

117 Rhythmic activity prevents competitive exclusion

118 The competitive exclusion principle asserts that two species cannot occupy the same ecological niche

119 indefinitely; if two species exist in exactly the same niche, then one species will eventually outcompete the

120 other and become the sole occupier of the niche. We modeled this scenario by simulating two species with no

121 circadian rhythms, i.e. with amplitude = 0. The two species are constantly active at a moderate level and thus

122 have no differentiation of temporal niches. In this situation, one species invariably dies out (Fig 2A-B). If both

123 species are rhythmic (amplitude = 1) and are active at the same time of day (same phase angle), there is again

124 no niche differentiation, and one species will eventually die out (Fig 2C-D). However, if both species are rhythmic

125 but are active during opposite times of day, then both species are likely to continue existing (Fig 2E-F). In 2 of

126 the 40 trials of this scenario, one species died out after some time, demonstrating that random fluctuations can

127 still cause a species to disappear. In the remaining 38 trials, both species survived at the end of 2000 days.

128

129 Fig 2. Circadian rhythms prevent competitive exclusion. (A) When two species with no activity rhythms

130 (amplitude = 0) are placed in the environment, one species will always die out. An example of an individual trial

131 is displayed. (B) Summary of 40 trials. Since the two species’ population levels tend to be symmetric, only the

132 species which is excluded is plotted for clarity. The trials were sorted by the day that competitive exclusion is

133 reached, and the median (20th) trial is represented by the dark line, while the 4th, 12th, 28th, and 36th trials are

134 represented by faint lines. (C) When two species have circadian rhythms (amplitude = 1) and are active at the

6 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

135 same time of day, one species will always die out, as before. Example individual trial. (D) Summary of 40 trials.

136 (E) When two species have circadian rhythms are active 180° out of phase, the two species can coexist

137 indefinitely. Example individual trial. (F) Summary of multiple trials. Since no species died out in this scenario,

138 five population profiles were selected at random to be plotted.

139

140 Character displacement of phase-angle

141 Having showed that being active at opposite phase angles is favorable for the coexistence of two species, we

142 ask whether species can evolve over time to reach such a state of temporal separation. When two similar

143 species occupying similar niches are found in the same geographical area, the differences between them tend to

144 become accentuated in order to distinguish their niches, in a phenomenon known as character displacement

145 [22]. We tested whether two circadian-rhythmic species would display character displacement on a temporal

146 axis. In this scenario, all individuals started at approximately at the same phase angle at the beginning of the

147 simulation, and the offspring’s phase angles were allowed to vary and thus evolve, while amplitudes were fixed

148 at 1.

149 The two species’ phase angles of activity did indeed diverge over time in the simulation (Fig 3A). After

150 starting at 0° separation, the phase-angle difference between the two species grew and eventually reached the

151 maximum of 180° and remained so thereafter (Fig 3B). The source of selective pressure is the difference in

152 resource availability between peak and peak-adjacent times (Fig 3C). During times of peak activity, available

153 resources are quickly consumed and resource density is thus very low, while in times adjacent to the peak, less

154 resources are consumed, and resource density is higher. The result of this time-resource gradient is that

155 individuals whose phase angles are at the edges of the population distribution have access to a more resource-

156 dense environment and thus gain more energy (Fig 3D). Since stored energy is proportional to reproduction

157 probability, the individuals at distribution’s edges reproduce more, and the species’ mean phase angle is shifted.

7 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

158

159 Fig 3. Character displacement of circadian phase. (A) Circular histogram displaying the separation of phase

160 angles of activity in two species over time in one trial. The difference in the two species’ phase angles quickly

161 diverges to 180°. (B) Summary of 40 trials. Trials were sorted by days until a phase difference of 150° was

162 reached; the median 20th trial is plotted in a dark line, while the 4th, 12th, 28th, and 36th trials are plotted in faint

163 lines. (C) When the two species are at similar phase angles of entrainment, there is a large difference in total

164 resources in the environment during different times of day. When the two species are separated in phase, there

165 is no longer a predictable cycle in resource availability. (D) Scatterplot of organisms’ stored energy vs their phase

166 angle of entrainment relative to the population mean. The parabolic best fit line is shown in red. Individuals at

167 the edges of the population phase distribution acquire more energy and consequently are more likely to

168 reproduce.

169

170 Intraspecific competition causes temporal niche expansion by arrhythmicity D

171 Previous work has shown that when interspecific competition is absent, the population’s niche width tends

172 to expand, as competition within the species pushes individuals to exploit a greater range of resources [23]. We

173 tested if this pattern would be true in this model. Indeed, when rhythmic amplitude was allowed to vary, the

174 amplitdue steadily decreased to near-zero levels, representing the transition from a temporal specialist to a

175 generalist strategy. To clarify, a broad circadian niche does not necessarily imply the absence of sleep or sleep-

176 like rest; these organisms could follow a cathemeral pattern in which sleep is scattered throughout the day.

177 However, other factors may prevent dampening of rhythms. To adapt to a temporal niche, organisms do not

178 only develop activity rhythms but additional morphological and physiological specializations as well, such as in

179 vision, temperature regulation, and non-locomotor circadian rhythms [9,24]. These adaptations allow an animal

180 to acquire more resources per unit energy expenditure during their specialized “on” phase, but often at the cost

8 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

181 of being less efficient during their “off” phase. A temporally specialized organism would thus incur costs when

182 adopting a generalist activity rhythm. To model these temporal adaptations, we added a specialization term to

183 the simulation. The degree of specialization over the day follows a cosine wave such that at specialization = 0.1,

184 an animal acquires an additional 10% energy from consuming a resource during the peak and 10% less energy

185 during the trough. For simplicity, we assume that the phase angle of the specialization wave matches exactly

186 that of the cosine wave of circadian activity, as opposed to modeling them as separate phenotypes, which would

187 naturally converge to match phase angles anyways so that organisms are most specialized when they are most

188 active.

189 We found that specialization must be quite high to overcome competition’s negative selection for amplitude

190 (Fig 4). At low levels of specialization, the equilibrium amplitude is positively correlated with the degree of

191 specialization. Only when specialization is at least 0.75 does the resulting amplitude approach 1 (Fig 4C). This is

192 rather intense specialization, indicating that resource acquisition is 7 times more efficient during the peak than

193 during the trough. This suggests that, in an environment where resource availability is roughly constant

194 throughout the day, intraspecific competition is a strong factor selecting for arrhythmicity.

195 When a population is freed from competition, the following niche expansion could occur through increasing

196 the niche width within individuals, or through increasing the variance in between individuals who are still

197 relatively specialized . Whereas low rhythmic amplitude represents within-individual niche expansion, we also

198 observed between-individual niche expansion, represented the greater range of phase angles when amplitude is

199 high (Fig 4D). When amplitude is low, there is no selective pressure to promote interindividual variance, so

200 sexual reproduction pulls the population towards the mean.

201

202 Fig 4. Intraspecific competition selects for arrhythmicity. (A) Even with a positive specialization value,

203 amplitude is driven towards low levels when there is only one species. (B) Amplitude settles on an intermediate

9 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

204 value when specialization = 0.4, demonstrating a positive correlation between specialization and resulting

205 amplitude. (C) In the single-species scenario, specialization must be set quite high at 0.75 for amplitude to reach

206 near 1. (D) Standard deviation of phase angle as a function of amplitude. This illustrates an inverse relationship

207 between expanded interindividual niche width (high phase angle variance) and expanded intraindividual niche

208 width (low amplitude).

209

210 Intraspecific competition allows rhythmicity to develop

211 While intraspecific competition acts to prevent rhythmicity, we asked if a competitor species would cause

212 rhythmicity to develop. When specialization = 0, organisms did not show a trend towards either high or low

213 rhythmicity (Fig 5A). Since the two species’ activity rhythms were in approximately opposite phase and similar

214 amplitude, the summed activity of all individuals in the environment was approximately constant throughout

215 the day, which in turn caused resource availability to be approximately constant. This situation is much like the

216 final condition of Fig 3A and Fig 3C. With no time-resource graident, there is no evolutionary pressure for

217 rhythmicity to evolve.

218 While interspecific competition alone does not positively select for amplitude, it also effectively neutralizes

219 intraspecific competition’s negative selection for amplitude. In this state, if the organisms have even a small

220 degree of specialization, it can drive amplitude to increase steadily over time in both species (Fig 5B), with

221 higher specialization values causing amplitude to increase more quickly. Whereas a specialization value of 0.1

222 causes amplitude rise to 1 when there are two species, amplitude barely rises when there is only one species

223 (Fig 4A), and a specialization value as extreme as 0.75 is needed to raise amplitude to the levels seen in the two

224 species scenario (Fig 4C).

225 The amplitudes of the two species tended to be correlated (Fig 5A-B). The correlation of the two species’

226 amplitudes can be explained by the fact that a chance increase in amplitude in one species causes a resource

10 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

227 density difference in time, which encourages the other species to increase their amplitude due to interspecific

228 competition and the first species to decrease their amplitude due to intraspecific competition.

229 The relative strength of interspecific and intraspecific competition influences the equilibrium amplitude of

230 each species. To simulate asymmetric populations, we defined a target population ratio, and the populations

231 were stabilized around this ratio by automatically adjusting their reproduction rate. When species A is set to be

232 two times as numerous as species B, species B develops high rhythmicity while species A’s amplitude hovers

233 around 0.7 (Fig 5C). If species A is set to be nine times as numerous as species B, species A’s amplitude remains

234 only at 0.2, demonstrating that intraspecific rather than interspecific competition is the much stronger force in

235 determining its equilibrium amplitude. This also demonstrates that a dominant species which has even mild

236 rhythms can strongly determine the rhythms of minor competitors which have smaller populations. In total,

237 these results suggest that the presence of competitors is a strong factor promoting the expression of biological

238 rhythms.

239

240 Fig 5. Interspecific competition with specialization allows development of biological rhythms. (A) With two

241 species, amplitude drifts randomly without pressure towards either 0 or 1. (B) When a specialization value is

242 added to the model, amplitudes rise steadily over time. (C) The final equilibrium amplitudes depend on relative

243 population numbers. When the blue species is twice as numerous as the orange, its final amplitude remains

244 lower than the orange’s. (D) When the blue species is nine times as numerous as the orange, blue’s amplitude

245 remains low while orange’s is high.

246

247 Population splitting by differentiation of phase

11 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

248 The single-species scenario illustrated that intraspecific competition causes expansions in both within-

249 individual and between-individual niche widths. Could an increase in between-individual variation cause the

250 population to split into two distinct groups? This would in effect cause speciation, or at least contribute strongly

251 to the ecological divergence and reproductive isolation which are precursors to full speciation. To test this,

252 assortative mating was introduced to the model. Individuals only mate with a partner whose phase differs by

253 less than 9 hours (135°), and the probability of choosing a given partner increases linearly as the phase

254 difference decreases to zero. Amplitudes were fixed at 1 for this test.

255 We found that, with the addition of assortative mating, the population did indeed segregate into two groups

256 (Fig 6A). We defined a cross-group mating value (CGM) to quantify reproductive isolation, where low CGM

257 indicates high reproductive isolation. The CGM tended to stay constant for some time and then drop suddenly

258 (Fig 6B). This suggests that there is a threshold effect, such that once the conditions occur for a secondary

259 population to escape “gene flow” with the main population, full reproductive isolation quickly follows. To

260 separate from the main population, there must be enough individuals at the far edges of the phase distribution

261 to sustain a “breeding population”, such that a tipping point is reached where individuals in the incipient

262 secondary population become more likely to breed within its own population rather than the main population.

263 Once this tipping point is reached, the secondary population quickly splits off from the main group. Given the

264 random nature of mate selection and progeny’s phase variance in our model, the conditions where this happens

265 occurs stochastically, explaining the large variance in speciation time in the simulations.

266

267 Fig 6. Population splitting by phase. A) Circular histograms showing distributions of phase angles of

268 entrainment in one trial. On day 340, The population was a single mating group (cross-group mating = 0.67). On

269 day 580, the population is in the process of splitting into two mating groups (CGM = 0.38). By day 630, there are

270 two populations which are almost completely reproductively isolated (CGM = 0.05). B) Summary of 40 trials.

12 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

271 Trials were sorted by days to reach CGM < 0.1; the 20th trial is plotted in dark blue, while the 4th, 12th, 28th, and

272 36th trials are plotted in light blue.

273

274 Population splitting was not guaranteed for all parameter settings. Conditions necessary to achieve

275 speciation in this model are somewhat constrained, which is unsurprising given the high barriers to sympatric

276 speciation in nature [25]. The time-resource gradient must be quite strong to reach the necessary levels of

277 density-dependent disruptive selection, and thus population splitting will not occur for low resource

278 regeneration rates or low population levels. A smaller mating window of course also promotes reproductive

279 isolation. Higher variance in offsprings’ phases also promotes reproductive isolation by increasing the chance of

280 a breakaway group to escape “gene flow” with the rest of the population. A long lifespan also favors speciation,

281 since individuals at the edges of the distribution then have more time to build up an energy surplus.

282

283 Population splitting vs. individual expansion. Intraindividual niche expansion will tend to oppose speciation,

284 since a decrease of rhythmic amplitude would weaken the time-resource gradient and broaden mating

285 windows. Conversely, speciation into two populations occupying opposite niches will remove selective pressure

286 for broad individual niches since the time-resource gradient would be erased (analogous to the last case in Fig

287 3C). Faced with intraspecific competition, which of these two mutually exclusive paths will a population take?

288 Results suggest that it depends heavily on the relative flexibility of the amplitude or phase traits. We set

289 standard deviations of phase angle and amplitude to various test values, with a higher standard deviation

290 resulting in quicker evolution of that trait. When phase standard deviation was high and amplitude standard

291 deviation was low, populations were more likely to speciate; conversely, when phase standard deviation was

292 low and amplitude standard deviation was high, populations tended to become arrhythmic (Table 1).

293

13 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

294 Table 1. Number of trials achieving speciation depend on the variances of amplitude versus phase angle.

Amplitude standard deviation

0.02 0.03 0.04 0.05

0.04 10/20 0/20 0/20 0/20

Phase standard 0.045 20/20 14/20 9/20 5/20

deviation 0.05 20/20 20/20 17/20 12/20

0.055 20/20 20/20 20/20 17/20

295

296

297 Discussion

298 This is the first modeling study to examine the ecological pressures which favor the evolution of biological

299 rhythms. While other modeling studies have examined the “internal” forces which make rhythms and sleep

300 physiologically efficient [26–28], none have looked at the “external” forces of the environment.

301

302 Timescales of analysis

303 Though we framed our model in terms of organisms evolving genotypically over time, niche partitioning can

304 equally occur at the timescale of individual behavior or ecological community [29], and the conclusions of this

305 study can easily be generalized to sub-evolutionary timescales and mechanisms. Many animals are known to

306 switch their temporal niches depending on various environmental factors [24]; for example, the aforementioned

307 studies on minks, spiny mice [30], and foxes [15] demonstrate temporal partitioning at the sub-evolutionary

308 level. Indeed, every clear observation of competition’s effect on temporal niches has occurred at the

309 behavioral/ecological level, and inferring the role of competition in a species’ evolution is difficult if not

14 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

310 impossible, since those historical interspecies interactions are unobservable. However, if temporal displacement

311 continues for an extended time, it seems likely that organisms would eventually evolve to match their internal

312 clock with their expressed behavior. Research on the health consequences of misalignment between the

313 circadian clock and activity rhythms [7,31,32] suggest clear selective mechanisms by which behavioral

314 displacements can translate into genetic/evolutionary changes.

315 The simulation was framed in terms of circadian rhythms, but the results could equally apply to seasonal

316 rhythms. A major difference between the daily and seasonal timescales is that individuals can adjust their daily

317 behavior to a new time through non-genetic mechanisms, whereas this is probably less common at longer

318 timescales, though it certainly depends heavily on the particular mechanisms governing these rhythms.

319

320 Competitive exclusion and character displacement

321 In our model, we demonstrated that competitive exclusion is unavoidable if two species share an

322 ecological niche. Two species which are temporally separated are much more likely to coexist for extended

323 periods of time. Additionally, we show that two circadian-rhythmic species which initially are active at the same

324 phase evolve to be active at opposite times of day in order to avoid competition for resources. The most likely

325 direct natural analog of this scenario is the introduction of a new species to a region already occupied by

326 another similar species. This could happen, for example, through the breakdown of geographical barriers,

327 chance migration to a new area [33], introduction by humans, or climate change-induced range shifts [34]. The

328 invasive American mink in the United Kingdom offers a good real-life example: originally nocturnal when first

329 introduced, the mink largely switched to diurnality when the native otters and polecats recovered their

330 population levels [35]. The previously mentioned studies in Argentinian foxes [15] and desert spiny mice [9] also

331 demonstrate niche switching.

15 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

332

333 Intraspecific competition and niche expansion

334 Intraspecific competition is hypothesized to increase the niche width in a population, especially in

335 environments where a population is released from competition from other species, which could occur after

336 migration or introduction to a new region, for instance [36,37]. The single-species simulations demonstrate this

337 phenomenon for temporal niches.

338 Niche expansion can happen either by increasing the variance of resource type usage between individuals

339 [38,39] or by increasing the range of resource usage of each individual [40]. Interindividual expansion appears to

340 be the more common pattern in nature in general [41], especially if intraindividual generalization has associated

341 costs [23]. Our single-species simulation displayed both interindividual variation, visible as a broadened phase-

342 angle distribution, and intraindividual generalization, visible as flattened amplitude. The model’s design does not

343 allow for very extreme interindividual expansion, since activity timing was determined strictly by genetics and

344 not by individuals’ behavioral adjustments, and sexual reproduction tended to pull genotypes towards the

345 population mean. Programming a non-genetic behavioral component may be interesting for future studies.

346 A few studies in fish have observed offset patterns of diel feeding activity between individuals, with larger

347 dominant individuals feed at preferred times while subordinates being pushed to other times [13,42]. This

348 suggests that preexisting heterogeneities in the population such as social could predispose niche

349 expansion to occur between rather than within individuals. We are aware of only one study to observe temporal

350 niche expansion in the wild; American minks which were introduced to a remote island with no competitors

351 were observed to have expanded temporal niches compared to their mainland counterparts [43]; it is unknown

352 whether this was due more inter- or intraindividual expansion. Intraspecific competition is likely to also play a

353 role in instances of niche expansion after release from predation [11]. Predator-free space may be seen as a

354 type of resource, which becomes more abundant after removal of the predator. Though never specifically

16 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

355 examined, it is reasonable to suspect that intraspecific competition could then drive the subsequent niche

356 expansion observed after predator release.

357 Arrhythmic/cathemeral activity patterns appear to be somewhat common [44,45]. However, besides the

358 aforementioned observation of invasive minks [43], there have not been any other experiments or observations

359 to directly test competition’s role in causing temporal niche expansion. We suggest that this phenomenon is not

360 uncommon and may explain the behavior and evolution of a number of these arrhythmic animals.

361

362 Competition facilitates of rhythms

363 The presence of an interspecific competitor erases the advantage that arrhythmicity in had in a single-

364 species scenario. A small amount of time-of-day specialization (theoretically, any amount of specialization) can

365 then cause organisms to evolve high amplitudes. We only modeled exploitative competition here, where

366 differences were only driven by differential access to resources. If interference competition were also present,

367 for example if a cost was incurred every time an individual encountered an allospecific, then development of

368 rhythmic amplitude would most likely occur even more quickly.

369 As previously mentioned, the comparison between invasive minks on an island and mainland minks is an

370 instance of apparent competition-mediated niche expansion/contraction [43], but we are not aware of any

371 studies which has observed competition inducing increased amplitude. Again, we believe this phenomenon does

372 exist broadly, but has simply not been observed yet. Another potential real-life analog is if two arrhythmic

373 species existing in a constant environment—cave-dwelling fishes for example [46]—are suddenly exposed to a

374 cycling environment and are thus pushed to temporally differentiate.

375

376 Speciation

17 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

377 When individuals were limited to mating with other individuals who were active at similar times of day,

378 intraspecific competition could cause a single population to separate into two groups which were largely

379 reproductively isolated, in effect causing speciation. This is specifically a case of sympatric speciation, in which

380 new species arise without geographic separation. Speciation required high levels of density-dependent selection

381 (i.e. a strong time-resource gradient) to occur. This is not surprising given that sympatric speciation in general is

382 believed to be quite difficult to achieve in nature, such that its existence at all was questioned before recent

383 decades [25]. A theoretical problem against sympatric speciation is that the phenotype which improves fitness

384 usually has no effect on mate choice, and thus even when disruptive selection exists, there is no mechanism for

385 it to translate to mating preferences. However, speciation by time (allochronic speciation) has an advantage

386 here because it is a “magic” trait [47], i.e. it has fitness effects which are acted on by natural selection, and it is

387 also directly responsible for assortative mating.

388 A recent review [48] finds good evidence for dozens of allochronic speciation events, with most cases

389 occurring at the seasonal rather than the circadian timespan, but although intraspecific competition is known to

390 play a role in promoting speciation in general [49,50], it is unclear how important intraspecific competition was

391 in these specific cases of allochronic speciation. In a study of Madeiran storm petrels, the authors hypothesize

392 that competition for food and nest space may have led to allochronic separation into hot- and cool-

393 breeding populations [51]. Besides competition, allochronic speciation can occur by other reasons too, such as

394 temporal isolation [52], environmental changes [53], or a founder effect [54]; temporal separation can also be

395 secondary to another [55] or serve to reinforce reproductive isolation. Intraspecific competition can

396 act alongside these other mechanisms to produce allochronic speciation.

397

398 Sleep

18 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

399 The question of why sleep exists is one of the big questions of biology. Traditionally framed something of a

400 paradox that an animal would abandon motor and sensory activity for several hours per day, it is perhaps not so

401 mysterious after all when considering that temporal niches exist. External pressures, whether they be

402 competition or other environmental forces, push animals to specialize for a time of day. Internal pressures, such

403 as energy conservation [56], the need to perform restorative functions, and the separation or coordination of

404 clashing or synergistic physiological processes [27,28,32], also encourage the evolution of sleep (or at least a rest

405 period). Especially with external and internal pressures combined, sleep/rest appears to be an unsurprising

406 evolutionary consequence. Stated colloquially, “if I can’t be active right now, then I might as well rest. If rest is

407 efficient, then I might as well do it when I can’t be active.”

408 As to why a defined sleep state exists rather than simply circadian-mediated inactivity, we propose that true

409 sleep can be viewed as an adaptation to delay rest. Urgent circumstances sometimes interrupt an animal’s

410 regularly scheduled rest, and thus the defining feature of true sleep, sleep homeostasis (“sleepiness”),

411 encourages animals to later pay back the lost rest. Another feature of true sleep is that it is a relatively distinct

412 mode with a well-defined sleep-wake boundary. In unpredictable environments, such a clear distinction allows

413 animals to maximize rest when the opportunity is available, rather than a slow transition between rest and

414 sleep.

415

416 Methodology and future directions

417 We intentionally created a model without distinct day and night phases, so that it would be more

418 generalizable to a variety of hypothetical conditions. Nevertheless, for most organisms, the changes between

419 day and night represent a major division in the environment [44], and thus alternative models may explicitly

420 include this. Activity patterns other than cosine waves, or testing the variable of daily total activity, could also be

421 interesting.

19 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

422 We decided not to use a genetic model of circadian rhythms, opting instead to model only phenotypes with

423 a constant distribution. We do not believe this is a weakness because it is more or less equivalent to a situation

424 of polygenic inheritance with many genes. However, a genetic model may have some advantages such as

425 allowing a clearer look at population genetics. Additional models have been developed to explain the theoretical

426 benefits of sleep and circadian rhythms from the point of view of internal physiological organization [27,28], and

427 it could be interesting to integrate models of these “internal” benefits of rhythms with the “external” ecological

428 benefits modeled here.

429 Many questions regarding this topic remain open for investigation. Among the many possible axes by which

430 competing species can differentiate, what promotes partitioning of time over partitioning of other resources?

431 How does competition interact with other forces, like predation, to influence rhythms? To what extent does

432 competition shape activity rhythms in nature? In addition to additional field studies, well-designed experiments

433 with lab rodents could even help us understand the circumstances in which activity rhythms are modified by

434 competition.

435

436 Methods

437 Environment and organisms

438 Using Python, we simulated communities of organisms existing in an environment consisting of 60 spaces,

439 where each space may contain a resource, contain an individual organism, contain multiple individuals, or be

440 empty. An individual moves by jumping to another random space, and if it moves to a space which contains a

441 resource, the resource is consumed.

20 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

442 600 resources per day are added randomly to the spaces of the environment at a constant rate. These

443 resources can represent food but can also represent resources in general including water, territory, and the

444 access to those resources.

445 An organism’s amount of movement varies across the day following a circadian activity rhythm, which is

446 modeled as a cosine wave with two parameters: amplitude, ranging from 0 to 1, and phase angle, ranging from

447 0° to 360° (Fig 1). The number of moves performed between times t0 and t1 is:

푡1푀푡표푡푎푙 448 푚 = (1 + a ∗ cos (푡 + 휃))푑푡 ∫ 360° 푡0

449 where m is the number of moves, Mtotal is the total number of moves per day = 20, a is the amplitude, and θ is

450 the phase angle. Moves were calculated in 10 timesteps per day (t1 - t0 = 36°). We believe a sinusoidal activity

451 rhythm is a reasonable approximation, and a more flexible activity pattern would in fact make temporal

452 segregation easier to achieve. Though an animal’s activity profile varies according to its

453 parameters, the total number of moves is fixed at 20 moves per day; we hold total movement and energy

454 expenditure constant so that the primary variable of interest is is the timing of movement. Each animal has the

455 capacity to store energy; 2 units of energy is expended per day for movement and metabolism, and 0.5 energy is

456 gained when the animal consumes a resource. Animals die if their energy reaches zero, or if they reach 12 days

457 of age. Values for arena size, energy expenditure, resource value, and death age are somewhat arbitrary, and

458 were chosen at reasonable numbers which limited computational load.

459

460 Reproduction

461 Depending on the scenario, simulations are run either with one or two species in the environment.

462 Throughout the day at a constant rate, pairs of animals belonging to the same species are randomly chosen to

463 mate. The probability of an individual being chosen for mating is proportional its energy; animals which have

21 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

464 acquired more energy thus tend to have better fitness. During each mating, one offspring is produced, to whom

465 each parent donates 1/3 of its energy. In some scenarios, the circadian amplitude and phase of all animals

466 remain fixed at the same values, while in other scenarios, there is variation in the circadian parameters, which

467 are acted on by natural selection to cause evolution. In the case of variable parameters, the child’s amplitude is

468 the mean of the parents’ ± 0.04 SD, and the child’s phase angle is the circular mean of the parents’ ± 0.04*360°

469 SD. Matings occur at a frequency such that the population of animals would increase by 10% each day if no

470 animals died. In our two-species simulation scenarios, when not testing competitive exclusion, we stabilized the

471 population levels so that random population fluctuations would not cause one species to disappear by chance.

472 For this, a target population ratio was set (usually 1:1), and the species with a population below target

473 reproduces slightly more, and the species with a higher population reproduces slightly less, proportional to the

474 difference in population.

푃푡표푡푎푙 475 푟 = 푟 ∗ 푘 ∗ 푎푑푗. 푝

476 For each species, radj. is the adjusted population growth rate, which depends on the default growth rate r = 0.1, k

477 the target fraction of the population which is composed of this species (k = 0.5 for a 1:1 ratio), p the population

478 of the this species, and Ptotal the population of all species. The number of progeny in a timestep t0 to t1 is thus:

푡1 ‒ 푡0 479 푁 = 푝 ∗ 푟 ∗ 푝푟표푔푒푛푦 푎푑푗. 360°

480 Calculations were performed in 10 timesteps per day.

481

482 Assortative mating

483 To study speciation, the standard simulation parameters were modified to model assortative mating. We

484 assume that mating must occur in a 9-hour (135°) time window which is fixed relative to the organism’s overall

22 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

485 phase angle of entrainment, and the probability of mating between two individuals is proportional to the

486 amount of overlap of their mating windows, ranging from 0 to 1. In other words, individuals which have a phase

487 difference of nine hours or more do not mate, and the probability of mating increases linearly as the phase

488 difference approaches zero. We define a “cross-group mating” metric to measure reproductive segregation in

489 the population. To calculate this, we first define an overlap matrix of mating windows, i.e. the matrix where

490 each cell Mij is the mating-window overlap between individuals i and j. K-means clustering is used on the overlap

491 matrix to define two clusters of individuals. The cross-group mating index is the mean of the overlap values

492 between individuals from different groups; this is equivalent to the probability that a given mating is between

493 different groups. Cross-group mating of 0 thus indicates complete reproductive isolation of two groups.

494

495 REFERENCES

496 1. Tilman D. Niche tradeoffs, neutrality, and community structure: A stochastic theory of resource 497 competition, invasion, and community assembly. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101: 10854–10861. 498 doi:10.1073/pnas.0403458101 499 2. Caro TM, Stoner CJ. The potential for interspecific competition among African carnivores. Biol Conserv. 500 2003;110: 67–75. 501 3. Maurer BA. Interference and exploitation in communities. Wilson Bull. 1984; 380–395. 502 4. Amarasekare P. Interference competition and species coexistence. Proc R Soc London Ser B Biol Sci. 503 2002;269: 2541–2550. 504 5. Gibson RN. Experiments on the tidal rhythm of Blennius pholis. J Mar Biol Assoc United Kingdom. 505 1967;47: 97–111. 506 6. Yerushalmi S, Green RM. Evidence for the adaptive significance of circadian rhythms. Ecol Lett. 2009;12: 507 970–981. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01343.x 508 7. Ouyang Y, Andersson CR, Kondo T, Golden SS, Johnson CH. Resonating circadian clocks enhance fitness in 509 cyanobacteria. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95: 8660–4. doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2010.03.015 510 8. Castillo-Ruiz A, Paul MJ, Schwartz WJ. In search of a temporal niche: social interactions. Progress in brain 511 research. Elsevier; 2012. pp. 267–280. 512 9. Kronfeld-Schor N, Dayan T. Partitioning of time as an ecological resource. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 513 2003;34: 153–181. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.011802.132435

23 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

514 10. Begon M, Townsend CR, Harper JL. : from individuals to . 2006. 515 11. McCauley DJ, Hoffmann E, Young HS, Micheli F. Night shift: expansion of temporal niche use following 516 reductions in predator density. PLoS One. 2012;7. 517 12. Bakker ES, Reiffers RC, Olff H, Gleichman JM. Experimental manipulation of predation risk and food 518 quality: effect on grazing behaviour in a central-place . Oecologia. 2005;146: 157–167. 519 13. Reebs SG. Plasticity of diel and circadian activity rhythms in fishes. Rev Fish Biol Fish. 2002;12: 349–371. 520 doi:10.1023/A:1025371804611 521 14. Kunz TH. Resource utilization: temporal and spatial components of activity in central Iowa. J 522 Mammal. 1973;54: 14–32. 523 15. Di Bitetti MS, Di Blanco YE, Pereira JA, Paviolo A, Pírez IJ. Time Partitioning Favors the Coexistence of 524 Sympatric Crab-Eating Foxes (Cerdocyon thous) and Pampas Foxes (Lycalopex gymnocercus). J Mammal. 525 2009;90: 479–490. doi:10.1644/08-mamm-a-113.1 526 16. Valeix M, Chamaillé-Jammes S, Fritz H. Interference competition and temporal niche shifts: elephants and 527 herbivore communities at waterholes. Oecologia. 2007;153: 739–748. doi:10.1007/s00442-007-0764-5 528 17. Adams RA, Thibault KM. Temporal resource partitioning by bats at water holes. J Zool. 2006;270: 466– 529 472. 530 18. Ziv Y, Abramsky Z, Kotler BP, Subach A. Interference competition and temporal and partitioning in 531 two gerbil species. Oikos. 1993; 237–246. 532 19. Kamenova S, Tougeron K, Cateine M, Marie A, Plantegenest M. Behaviour-driven micro-scale niche 533 differentiation in carabid beetles. Entomol Exp Appl. 2015;155: 39–46. 534 20. Hayward MW, Slotow R. Temporal partitioning of activity in large african carnivores: Tests of multiple 535 hypotheses. African J Wildl Res. 2009;39: 109–125. doi:10.3957/056.039.0207 536 21. Heesy CP, Hall MI. The nocturnal bottleneck and the evolution of mammalian vision. Brain Behav Evol. 537 2010;75: 195–203. Available: https://www.karger.com/DOI/10.1159/000314278 538 22. Grant PR, Grant BR. Evolution of character displacement in Darwin’s finches. Science (80- ). 2006;313: 539 224–226. doi:10.1126/science.1128374 540 23. Sjödin H, Ripa J, Lundberg P. Principles of niche expansion. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2018;285. 541 doi:10.1098/rspb.2018.2603 542 24. Hut RA, Kronfeld-Schor N, van der Vinne V, De la Iglesia H. In search of a temporal niche: Environmental 543 factors. 1st ed. Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier B.V.; 2012. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-59427-3.00017-4 544 25. Via S. Sympatric speciation in animals: the ugly duckling grows up. Trends Ecol Evol. 2001;16: 381–390. 545 doi:10.1002/eqe.4290130210 546 26. Hellwegera FL. Resonating circadian clocks enhance fitness in cyanobacteria in silico. Ecol Modell. 547 2010;221: 1620–1629. doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2010.03.015 548 27. Schmidt MH. The energy allocation function of sleep: a unifying theory of sleep, torpor, and continuous 549 wakefulness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47: 122–153. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.001 550 28. Schmidt MH, Swang TW, Hamilton IM, Best JA. State-dependent metabolic partitioning and energy

24 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

551 conservation: A theoretical framework for understanding the function of sleep. PLoS One. 2017;12: 1–15. 552 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0185746 553 29. Mrosovsky N, Hattar S. Diurnal mice (Mus musculus) and other examples of temporal niche switching. J 554 Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sensory, Neural, Behav Physiol. 2005;191: 1011–1024. doi:10.1007/s00359- 555 005-0017-1 556 30. Gutman R, Dayan T. Temporal partitioning: An experiment with two species of spiny mice. Ecology. 557 2005;86: 164–173. doi:10.1890/03-0369 558 31. Rüger M, Scheer FAJL. Effects of circadian disruption on the cardiometabolic system. 2009; 245–260. 559 doi:10.1007/s11154-009-9122-8 560 32. Baron KG, Reid KJ. Circadian misalignment and health. Int Rev psychiatry. 2014;26: 139–154. 561 33. Lomolino M V. A call for a new paradigm of island . Glob Ecol Biogeogr. 2000;9: 1–6. 562 34. Thomas CD. Climate, climate change and range boundaries. Divers Distrib. 2010;16: 488–495. 563 doi:10.1111/j.1472-4642.2010.00642.x 564 35. Harrington LA, Harrington AL, Yamaguchi N, Thom MD, Ferreras P, Windham TR, et al. The impact of 565 native competitors on an alien invasive: Temporal niche shifts to avoid interspecific aggression? Ecology. 566 2009;90: 1207–1216. doi:10.1890/08-0302.1 567 36. Lister BC. The nature of niche expansion in West Indian Anolis I: ecological consequences of 568 reduced competition. Evolution (N Y). 1976; 659–676. 569 37. Bolnick DI. Intraspecific competition favours niche width expansion in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature. 570 2001;410: 463–466. doi:10.1038/35068555 571 38. Svanbäck R, Bolnick DI. Intraspecific competition drives increased resource use diversity within a natural 572 population. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2006;274: 839–844. 573 39. Van Valen L. Morphological variation and width of ecological niche. Am Nat. 1965;99: 377–390. 574 40. Bolnick DI, Ingram T, Stutz WE, Snowberg LK, Lau OL, Pauli JS. Ecological release from interspecific 575 competition leads to decoupled changes in population and individual niche width. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 576 2010;277: 1789–1797. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0018 577 41. Bolnick DI, Svanbäck R, Araújo MS, Persson L. Comparative support for the niche variation hypothesis 578 that more generalized populations also are more heterogeneous. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104: 579 10075–10079. doi:10.1073/pnas.0703743104 580 42. Alanärä A, Burns MD, Metcalfe NB. Intraspecific resource partitioning in brown trout: The temporal 581 distribution of foraging is determined by social rank. J Anim Ecol. 2001;70: 980–986. doi:10.1046/j.0021- 582 8790.2001.00550.x 583 43. Crego RD, Jiménez JE, Rozzi R. Potential niche expansion of the American mink invading a remote island 584 free of nativepredatory mammals. PLoS One. 2018;13: 1–18. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0194745 585 44. van Schaik CP, Griffiths M. Activity periods of Indonesian rain forest mammals. Biotropica. 1996;28: 105– 586 112. doi:10.2307/2388775 587 45. Roll U, Dayan T, Kronfeld-Schor N. On the role of phylogeny in determining activity patterns of rodents. 588 Evol Ecol. 2006;20: 479–490. doi:10.1007/s10682-006-0015-y

25 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

589 46. Kavanau JL. Vertebrates that never sleep: Implications for sleep’s basic function. Brain Res Bull. 1998;46: 590 269–279. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(98)00018-5 591 47. Servedio MR, Doorn GS Van, Kopp M, Frame AM, Nosil P. Magic traits in speciation: “magic” but not rare? 592 Trends Ecol Evol. 2011;26: 389–397. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.04.005 593 48. Taylor RS, Friesen VL. The role of allochrony in speciation. Mol Ecol. 2017;26: 3330–3342. 594 doi:10.1111/mec.14126 595 49. Laland KN, Sterelny K. The conditions for speciation through intraspecific competition. Evol Int J Org Evol. 596 2006;60: 1751–1762. doi:10.1554/05-570.1 597 50. Bolnick DI. Can intraspecific competition drive disruptive selection? An experimental test in natural 598 populations of sticklebacks. Evolution (N Y). 2004;58: 608–618. doi:10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb01683.x 599 51. Monteiro LR, Furness RW. Speciation through temporal segregation of Madeiran storm petrel 600 (Oceanodroma castro) populations in the Azores? Philos Trans R Soc London Ser B Biol Sci. 1998;353: 601 945–953. 602 52. Hendry AP, Day T. Population structure attributable to reproductive time: Isolation by time and 603 adaptation by time. Mol Ecol. 2005;14: 901–916. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02480.x 604 53. Yamamoto S, Sota T. Parallel allochronic divergence in a winter due to disruption of reproductive 605 period by winter harshness. Mol Ecol. 2012;21: 174–183. 606 54. Santos H, Rousselet J, Magnoux E, Paiva M-R, Branco M, Kerdelhué C. Genetic isolation through time: 607 allochronic differentiation of a phenologically atypical population of the pine processionary moth. Proc R 608 Soc B Biol Sci. 2007;274: 935–941. 609 55. Ragland GJ, Sim SB, Goudarzi S, Feder JL, Hahn DA. Environmental interactions during host race 610 formation: host fruit environment moderates a seasonal shift in phenology in host races of Rhagoletis 611 pomonella. Funct Ecol. 2012;26: 921–931. 612 56. Siegel JM. Sleep viewed as a state of adaptive inactivity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10: 747–753. 613 doi:10.1038/nrn2697 614

26 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

615

616 Fig 1. Example activity rhythm profiles. Activity profiles are modeled as cosine waves with properties amplitude

617 and phase angle. Two example profiles are shown. In blue, the activity rhythm has amplitude = 1 and phase

618 angle = 0°. In orange, amplitude = 0.4 and phase angle = 90°. Regardless of changes in circadian parameters, the

619 number of moves per day is fixed at 20 (equivalent to the area under each curve).

27 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

620

621 Fig 2. Circadian rhythms prevent competitive exclusion. (A) When two species with no activity rhythms

622 (amp.=0) are placed in the environment, one species will always die out. An example of an individual trial is

623 displayed. (B) Summary of 40 trials. Since the two species’ population levels tend to be symmetric, only the

624 species which is excluded is plotted for clarity. The trials were sorted by the day that competitive exclusion is

625 reached, and the median (20th) trial is represented by the dark line, while the 4th, 12th, 28th, and 36th trials are

626 represented by faint lines. (C) When two species have circadian rhythms (amp.=1) and are active at the same

627 phase, one species will always die out, as before. An example individual trial is displayed. (D) Summary of 40

628 trials. (E) When two species have circadian rhythms are active 180° out of phase, the two species can coexist

629 indefinitely. An example individual trial is displayed. (F) Summary of multiple trials. Since no species died out in

630 this scenario, five population profiles were selected at random to be plotted.

28 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

631

632 Fig 3. Character displacement of circadian phase. (A) Circular histogram displaying the separation of phase

633 angles of activity in two species over time in one trial. The difference in the two species’ phase angles of

634 entrainment quickly diverges to 180°. (B) Summary of 40 trials. Trials were sorted by days until a phase

635 difference of 150° was reached. The 20th trial is plotted in a dark line, while the 4th, 12th, 28th, and 36th trials are

636 plotted in faint lines. (C) When the two species are at similar phase angles of entrainment, there is a large

637 difference in total resources in the environment during different times of day. When the two species are

638 separated in phase, there is no longer a predictable cycle in resource availability. (D) Scatterplot of organisms’

639 stored energy vs their phase angle of entrainment relative to the population mean. The parabolic best fit line is

640 shown in red. Individuals at the edges of the population phase distribution acquire more energy and

641 consequently are more likely to mate.

29 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

642

643 644 Fig 4. Intraspecific competition selects for arrhythmicity. (A) Even with a positive specialization value,

645 amplitude is driven towards low levels when there is only one species. (B) Amplitude settles on an intermediate

646 value when specialization = 0.4, demonstrating a positive correlation between specialization and resulting

647 amplitude. (C) In the single-species scenario, specialization must be set quite high at 0.75 for amplitude to reach

648 near 1. (D) Standard deviation of phase angle as a function of amplitude. This illustrates an inverse relationship

649 between expanded interindividual niche width (high phase angle variance) and expanded intraindividual niche

650 width (low amplitude).

30 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

651

652 Fig 5. Interspecific competition with specialization allows development of biological rhythms. (A) With two

653 species, amplitude drifts randomly without pressure towards either 0 or 1. (B) When a specialization value is

654 added to the model, amplitudes rise steadily over time. (C) The final equilibrium amplitudes depend on relative

655 population numbers. When the blue species is twice as numerous as the orange, its final amplitude remains

656 lower than the orange’s. (D) When the blue species is nine times as numerous as the orange, blue’s amplitude

657 remains low while orange’s is high.

31 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

658

659 Fig 6. Population splitting by phase. (A) Circular histograms showing distributions of phase angles of

660 entrainment in one trial. On day 340, The population was a single mating group (cross-group mating = 0.67). On

661 day 580, the population is in the process of splitting into two mating groups (CGM = 0.38). By day 630, there are

662 two populations which are almost completely reproductively isolated (CGM = 0.05). (B) Summary of 40 trials.

663 Trials were sorted by days to reach CGM < 0.1; the 20th trial is plotted in dark blue, while the 4th, 12th, 28th, and

664 36th trials are plotted in light blue.

665

32 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

666 Supplementary information captions

667 S1 Dataset. Population sizes over time in competitive exclusion tests. Dataset for Fig 2. Sheets of the Excel file

668 are labeled A-C, and each sheet contains population levels of two species over 1200 days in 40 trials. (A)

669 Organisms have amplitude = 0. Dataset for Fig 2A. (B) Organisms have amplitude = 1, and the two species have

670 the same phase angle. Dataset for Fig 2B. (C) Organisms have amplitude = 1, and the species have a phase angle

671 difference of 180°. Dataset for Fig 2C.

672

673 S2 Dataset. Phase angles during character displacement. Dataset for Fig 3. Mean peak-activity phases for two

674 species over 400 days in 20 trials.

675

676 S3 Dataset. Amplitude of a single species over time. Dataset for Fig 4 A-C. Mean population amplitude for a

677 single species over 8000 days. 20 trials were performed for each of three specialization values: 0.1, 0.4, and

678 0.75.

679

680 S4 Dataset. Relationship between amplitude and phase variance. Dataset for Fig 4D. Amplitudes (representing

681 intraindividual niche expansion) were progressively stepped down from 0.99 to 0. For each amplitude increment

682 of 0.01, 500 samples of the standard deviation of phase angle (representing interindividual niche expansion)

683 were taken.

684

685 S5 Dataset. Amplitude of two species. Dataset for Fig 5 A-B. Sheets of the Excel file are labeled A-B, and each

686 sheet contains the mean population amplitudes of two species over 25000 days in 20 trials. (A) Specialization

33 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.22.055160; this version posted April 22, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license.

687 was set to 0, and target population ratio between the two species was set at 1:1. One trial was chosen for Fig

688 5A. (B) Specialization = 0.1, population ratio 1:1. One trial was chosen for Fig 5B.

689

690 S6 Dataset. Amplitude of two species with uneven populations. Dataset for Fig 5 C-D. (A) Specialization was set

691 to 0.1, and target population ratio between the two species was set at 2:1. One trial was chosen for Fig 5C. (B)

692 Specialization = 0.1, population ratio 9:1. One trial was chosen for Fig 5D.

693

694 S7 Dataset. Cross-group mating values during speciation. Dataset for Fig 6. Cross-group mating (CGM) values

695 over 1000 days in 40 trials.

696

697 S8 Dataset. Evolution of amplitude versus speciation. Dataset for Table 1. Amplitude and cross-group mating

698 (CGM) values over 1000 days. Each sheet of the Excel file contains 20 trials in one combination of amplitude

699 standard deviation and phase-angle standard deviation.

700

701 S9 code. Python script for the simulation.

34