Optimality Theory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Optimality Theory Optimality Theory OTA01 1 8/6/04, 11:15 AM H OTA01 2 8/6/04, 11:15 AM Optimality Theory Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar Alan Prince Rutgers University Paul Smolensky Johns Hopkins University OTA01 3 8/6/04, 11:15 AM © 2004 by Alan Prince and Paul Smolensky blackwell publishing 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148-5020, USA 108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK 550 Swanston Street, Carlton, Victoria 3053, Australia The right of Alan Prince and Paul Smolensky to be identified as the Authors of this Work has been asserted in accordance with the UK Copyright, Designs, and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, except as permitted by the UK Copyright, Designs, and Patents Act 1988, without the prior permission of the publisher. First published 2004 by Blackwell Publishing Ltd Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Prince, Alan. Optimality theory : Constraint interaction in generative grammar / Alan Prince and Paul Smolensky. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 1–4051–1932–2 (hardcover : alk. paper) — ISBN 1–4051–1933–0 (pbk. : alk. paper) 1. Optimality theory (Linguistics) 2. Constraints (Linguistics) 3. Grammar, Comparative and general—Phonology. I. Smolensky, Paul, 1955– II. Title. P158.42.P75 2004 415—dc22 2004000723 A catalogue record for this title is available from the British Library. Set in 10/12.5 pt Palatino by Graphicraft Limited, Hong Kong Printed and bound in the United Kingdom by MPG Books Ltd, Bodmin, Cornwall The publisher’s policy is to use permanent paper from mills that operate a sustainable forestry policy, and which has been manufactured from pulp processed using acid-free and elementary chlorine-free practices. Furthermore, the publisher ensures that the text paper and cover board used have met acceptable environmental accreditation standards. For further information on Blackwell Publishing, visit our website: http://www.blackwellpublishing.com OTA01 4 8/6/04, 11:15 AM Contents Prefatory Note ix Acknowledgments x 1 Preliminaries 1 1.1 Background and Overview 1 1.2 Optimality 4 1.3 Overall Structure of the Argument 8 Part I Optimality and Constraint Interaction 11 Overview of Part I 13 2 Optimality in Grammar: Core Syllabification in Imdlawn Tashlhiyt Berber 14 2.1 The Heart of Dell and Elmedlaoui 14 2.2 Optimality Theory 20 2.3 Summary of Discussion to Date 26 3 Generalization-Forms in Domination Hierarchies I Do Something Only When: Triggering, or The Theory of Economy 27 Blocking and Triggering: Profuseness and Economy 27 3.1 Epenthetic Structure 28 3.2 The Failure of Bottom-Up Constructionism 33 4 Generalization-Forms in Domination Hierarchies II Do Something Except When: Blocking, or The Theory of Profuseness 39 OTA01 5 8/6/04, 11:15 AM vi Contents 4.1 Edge-Oriented Infixation 40 4.2 Interaction of Weight Effects with Extrametricality 45 4.2.1 Background: Prominence-Driven Stress Systems 45 4.2.2 The Interaction of Weight and Extrametricality: Kelkar’s Hindi/Urdu 47 4.3 Nonfinality and Nonexhaustiveness 50 4.3.1 Nonfinality and the Laws of Foot Form: Raw Minimality 56 4.3.2 Nonfinality and the Laws of Foot Form: Extended Minimality Effects 61 4.4 Summary of Discussion of the Except When Effect 65 4.5 Except Meets Only: Triggering and Blocking in a Single Grammar 66 5 The Construction of Grammar in Optimality Theory 80 5.1 Construction of Harmonic Orderings from Phonetic and Structural Scales 80 5.2 The Theory of Constraint Interaction 82 5.2.1 Comparison of Entire Candidates by a Single Constraint 82 5.2.1.1 Ons: Binary constraints 82 5.2.1.2 Hnuc: Non-binary constraints 86 5.2.2 Comparison of Entire Candidates by an Entire Constraint Hierarchy 88 5.2.3 Discussion 91 5.2.3.1 Non-locality of interaction 91 5.2.3.2 Strictness of domination 94 5.2.3.3 Serial vs. parallel Harmony evaluation and Gen 94 5.2.3.4 Binary vs. non-binary constraints 97 5.3 Panini’s Theorem on Constraint Ranking 97 Part II Syllable Theory 101 Overview of Part II 103 6 Syllable Structure Typology: The CV Theory 105 6.1 The Jakobson Typology 105 6.2 The Faithfulness Interactions 107 6.2.1 Groundwork 108 6.2.2 Basic CV Syllable Theory 111 6.2.2.1 Onsets 111 6.2.2.2 Codas 114 6.2.3 The Theory of Epenthesis Sites 116 7 Constraint Interaction in Lardil Phonology 119 7.1 The Constraints 120 7.2 The Ranking 129 OTA01 6 8/6/04, 11:15 AM Contents vii 7.2.1 Some Ranking Logic 129 7.2.2 Ranking the Constraints 132 7.3 Verification of Forms 139 7.3.1 Consonant-Final Stems 140 7.3.2 Vowel-Final Stems 143 7.4 Discussion 146 8 Universal Syllable Theory: Ordinal Construction of C/V and Onset/Coda Licensing Asymmetry 150 8.1 Associational Harmony 156 8.1.1 Deconstructing Hnuc: Berber, Take 1 156 8.1.2 Restricting to Binary Marks 159 8.2 Reconstructing the C and V Classes: Emergent Parameter Setting via Constraint Ranking 164 8.2.1 Harmonic Completeness of Possible Onsets and Peaks 164 8.2.2 Peak- and Margin-Affinity 166 8.2.3 Interactions with Parse 168 8.2.4 Restricting Deletion and Epenthesis 169 8.2.5 Further Necessary Conditions on Possible Onsets and Nuclei 170 8.2.6 Sufficient Conditions on Possible Onsets and Nuclei 172 8.3 The Typology of Onset, Nucleus, and Coda Inventories 177 8.3.1 The Typology of Onset and Nucleus Inventories 177 8.3.2 Onset/Coda Licensing Asymmetries 184 8.3.3 An Example: Berber, Take 2 193 8.4 Simplifying the Theory by Encapsulating Constraint Packages 197 8.4.1 Encapsulating the Association Hierarchies 197 8.4.2 An Example: Berber, Take 3 199 8.4.3 Sufficiency and Richness of the Encapsulated Theory 200 Part III Issues and Answers in Optimality Theory 203 Overview of Part III 205 9 Inventory Theory and the Lexicon 207 9.1 Language-Particular Inventories 207 9.1.1 Harmonic Bounding and Nucleus, Syllable, and Word Inventories 209 9.1.2 Segmental Inventories 212 9.2 Universal Inventories 218 9.2.1 Segmental Inventories 219 9.2.2 Syllabic Inventories 224 9.3 Optimality in the Lexicon 225 OTA01 7 8/6/04, 11:15 AM viii Contents 10 Foundational Issues and Theory-Comparisons 232 10.1 Thinking about Optimality 232 10.1.1 Fear of Optimization 232 10.1.2 The Reassurance 233 10.2 The Connectionism Connection, and Other Computation- Based Comparisons 234 10.2.1 Why Optimality Theory Has Nothing To Do with Connectionism 234 10.2.2 Why Optimality Theory Is Deeply Connected to Connectionism 235 10.2.3 Harmony Maximization and Symbolic Cognition 236 10.3 Analysis of ‘Phonotactics + Repair’ Theories 238 10.3.1 CV Syllable Structure and Repair 242 10.3.2 General Structure of the Comparisons: Repair Analysis 245 10.3.3 Persistent Rule Theory 247 10.3.3.1 English closed syllable shortening 248 10.3.3.2 Shona tone spreading 250 10.3.3.3 Summary 251 10.3.4 The Theory of Constraints and Repair Strategies 252 Appendix 258 A.1 The Cancellation and Cancellation/Domination Lemmas 258 A.2 CV Syllable Structure 262 A.3 Panini’s Theorem on Constraint Ranking 264 References 266 Index of Constraints 281 Index of Languages 283 General Index 284 OTA01 8 8/6/04, 11:15 AM Prefatory Note This book derives from an informally circulated manuscript which was issued as a Technical Report by the University of Colorado Computer Science Department (CU-CS-696-95) and the Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science (RuCCS-TR-2), eventually coming to repose on the Rutgers Optimality Archive as ROA-537 (http://roa.rutgers.edu). The current text is content-wise identical to its predecessors, with correction of as many typos, oversights, inconsistencies, and outright errors as we could track down. Footnote and example numbering has been retained. In revising the biblio- graphy, we have tried to reconcile the twin goals of reference: to identify our sources and to provide the reader with usable modes of access to them. Those familiar with an earlier version of the text will not find new notions or notations here, but in various places a certain amount of local rewording has been attempted in the name of clarity. The authors’ names are arranged in lexicographic order. Alan Prince Paul Smolensky January, 2004 OTA01 9 8/6/04, 11:15 AM Acknowledgments Special thanks to John McCarthy for detailed discussion of virtually every issue raised here and for a fine-grained skepsis of the entire first draft; special thanks also to Junko Itô, Armin Mester, and Jaye Padgett for a valuable fine-grained re- view of the completed manuscript. These have resulted in innumerable improve- ments and would have resulted in innumerably more, were this a better world. We wish to thank Robert Kirchner, Armin Mester, and Junko Itô for specific remarks that have had significant impact on the development of this work; John McCarthy for his comments and suggestions in re chapters 7 and 9; Jane Grimshaw, Géraldine Legendre, and Bruce Tesar for much valuable discussion; as well as Vieri Samek-Lodovici, Cheryl Zoll, Henrietta Hung, Mark Hewitt, Ad Neeleman, Diana Archangeli, Henry Churchyard, Doug Pulleyblank, Moira Yip, Tom Bever, Larry Hyman, Andy Black, Mike Jordan, David Perlmutter, Lauri Karttunen, René Kager, Paul Kiparsky, Mike Kenstowicz, Ellis Visch, András Kornai, Akin Akinlabi, Clayton Lewis, Merrill Garrett, Jim Martin, Clara Levelt, Mike Mozer, Maria Bittner, Alison Prince, Dave Rumelhart, Mark Liberman, Jacques Mehler, Steve Pinker, Daniel Büring, Katharina Hartmann, Joshua Legendre Smolensky, Ray Jackendoff, Bruce Hayes, Geoff Pullum, Gyanam Mahajan, Harry van der Hulst, William Labov, Brian McHugh, Gene Buckley, Will Leben, Jaye Padgett, Loren Billings, and Fuad Isabegovic.
Recommended publications
  • Paul Smolensky
    Vita PAUL SMOLENSKY Department of Cognitive Science 11824 Mays Chapel Road 239A Krieger Hall Timonium, MD 21093-1821 Johns Hopkins University (667) 229-9509 Baltimore, MD 21218-2685 May 5, 1955 (410) 516-5331 Citizenship: USA [email protected] cogsci.jhu.edu/directory/paul-smolensky/ DEGREES Ph.D. in mathematical physics, Indiana University, 1981. M.S. in physics, Indiana University, 1977. A.B. summa cum laude in physics, Harvard University, 1976. PROFESSIONAL POSITIONS Partner Researcher, Microsoft Research Artificial Intelligence, Redmond WA, Dec. 2016−present. Krieger-Eisenhower Professor of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, 2006–present. Full Professor, Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, 1994–2006. Chair, Department of Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University, Jan. 1997−June 1998 (Acting), July 1998−June 2000 Professor, Department of Computer Science, University of Colorado at Boulder, Full Professor, 1994–95 (on leave, 1994–95). Associate Professor, 1990–94. Assistant Professor, 1985–90. Assistant Research Cognitive Scientist (Assistant Professor – Research), Institute for Cognitive Science, University of California at San Diego, 1982–85. Visiting Scholar, Program in Cognitive Science, University of California at San Diego, 1981–82. Adjunct Professor, Department of Linguistics, University of Maryland at College Park, 1994–2010. Assistant Director, Center for Language and Speech Processing, Johns Hopkins University, 1995–2008. Director, NSF IGERT Training Program, Unifying the Science of Language, 2006−2015. Director, NSF IGERT Training Program in the Cognitive Science of Language, 1999−2006. International Chair, Inria Paris (National Institute for Research in Computer Science and Automation), 2017−2021. Visiting Scientist, Inserm-CEA Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, NeuroSpin Center, Paris, France, 2016.
    [Show full text]
  • University of Groningen Finding the Right Words Bíró, Tamás Sándor
    University of Groningen Finding the right words Bíró, Tamás Sándor IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2006 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Bíró, T. S. (2006). Finding the right words: implementing optimality theory with simulated annealing. s.n. Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). The publication may also be distributed here under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license. More information can be found on the University of Groningen website: https://www.rug.nl/library/open-access/self-archiving-pure/taverne- amendment. Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 29-09-2021 Bibliography Arto Anttila. Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 20:1{42, 2002. Also: ROA-425.
    [Show full text]
  • A View from Gradient Harmonic Grammar
    Asymmetries in Long-Distance Dependencies: A View from Gradient Harmonic Grammar constituent class membership to a degree, and presupposes that instead of standard category symbols like [X], there are weighted category symbols like [αX] (where α ranges over the real numbers in Hyunjung Lee & Gereon M¨uller (Universit¨at Leipzig) [0,1]). Rules, filters, and other syntactic building blocks are given upper and lower threshold values WorkshoponLong-DistanceDependencies,HUBerlin October 4-5, 2018 of α between which they operate. 1. Introduction Note: This way, the concept of varying strength of syntactic categories (see Chomsky (2015) for a recent Claim: reappraisal) can be formally implemented in the grammar. Gradient Harmonic Grammar (Smolensky & Goldrick (2016)) offers a new perspective on how to derive three different types of asymmetries as they can be observed with long-distance dependencies Observation: in the world’s languages: So far, most of the work on GHG has been in phonology (e.g., Zimmermann (2017), Faust & Smo- • asymmetries between movement types lensky (2017), Kushnir (2018)); but cf. Smolensky (2017), M¨uller (2017b), Lee (2018) for syntactic applications. • asymmetries between types of moved items • asymmetries between types of local domain 1.3. Harmonic Serialism Background assumptions: Note: (i) Harmonic Grammar Harmonic serialism is a strictly derivational version of optimality theory. (ii) Gradient Representations (3) Harmonic serialism (McCarthy (2008), Heck & M¨uller (2013)): (iii) Harmonic Serialism a. GivensomeinputIi, the candidate set CSi = {Oi1,Oi2, ...Oin} is generated by applying at 1.1. Harmonic Grammar most one operation to Ii. Harmonic Grammar (Smolensky & Legendre (2006), Pater (2016)): A version of optimality theory b.
    [Show full text]
  • Inflectional Morphology in Optimality Theory
    Inflectional Morphology in Optimality Theory A Dissertation Presented by Zheng Xu to The Graduate School in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics Stony Brook University August 2007 Copyright by Zheng Xu August 2007 Stony Brook University The Graduate School Zheng Xu We, the dissertation committee for the above candidate for the Doctor of Philosophy degree, hereby recommend acceptance of this dissertation Mark Aronoff, Professor of Linguistics Robert D. Hoberman, Professor of Linguistics Alice C. Harris, Professor of Linguistics James P. Blevins, Assistant Director of Research Research Center for English and Applied Linguistics University of Cambridge This dissertation is accepted by the Graduate School Lawrence Martin Dean of the Graduate School ii Abstract of the Dissertation Inflectional Morphology in Optimality Theory by Zheng Xu Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics Stony Brook University 2007 This dissertation proposes an inferential-realizational model of inflectional morphology (Matthews 1972, Zwicky 1985, Anderson 1992, Aronoff 1994, Stump 2001) within the framework of Optimality Theory (Prince and Smolensky 1993). Following Russell 1995, Yip 1998, Hyman 2003, MacBride 2004, I assume that the phonological information of inflectional affixes is introduced through realization constraints (RC) which associate abstract morphosyntactic or semantic feature values with phonological forms. I propose that rankings of realization constraints conform to the specificity condition, i.e. a constraint realizing a more specific morphosyntactic feature value set outranks a less specific realization constraint. I also propose that the unmarked situation in which one feature value is realized by one form (Wurzel 1989) is encoded in two universal and violable markedness constraints, *FEATURE SPLIT which bans the realization of a feature value by more than one form and *FEATURE FUSION which bans a form realizing more than one feature value.
    [Show full text]
  • Generalized Alignment John J
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series Linguistics January 1993 Generalized alignment John J. McCarthy University of Massachusetts, Amherst, [email protected] Alan Prince Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs Part of the Morphology Commons, Near Eastern Languages and Societies Commons, and the Phonetics and Phonology Commons Recommended Citation McCarthy, John J. and Prince, Alan, "Generalized alignment" (1993). Yearbook of Morphology. 12. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs/12 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Linguistics at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Generalized Alignment* John J. McCarthy Alan S. Prince University of Massachusetts, Amherst Rutgers University §1. Introduction Overt or covert reference to the edges of constituents is a commonplace throughout phonology and morphology. Some examples include: •In English, Garawa, Indonesian and a number of other languages, the normal right-to-left alternation of stress is interrupted word-initially: (1) Initial Secondary Stress in English (Tàta)ma(góuchee) *Ta(tàma)(góuchee) (Lùxi)pa(lílla) *Lu(xìpa)(lílla) As the foot-brackets ( ) indicate, the favored outcome is one in which
    [Show full text]
  • Harmonic Grammar with Linear Programming
    To appear in Phonology 27(1):1–41 February 18, 2010 Harmonic grammar with linear programming: From linear systems to linguistic typology∗ Christopher Potts Joe Pater Stanford University UMass Amherst Karen Jesney Rajesh Bhatt UMass Amherst UMass Amherst Michael Becker Harvard University Abstract Harmonic Grammar (HG) is a model of linguistic constraint interac- tion in which well-formedness is calculated in terms of the sum of weighted constraint violations. We show how linear programming algorithms can be used to determine whether there is a weighting for a set of constraints that fits a set of linguistic data. The associated software package OT-Help provides a practical tool for studying large and complex linguistic systems in the HG framework and comparing the results with those of OT. We first describe the translation from harmonic grammars to systems solvable by linear programming algorithms. We then develop an HG analysis of ATR harmony in Lango that is, we argue, superior to the existing OT and rule-based treatments. We further highlight the usefulness of OT-Help, and the analytic power of HG, with a set of studies of the predictions HG makes for phonological typology. Keywords: Harmonic Grammar, Optimality Theory, linear programming, typology, Lango, ATR harmony, positional markedness, positional faithfulness 1 Introduction We examine a model of grammar that is identical to the standard version of Optimality Theory (OT: Prince & Smolensky 1993/2004), except that the optimal Our thanks to Ash Asudeh, Tim Beechey, Maitine Bergonioux, Paul Boersma, John Colby, Kathryn ∗ Flack, Edward Flemming, Bob Frank, John Goldmsith, Maria Gouskova, Bruce Hayes, René Kager, Shigeto Kawahara, John Kingston, John McCarthy, Andrew McKenzie, Ramgopal Mettu, Alan Prince, Kathryn Pruitt, Jason Riggle, Jim Smith, and Paul Smolensky, and other participants in conferences and courses where this material was presented.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Optimality Theory
    9781405151368_4_001.qxd 11/30/07 5:01 PM Page 1 1 An Introduction to Optimality Theory 1.1 How OT Began Around 1990, Alan Prince and Paul Smolensky began collaborating on a new theory of human language. This collaboration led in fairly short order to a book-length manuscript, Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Photocopies of the manuscript were widely distributed and had a terrific impact on the field of linguistics, even though it wasn’t formally published until more than a decade later (as Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004). OT had and continues to have its greatest effect on phonology, but it has also led to important work in syntax, semantics, sociolinguistics, historical linguistics, and other areas. OT belongs on anyone’s list of the top three developments in the history of generative grammar. One of Prince and Smolensky’s goals for OT was to solve a long- standing problem in phonology. Phonological theory in the tradition of Chomsky and Halle’s (1968) The Sound Pattern of English (SPE) was based on rewrite rules. The rewrite rule A → B/C___D describes an input configuration CAD and an A → B transformation that applies to it. Rewrite rules can describe lots of phenomena, but they do a poor job of explaining how phonological systems fit together. (For a brief explanation of SPE’s main assumptions, see the boxed text at the end of this section.) To illustrate, we’ll look at some data from Yawelmani, a nearly extinct dialect of the California Penutian language Yokuts (Newman 1944).1 In this language, syllables cannot be bigger than CVC (consonant- vowel-consonant).
    [Show full text]
  • Ranking and Necessity
    Ranking and Necessity Part I: The Fusional Reduction Algorithm ● 12-16-2005 ● Adrian Brasoveanu and Alan Prince Department of Linguistics Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science Rutgers University, New Brunswick ABSTRACT Understanding a linguistic theory within OT requires an exact characterization of the ranking conditions necessitated by data. We describe (Part I) and justify (Part II) an algorithm which calculates the necessary and sufficient ranking conditions inherent in any collection of candidates and presents them in a maximally concise and informative way. The algorithm, stemming from the original proposal of Brasoveanu 2003, develops in the setting of the fusional ERC theory of Prince 2002. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors, whose names are arranged alphabetically, would like to thank Jane Grimshaw, Naz Merchant, Paul Smolensky, and Bruce Tesar for useful discussion; and, for helpful comments, audiences at the Rutgers Optimality Research Group (April 2004, Nov. 2005), HUMDRUM (May 2004), and the LSA 2005 Summer Institute. For support during aspects of the investigation, we thank the John Simon Guggenheim Memorial Foundation, the National Science Foundation (Grant No. BCS-0083101), and the National Institutes of Health (NRSA Training Grant No. 1-T32-MH-19975-05). The opinions, findings, conclusions, and recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation or of the National Institutes of Health. The authors also claim responsibility for any errors, omissions, misprisions, or infelicities that may have crept into the text. Ranking and Necessity Part I 1. Beyond the Sufficient………………………………………………………………..3 2. The Consequences of Comparison………………………………………………..6 2.1 The Elementary Ranking Condition……………………………………..6 2.2 Informativeness……………………………………………………………9 2.3 ERCs and Consequences………………………………………………12 2.4 Remark on the Logical Background…………………………………...23 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetically Driven Phonology: the Role of Optimality Theory and Inductive Grounding1
    Phonetically Driven Phonology: The Role of Optimality Theory and Inductive Grounding1 Bruce P. Hayes UCLA Final version: August 1997 Written for the proceedings volume of the 1996 Milwaukee Conference on Formalism and Functionalism in Linguistics Abstract Functionalist phonetic literature has shown how the phonologies of human languages are arranged to facilitate ease of articulation and perception. The explanatory force of phonological theory is greatly increased if it can directly access these research results. There are two formal mechanisms that together can facilitate the link-up of formal to functional work. As others have noted, Optimality Theory, with its emphasis on directly incorporating principles of markedness, can serve as part of the bridge. Another mechanism is proposed here: an algorithm for inductive grounding permits the language learner to access the knowledge gained from experience in articulation and perception, and form from it the appropriate set of formal phonological constraints. 1 I would like to thank Marco Baroni, Patricia Keating, Donca Steriade, and members of talk audiences at MIT, Arizona, Tilburg, Utrecht, Cornell, UC Santa Cruz, UCLA, and the Formalism/Functionalism conference for helpful input in the preparation of this paper. Phonetically-Driven Phonology p. 2 1. Phonological Functionalism The difference between formalist and functionalist approaches in linguistics has taken different forms in different areas. For phonology, and particularly for the study of fully- productive sound patterns, the functionalist approach has traditionally been phonetic in character. For some time, work in the phonetic literature, such as Ohala (1974, 1978, 1981, 1983), Ohala and OHALA (1993), Liljencrants and Lindblom (1972), Lindblom (1983, 1990), and Westbury and Keating (1986), has argued that the sound patterns of languages are effectively arranged to facilitate ease of articulation and distinctness of contrasting forms in perception.
    [Show full text]
  • Generative Linguistics and Neural Networks at 60: Foundation, Friction, and Fusion*
    Generative linguistics and neural networks at 60: foundation, friction, and fusion* Joe Pater, University of Massachusetts Amherst October 3, 2018. Abstract. The birthdate of both generative linguistics and neural networks can be taken as 1957, the year of the publication of foundational work by both Noam Chomsky and Frank Rosenblatt. This paper traces the development of these two approaches to cognitive science, from their largely autonomous early development in their first thirty years, through their collision in the 1980s around the past tense debate (Rumelhart and McClelland 1986, Pinker and Prince 1988), and their integration in much subsequent work up to the present. Although this integration has produced a considerable body of results, the continued general gulf between these two lines of research is likely impeding progress in both: on learning in generative linguistics, and on the representation of language in neural modeling. The paper concludes with a brief argument that generative linguistics is unlikely to fulfill its promise of accounting for language learning if it continues to maintain its distance from neural and statistical approaches to learning. 1. Introduction At the beginning of 1957, two men nearing their 29th birthdays published work that laid the foundation for two radically different approaches to cognitive science. One of these men, Noam Chomsky, continues to contribute sixty years later to the field that he founded, generative linguistics. The book he published in 1957, Syntactic Structures, has been ranked as the most influential work in cognitive science from the 20th century.1 The other one, Frank Rosenblatt, had by the late 1960s largely moved on from his research on perceptrons – now called neural networks – and died tragically young in 1971.
    [Show full text]
  • Speakers Treat Transparent and Opaque Alternation Patterns Differently — Evidence from Chinese Tone Sandhi*
    This is a printout of the final PDF file and has been approved by me, the author. Any mistakes in this printout will not be fixed by the publisher. Here is my signature and the date 06/10/2018 Speakers Treat Transparent and Opaque Alternation Patterns Differently — Evidence from Chinese Tone Sandhi* Jie Zhang 1. Introduction 1.1 Opacity The study of opacity has had a long tradition in generative phonology. Kiparsky (1973: p.79) defined two types of opacity for a phonological rule, as in (1). The first type is also known as underapplication opacity, and the opaque rule is non-surface-true; the second type is also known as overapplication opacity, and the opaque rule is non-surface-apparent (e.g., Bakovic 2007). (1) Opacity: A phonological rule P of the form A → B / C __ D is opaque if there are surface structures with any of the following characteristics: (a) Instance of A in the environment C __ D; (b) Instance of B derived by P in environments other than C __ D. In the rule-based framework, opacity can generally be derived by ordering the opaque rule before another rule that could either set up or destroy the application of the opaque rule (but see Bakovic 2007, 2011, who advocates the decoupling of opacity and rule ordering). In a surface-oriented theory like Optimality Theory (OT; Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004), however, opacity poses significant challenges. Without modification to the theory, neither underapplication nor overapplication opacity can be straightforwardly derived. A number of general solutions for opacity have been proposed within OT, including the Sympathy Theory (McCarthy 1999), Stratal OT (Kiparsky 2000, Nazarov and Pater 2017), OT with Candidate Chains (OT-CC; McCarthy 2007), and Serial Markedness Reduction (Jarosz 2014, 2016).
    [Show full text]
  • Optimality Theory
    This page intentionally left blank This page intentionally left blank Optimality Theory This is an introduction to Optimality Theory, the central idea of which is that surface forms of language reflect resolutions of conflicts be- tween competing constraints. A surface form is ‘optimal’ if it incurs the least serious violations of a set of constraints, taking into account their hierarchical ranking. Languages differ in the ranking of con- straints; and any violations must be minimal. The book does not limit its empirical scope to phonological phenomena, but also contains chapters on the learnability of OT grammars; OT’s implications for syntax; and other issues such as opacity. It also reviews in detail a selection of the considerable research output which OT has already produced. Exercises accompany chapters 1–7, and there are sections on further reading. Optimality Theory will be welcomed by any lin- guist with a basic knowledge of derivational Generative Phonology. RENÉ KAGER teaches linguistics at Utrecht University, the Netherlands. CAMBRIDGE TEXTBOOKS IN LINGUISTICS General editors: s. r. anderson, j. bresnan, b. comrie, w. dressler, c. ewen, r. huddleston, r. lass, d. lightfoot, j. lyons, p. h. matthews, r. posner, s. romaine, n. v. smith, n. vincent OPTIMALITY THEORY In this series p. h. matthews Morphology Second edition b. comrie Aspect r. m. kempson Semantic Theory t. bynon Historical Linguistics j. allwood, l.-g. anderson and ö . dahl Logic in Linguistics d. b. fry The Physics of Speech r. a. hudson Sociolinguistics Second edition a. j. elliott Child Language p. h. matthews Syntax a. radford Transformational Syntax l.
    [Show full text]