Optimal Foraging and Predator-Prey Dynamics, II

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Optimal Foraging and Predator-Prey Dynamics, II Theoretical Population Biology 55, 111126 (1999) Article ID tpbi.1998.1399, available online at http:ÂÂwww.idealibrary.com on Optimal Foraging and PredatorPrey Dynamics, II Vlastimil Kr ivan and Asim Sikder Department of Theoretical Biology, Institute of Entomology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, and Faculty of Biological Sciences USB, Branis ovska 31, 370 05 C8 eske Bude jovice, Czech Republic E-mail: krivanÄentu.cas.cz Received May 11, 1997 In this paper we consider one-predatortwo-prey population dynamics described by a control system. We study and compare conditions for permanence of the system for three types of predator feeding behaviors: (i) specialized feeding on the more profitable prey type, (ii) generalized feeding on both prey types, and (iii) optimal foraging behavior. We show that the region of parameter space leading to permanence for optimal foraging behavior is smaller than that for specialized behavior, but larger than that for generalized behavior. This suggests that optimal foraging behavior of predators may promote coexistence in predatorprey systems. We also study the effects of the above three feeding behaviors on apparent competition between the two prey types. ] 1999 Academic Press Key Wordsy optimal foraging; population dynamics; apparent competition; persistence; permanence. INTRODUCTION profitable prey type is more gradual than predicted by optimal foraging theory. In Kr ivan (1996) a two-preyone-predator population Kr ivan (1996) showed that optimal foraging leads dynamic model with optimal predator foraging behavior naturally to a more general class of population dynami- was considered. This model assumes that predators cal systems which are described by differential inclusions forage according to optimal foraging theory (Charnov, (i.e., by differential equations with multivalued right- 1976; Stephens and Krebs, 1986), which predicts that the hand sides), and to the emergence of partial preferences more profitable prey type is always included in the for the alternative prey type as a consequence of popula- predator diet while the less profitable (i.e., alternative) tion dynamics. The multivaluedness in the model des- prey type is included with probability one only if the den- cription is due to the non-uniqueness of optimal foraging sity of the more profitable prey type falls below a critical strategy when the more profitable prey type reaches the threshold. Therefore, the optimal foraging model does critical threshold. It was shown that the interplay among not predict the emergence of partial preferences for the behavioral ecology and population dynamics is twofold: alternative prey type which is either completely included optimal foraging influences population dynamics, which, or excluded from predators' diet. The predictions of in turn, affects optimal prey diet. Analysis given in optimal foraging theory were compared with several Kr ivan (1996) focused mainly on local stability of the experimental and field studies in Stephens and Krebs ecological equilibrium and analytical results given there (1986) (see also Richardson and Verbeeck (1992)). These were only for a special case in which both prey types had studies support the idea that the diet choice is based on the same intrinsic per capita growth rate. The reason for food profitability although the inclusion of the less this limitation was due to the fact that even local stability 111 0040-5809Â99 K30.00 Copyright ] 1999 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 112 Kr @van and Sikder analysis for the general case gives very complex expres- predators follow optimal diet choice and predatorprey sions which cannot be efficiently analyzed. On the population dynamics is of the RosenzweigMacArthur numerical evidence it was conceived there that optimal type. foraging may lead to permanence in the one-predator two-prey model; i.e., all three populations can coexist indefinitely. Optimal foraging can also reduce amplitude POPULATION DYNAMICS of fluctuations in population densities when compared with generalist predator behavior. In this part we survey the model introduced in Kr ivan In this paper we study analytically the effect of optimal (1996) where more details can be found. We consider foraging on permanence of the one-predatortwo-prey predators foraging on two prey types. Predator density is system introduced in Kr ivan (1996). We show that the denoted by x , and prey densities are x and x , respec- conditions for permanence are a great deal simpler and 3 1 2 tively. Population dynamics is described by a system of more efficiently tractable than those for local asymptotic differential equations stability. To conclude that all populations can coexist, permanence theory (Hofbauer and Sigmund, 1984; x1 u1 *1 x1 x3 Butler and Waltman, 1986; Hutson and Schmitt, 1992) x$1 =a1 x1 1& & does not require consideration of the complicated \ K1 + 1+u1 h1 *1 x1+u2 h2 *2 x2 behavior of interior (i.e., all populations present) orbits x2 u2 *2 x2 x3 of the model found in Kr ivan (1996). Permanence means x$2=a2 x2 1& & (1) K 1+u h * x +u h * x that there is a lower positive bound such that in a long \ 2 + 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 term run all population densities (initially positive) will u1 e1 *1 x1 x3+u2 e2 *2 x2 x3 be above this bound and no population density tends to x$3= &mx3 . 1+u1 h1 *1 x1+u2 h2 *2 x2 infinity. In Butler and Waltman (1986) the results con- cerning permanence are given for continuous dynamical Here u denotes the probability that a predator will systems, i.e., for systems in which trajectories depend i attack the prey type i, * is the search rate of a predator continuously on initial data. Since the system studied in i for the ith prey type, e is the expected net energy gained Kr ivan (1996) is described by differential inclusions i from the ith prey type, and h is the expected handling which define a continuous dynamical system, we can use i time spent with the ith prey type. In what follows we the results of Butler and Waltman (1986) to study its per- assume that either u and u are fixed if predators show manence. 1 2 fixed preferences for their prey or they are chosen accord- We consider the effects of three types of predator ing to the optimal diet model. Maximization of the net foraging behaviors: (i) predators specialize on the more energy intake rate which is used as the fitness measure in profitable prey type only, (ii) predators are generalists optimal foraging theory (Stephens and Krebs, 1986; and they feed on both prey types, and (iii) predators Kr ivan, 1996) is equivalent to maximization of the forage adaptively following rules of optimal foraging instantaneous per capita predator growth rate x$ Âx .We theory. We derive and compare permanence conditions 3 3 remark that model (1) does not consider any direct com- for the system incorporating the above three predator petition between the two prey populations, but the two foraging behavior types. We show that the region of the prey types are in apparent competition through the parameter space for which permanence holds for adap- shared predation (Holt, 1977). Of course, this apparent tive foragers is larger than that for non-adaptive competition appears only provided that both prey types generalist foragers, but smaller than that for specialist are included in predators' diet. foragers which feed only on the more profitable prey Throughout the paper we will assume that the first type. prey type is more profitable for predators than the We do not consider any direct competition among the second alternative prey type, by which we mean that two prey types. Therefore, in the predator absence both prey populations coexist at their equilibrium levels. e e When predators are introduced, predator-mediated 1 > 2 . apparent competition (Holt, 1977) among prey popula- h1 h2 tions may drive on prey type to extinction. For generalist predators the indirect effects of predation on competition If predators follow optimal diet choice, the optimal among the two prey types were studied by Holt (1977). strategy of a predator when encountering a prey depends In this paper we extend this study to the case where on the density of the more profitable prey type which is Optimal Foraging 113 always attacked upon an encounter, i.e., u =1. The x * x x 1 x$ =a x 1& 1 & 1 1 3 alternative prey type is attacked with probability one 1 1 1 \ K1 + 1+h1 *1 x1 (u1=1) if the density of the more profitable prey type is x2 below the critical threshold x$2=a2 x2 1& (3) \ K2 + e2 e1 *1 x1 x3 x*1= . x$3= &mx3 . *1(e1 h2&e2 h1) 1+h1 *1 x1 Note that the equation for x in (3) is independent of the If the first prey type density is above x* then the alter- 2 1 other two equations. native prey type is not attacked upon an encounter In G0 the right-hand side of (1) is not uniquely defined (u =0) since it pays off for predators to search for the 2 since 0u 1. However, it was shown in Kr ivan (1996) more profitable prey type. Following Murdoch and 2 that in the subregion of G0 described by Oaten (1975) we call x*1 the switching density because predators switch their behavior at this prey density. If the a x* first prey type density equals x* then optimal predator 1 1& 1 (1+h * x*) 1 * K 1 1 1 strategy is not uniquely defined by maximization of 1 \ 1 + x$ Âx , i.e, 0u 1. Thus, control u as a function of the a x* 3 3 2 2 <x < 1 1& 1 (1+h * x*+h * x ) more profitable prey type density is a step-like function 3 1 1 1 2 2 2 *1 \ K1 + and (1) which is driven by optimal predator foraging strategy is not a differential equation but a differential trajectories of (1) cannot leave G0.
Recommended publications
  • Foraging Modes of Carnivorous Plants Aaron M
    Israel Journal of Ecology & Evolution, 2020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22244662-20191066 Foraging modes of carnivorous plants Aaron M. Ellison* Harvard Forest, Harvard University, 324 North Main Street, Petersham, Massachusetts, 01366, USA Abstract Carnivorous plants are pure sit-and-wait predators: they remain rooted to a single location and depend on the abundance and movement of their prey to obtain nutrients required for growth and reproduction. Yet carnivorous plants exhibit phenotypically plastic responses to prey availability that parallel those of non-carnivorous plants to changes in light levels or soil-nutrient concentrations. The latter have been considered to be foraging behaviors, but the former have not. Here, I review aspects of foraging theory that can be profitably applied to carnivorous plants considered as sit-and-wait predators. A discussion of different strategies by which carnivorous plants attract, capture, kill, and digest prey, and subsequently acquire nutrients from them suggests that optimal foraging theory can be applied to carnivorous plants as easily as it has been applied to animals. Carnivorous plants can vary their production, placement, and types of traps; switch between capturing nutrients from leaf-derived traps and roots; temporarily activate traps in response to external cues; or cease trap production altogether. Future research on foraging strategies by carnivorous plants will yield new insights into the physiology and ecology of what Darwin called “the most wonderful plants in the world”. At the same time, inclusion of carnivorous plants into models of animal foraging behavior could lead to the development of a more general and taxonomically inclusive foraging theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimal Foraging Theory and the Psychology of Learning Alan Kamil
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Avian Cognition Papers Center for Avian Cognition 1983 Optimal Foraging Theory and the Psychology of Learning Alan Kamil Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/biosciaviancog Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, Cognition and Perception Commons, Forest Sciences Commons, Ornithology Commons, and the Other Psychology Commons This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Center for Avian Cognition at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Avian Cognition Papers by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. digitalcommons.unl.edu Optimal Foraging Theory and the Psychology of Learning Alan C. Kamil Departments of Psychology and Zoology University of Massachusetts Amherst, Massachusetts 01003 Synopsis The development of optimization theory has made important contri- butions to the study of animal behavior. But the optimization approach needs to be integrated with other methods of ethology and psychology. For example, the ability to learn is an important component of efficient foraging behavior in many species, and the psychology of animal learn- ing could contribute substantially to testing and extending the predic- tions of optimal foraging theory. Introduction When the animal behavior literature of the 1960s is compared with the literature of today, it is apparent that a revolution has taken place. Many of the phenomena of behavioral ecology and sociobiology that are taken for granted today, that are presented in undergraduate text- books (e.g., Krebs and Davies, 1981), would have seemed implausible just 15 to 20 years ago.
    [Show full text]
  • ABSTRACT FAVREAU, JORIE M. the Effects Of
    ABSTRACT FAVREAU, JORIE M. The effects of food abundance, foraging rules and cognitive abilities on local animal movements. (Under the direction of Roger A. Powell.) Movement is nearly universal in the animal kingdom. Movements of animals influence not only themselves but also plant communities through processes such as seed dispersal, pollination, and herbivory. Understanding movement ecology is important for conserving biodiversity and predicting the spread of diseases and invasive species. Three factors influence nearly all movement. First, most animals move to find food. Thus, foraging dictates, in part, when and where to move. Second, animals must move by some rule even if the rule is “move at random.” Third, animals’ cognitive capabilities affect movement; even bees incorporate past experience into foraging. Although other factors such as competition and predation may affect movement, these three factors are the most basic to all movement. I simulated animal movement on landscapes with variable patch richness (amounts of food per food patch), patch density (number of patches), and variable spatial distributions of food patches. From the results of my simulations, I formulated hypotheses about the effects of food abundance on animal movement in nature. I also resolved the apparent paradox of real animals’ movements sometimes correlating positively and sometimes negatively with food abundance. I simulated variable foraging rules belonging to 3 different classes of rules (when to move, where to move, and the scale at which to assess the landscape). Simulating foraging rules demonstrated that variations in richness and density tend to have the same effects on movements, regardless of foraging rules. Still, foraging rules affect the absolute distance and frequency of movements.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploitative Interactions: Predation,Herbivory, Parasitism, and Disease Chapter 14
    Exploitative Interactions: Predation,Herbivory, Parasitism, and Disease Chapter 14 1 Copyright © The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. Introduction • Exploitation: Interaction between populations that enhances fitness of one individual while reducing fitness of the exploited individual. Predators kill and consume other organisms. Parasites live on host tissue and reduce host fitness, but do not generally kill the host. Parasitoid is an insect larva that consumes the host. Pathogens induce disease. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=IgrOK_831DY&feature=related http://www.colbertnation.com/the-colbert-report-videos/257752/december-07-2009/craziest-f--king-thing-i-ve-ever-heard---tongue-eating-parasite Wasps on a hornworm – insides are gone!!! 2 Complex Interactions • There are parasites that alter host behavior They’re pretty cool!!! http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=f5UzztCns-Y http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=4PB4SjX8QkA http://www.youtube.com/watch ?v=vMG- http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EWB_COSUXMwLWyNcAs&feature=related 3 More examples! • The rabies virus increases saliva production and makes the infected host aggressive. When a rabid animal bites a host the virus is spread via saliva in the wound. • Toxiplasma gondii causes infected rodents to specifically lose their inborn aversion to cat pheromones. Infected cats in turn spread toxiplasma through their droppings. People infected with toxiplasma also exhibit behavioral changes, particularly a decrease in "novelty seeking". • http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=nEyIZLQewX8 http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=5qHNoTZMz6w&NR=1 • • Grasshoppers infected with the hairworm (Spinochordodes tellinii • become more likely to jump into water where the hair worm reproduces.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimal Foraging: Some Theoretical Explorations Eric Charnov
    University of New Mexico UNM Digital Repository Biology Faculty & Staff ubP lications Scholarly Communication - Departments 2-24-2006 Optimal Foraging: Some Theoretical Explorations Eric Charnov Gordon H. Orians Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalrepository.unm.edu/biol_fsp Part of the Biology Commons Recommended Citation Charnov, Eric and Gordon H. Orians. "Optimal Foraging: Some Theoretical Explorations." (2006). https://digitalrepository.unm.edu/biol_fsp/45 This Book is brought to you for free and open access by the Scholarly Communication - Departments at UNM Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Biology Faculty & Staff ubP lications by an authorized administrator of UNM Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. OPTIMAL FORAGING: SOME THEORETICAL EXPLORATIONS by Eric L. Charnov † Center for Quantitative Science University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195 and Institute of Animal Resource Ecology University of British Columbia Vancouver 8, Canada and Gordon H. Orians Department of Zoology University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195 1973 † Present Address: Department of Biology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT INTRODUCTION This book had its genesis in a rather disorganized and poorly conceived lecture on optimal foraging theory that one of us (GHO) gave to an advanced ecology class during the winter of 1971. The deficiencies in that treatment, combined with the obvious potential of an improved conceptual- ization and analysis of the problem, led to the formation of a small seminar on optimal foraging theory attended by Charles Fowler, Nolan Pearson and ourselves. This seminar, which extended over two academic quarters, produced the basic fine-grained foraging models and some hints about wider applications of the results.
    [Show full text]
  • Intraspecific Competition Affects the Strength of Individual Specialization: an Optimal Diet Theory Method
    Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2005, 7: 993–1012 Intraspecific competition affects the strength of individual specialization: an optimal diet theory method Richard Svanbäck1,2* and Daniel I. Bolnick3,4 1Department of Ecology and Environmental Science, Umeå University, SE-901 87 Umeå, Sweden, 2Department of Limnology, Evolutionary Biology Centre, Uppsala University, Norbyv. 20, SE-752 36 Uppsala, Sweden, 3Section of Evolution and Ecology, Center for Population Biology, Storer Hall, University of California, Davis, CA 95616, USA and 4Integrative Biology, University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station, C0930, Austin, TX 78712-0253, USA ABSTRACT Question: Why would individuals that inhabit the same environment choose to feed on different subsets of the available resources? Mathematical method: We outline a flexible model that combines phenotypic variation with optimal diet theory and population dynamics. We then apply this model to investigate the role of different types of trade-offs, phenotype diversity and level of competition in determining the degree of individual specialization. Key assumptions: The foragers in the model are omniscient and maximize energy intake per time unit. Conclusion: Numerical simulations match empirical observations that changes in population density can alter the degree of individual specialization. Forager density and phenotypic variation affected prey densities, which in turn affected forager diet breadth and fitness (energy income). We propose that this feedback can explain the empirical relationship between forager density and the degree of individual specialization in the forager population. Keywords: density dependence, frequency dependence, individual specialization, inter- individual variation, intra-population variation, niche breadth. INTRODUCTION Many apparently generalist populations are in fact composed of relatively specialized individuals (Bolnick et al., 2003).
    [Show full text]
  • Foraging Activities of Eastern Chipmunks (Tamias Striatus): Food Acquisition and Cache Defense
    FORAGING ACTIVITIES OF EASTERN CHIPMUNKS (TAMIAS STRIATUS): FOOD ACQUISITION AND CACHE DEFENSE By Corey J Santorello Optimal foraging theory was developed as a theoretical framework for understanding the evolution of feeding strategies. It states that animals forage in a way that will maximize their net energy intake per unit time, thereby maximizing their individual fitness. A behavior whereby animals store surplus food that potentially can be consumed later is called caching. The eastern chipmunk (Tamias striatus) caches food in larder hoards and scatter hoards throughout the spring, summer, and fall. During each season I would observe eastern chipmunk activities. To construct a preliminary time budget of chipmunk food acquisition and cache defensive behaviors, I observed eastern chipmunks throughout their period of above- ground activity for several hours each day. The study was conducted on a 4-ha plot in Hartman Creek State Park located north of Hartman Lake near Waupaca, Wisconsin. I live trapped T. striatus from late May to early June to uniquely mark individuals for focal observations. I conducted observations on all individuals (both marked and unmarked) from early June through October during 2013 and 2014. Four behaviors that I recorded were territorial/alarm calling, foraging, expelling conspecifics from a territory, and scanning for conspecifics or predators. Including all individuals (marked and unmarked) from both years of the study, linear analysis of categorical data revealed a difference in the distributions of behaviors across months and between years. The month*year interaction was also significant. Using the marked male and female data from the 2014 field season, I looked at the difference in distribution between behaviors for sex and month.
    [Show full text]
  • Influence of Predation on Feeding and Giving-Up Density of Forest Rodents
    INFLUENCE OF PREDATION ON FEEDING AND GIVING-UP DENSITY OF FOREST RODENTS A thesis submitted to the Kent State University Honors College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for University Honors by Morgan Hirsh May, 2020 Thesis written by Morgan Hirsh Approved by ________________________________________________________________, Advisor _____________________________________, Chair, Department of Biological Sciences Accepted by _____________________________________________________, Dean, Honors College ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES…..…………………………………………………………….....…..iv ACKNOWLEDGMENT…………………….……………………………………….....…v CHAPTER I. INTRODUCTION……………………..………………………….………1 II. METHODS………….……………..……………………………….……12 Study Sites……………………………….……………………………….12 Focal Organisms………………………..……….……………………….14 Design of Experiments in this Study………..……….…………………….14 Game Camera Analysis………...………..……………………………….18 Statistical Analysis………………..……..……………………………….19 III. RESULTS……………………………………….……………….………20 IV. DISCUSSION…..……………………………….……………….………27 Sampling Methods……………………..…………...…………………….29 Effects of Predator Cues and Food Availability………………..……..….30 Potential Sources of Error in this Study……..………...………………….31 Future Studies………………..……...…..……………………………….32 Implications of this Study…………………………...……………………35 REFERENCES…………………………………………………….…………………….36 iii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: Study Sites for All Experiments…………………………………………………13 Figure 2: Diagram of Experimental Setup of Experiments 1, 2, & 3………….…………..16 Figure 3. Feeding Experiment #1 – 15 December – 23
    [Show full text]
  • Focusing the Metaphor: Plant Root Foraging Behaviour
    Opinion Focusing the metaphor: plant root foraging behaviour Gordon G. McNickle, Colleen Cassady St. Clair and James F. Cahill Jr Department of Biological Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB T6G 2E9, Canada Many authors assert that plants exhibit complex beha- animal behavioural ecology. We will focus primarily on the viours which are analogous to animal behaviour. How- ultimate causes of root foraging behaviour (Box 1), because ever, plant ecologists rarely root these studies in a the proximate causes of root foraging are reviewed else- conceptual foundation as fertile as that used by animal where (Box 2) [12,23–29]. Also following the traditions of behaviourists. Here we adapt the optimality principles animal behavioural ecology, we focus on the behaviour of that facilitated numerous advances in the study of individuals rather than interspecific variation (Box 1). Our animal foraging behaviour to create one possible frame- discussions are centered on the ‘general’ biology of plants work for plant foraging behaviour. Following the and animals, but this does not rule out the potential for traditions of animal foraging ecology, we discuss issues ‘atypical’ taxa to provide critical insights. We begin our of search and handling in relation to plant root foraging. discussion of root foraging by contrasting aspects of the We also develop a basic plant-centered model that basic biology of plants and animals, which will necessarily incorporates modular growth and foraging currencies affect how plants exhibit behaviour. relevant to plant growth. We conclude by demonstrating how this new foundation could be adapted to address How do plants and animals express behaviour? five fundamental questions in plant foraging ecology.
    [Show full text]
  • Prey Removal in Cotton Crops Next to Woodland Reveals Periodic Diurnal and Nocturnal Invertebrate Predation Gradients from the Crop Edge by Birds and Bats Heidi L
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Prey removal in cotton crops next to woodland reveals periodic diurnal and nocturnal invertebrate predation gradients from the crop edge by birds and bats Heidi L. Kolkert *, Rhiannon Smith , Romina Rader & Nick Reid Factors infuencing the efcacy of insectivorous vertebrates in providing natural pest control services inside crops at increasing distances from the crop edge are poorly understood. We investigated the identity of vertebrate predators (birds and bats) and removal of sentinel prey (mealworms and beetles) from experimental feeding trays in cotton crops using prey removal trials, camera traps and observations. More prey was removed during the day than at night, but prey removal was variable at the crop edge and dependent on the month (refecting crop growth and cover) and time of day. Overall, the predation of mealworms and beetles was 1-times and 13-times greater during the day than night, respectively, with predation on mealworms 3–5 times greater during the day than night at the crop edge compared to 95 m inside the crop. Camera traps identifed many insectivorous birds and bats over crops near the feeding trays, but there was no evidence of bats or small passerines removing experimental prey. A predation gradient from the crop edge was evident, but only in some months. This corresponded to the foraging preferences of open-space generalist predators (magpies) in low crop cover versus the shrubby habitat preferred by small passerines, likely facilitating foraging away from the crop edge later in the season. Our results are in line with Optimal Foraging Theory and suggest that predators trade-of foraging behaviour with predation risk at diferent distances from the crop edge and levels of crop cover.
    [Show full text]
  • How Much Is Explained by the Optimal Foraging Theory?
    [Downloaded free from http://www.conservationandsociety.org on Friday, January 13, 2012, IP: 129.79.203.177] || Click here to download free Android application for this journal Conservation and Society 9(3): 236-246, 2011 Article Foraging Behaviour of Brazilian Riverine and Coastal Fishers: How Much is Explained by the Optimal Foraging Theory? Priscila F.M. Lopesa,b #, Mariana Clauzeta,c, Natalia Hanazakia,d, Milena Ramiresa, Renato A.M. Silvanoa,e and Alpina Begossia,f aFisheries and Food Institute (FIFO), Campinas, SP, Brazil bDepto. de Botânica, Ecologia e Zoologia, CB, UFRN, Natal, RN, Brazil cPós Graduação em Desenvolvimento e Sociedade, UNICAMP, Campinas, SP, Brazil dDepto. de Ecologia e Zoologia, CCB, UFSC, Florianópolis, SC, Brazil eDepto. de Ecologia, UFRGS, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil fCAPESCA (LEPAC, PREAC) and CMU (UNICAMP), Campinas, SP, Brazil #Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] Abstract Optimal Foraging Theory (OFT) is here applied to analyse the foraging behaviour of Brazilian artisanal fi shers of the Atlantic coast (Itacuruçá and São Paulo Bagre villages) and of the inland Amazonian region (Jarauá and Ebenezer villages). Two OFT predictions are tested. Hypotheis1: A fi sher who travels to more distant sites should return with more fi sh, and Hypothesis 2: The further a fi sher goes, the longer s/he should stay fi shing in a patch. OFT did not explain fi shers’ behaviour (non-signifi cant regressions for coastal villages) or explain it in specifi c seasons (low water season for one Amazonian village: H1 r2=24.1; H2 r2=37.2) and in specifi c habitats (e.g., lakes and backwaters in Jarauá village, Lakes: H1 r2=13.5; H2 r2=24.0; Backwaters: H1 r2=34.4; H2 r2=46.5).
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of the Optimal Foraging Theory Literature in Zooarchaeology and Archaeobotany
    Research Communications Beyond Depression? A Review of the Optimal Foraging Theory Literature in Zooarchaeology and Archaeobotany Emily Lena Jones1* and David A. Hurley2 1Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA. 2University Libraries, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA. *[email protected] Abstract The use of optimal foraging theory in archaeology has been criticized for focusing heavily on “negative” human- environmental interactions, particularly anthropogenic resource depression, in which prey populations are reduced by foragers’ own foraging activities. In addition, some researchers have suggested the focus on resource depression is more common in the zooarchaeological literature than in the archaeobotanical literature, indicating fundamental differences in the ways zooarchaeologists and archaeobotanists approach the archaeological record. In this paper, we assess these critiques through a review of the literature between 1997 and 2017. We find that studies identifying resource depression occur at similar rates in the archaeobotanical and zooarchaeological literature. In addition, while earlier archaeological applications of optimal foraging theory did focus heavily on the identification of resource depression, the literature published between 2013 and 2017 shows a wider variety of approaches. Received September 5, 2016 OPEN ACCESS Accepted March 10, 2017 DOI 10.14237/ebl.8.1.2017.786 Keywords Resource depression, Optimal foraging theory, Prey choice, Zooarchaeology, Archaeobotany Copyright © 2017 by the author(s); licensee Society of Ethnobiology. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International Public License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0), which permits non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
    [Show full text]