3V\FKRORJLFD Deak, C and Saroglou, V (2017). Terminating a Child’s Life? Religious, Moral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors Underlying Non-Acceptance ψ %HOJLFD of Child . Psychologica Belgica, 57(1), pp. 59–76, DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.341

RESEARCH ARTICLE Terminating a Child’s Life? Religious, Moral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors Underlying Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia Csilla Deak*,† and Vassilis Saroglou*

Is opposition to child euthanasia motivated only by ideology, or also by other ­personality characteristics and individual differences? In , the first country to legalize child euthanasia (in 2014), we investigated religious, moral, emotional, and cognitive factors underlying the (dis)approval of this legalization (N = 213). Disapproval was associated with religiousness, collectivistic morality (loyalty and purity), and prosocial dispositions, in terms of emotional empathy and behavioral generosity, but not values (care and fairness). It was also associated with low ­flexibility in existential issues and a high endorsement of slippery slope arguments, but not necessarily low openness to experience. A regression analysis showed that in addition to religiousness, low flexibility in existential issues and high empathy and generosity distinctly predicted opposition to child euthanasia. Whereas most of the findings parallel those previously reported for adult euthanasia, the role of prosocial inclinations in predicting moral opposition seems to be specific to child euthanasia.

Keywords: conservatism; euthanasia; moral foundations; prosociality; religion

Though socially and morally ­controversial, In 2002, euthanasia, defined as a deliberate euthanasia- of humans or animals- has life-ending act by another ­person at the been practiced for centuries across cultures patient’s request, became legal. Twelve (McDougall & Norman, 2007). Recently, in years later, in 2014, Belgium became the a few countries in the world, euthanasia of first country in the world to legalize child human adults has been decriminalized with euthanasia, that is, to extend the possibil- the law in these countries specifying condi- ity of legal euthanasia to minors, without tions under which this practice is permit- any age limit—whereas, technically, in the ted. Belgium is one of these few countries: , euthanasia of minors is legally not extended under the age of 12. Both adult * Université catholique de Louvain, BE and child euthanasia are generally accepted † ­Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research, BE in Belgian society, with the latter being Corresponding author: Csilla Deak supported by about 74% of the population ([email protected]) (before the law was passed; Laporte, 2013). 60 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia

Even if largely accepted within Belgium, imply opposition to euthanasia in general, non-negligible variability still exists in the ­including child euthanasia (Gill, 1998). In the attitudes of acceptance versus disapproval past (Grell & Cunningham, 2007), and some- of the legalization of child euthanasia (e.g., times today in fundamentalist communities,­ Roelands, Van den Block, Geurts, Deliens, religions have occasionally been, or may & Cohen, 2015). There are also individuals, still be, reluctant to accept any medical groups, or scholars who publicly criticize this ­procedure on the human body (Vess, Arndt, law or some of its aspects (e.g., Siegel, Sisti, & Cox, Routledge, & Goldenberg, 2009). Caplan, 2014). The present work focuses on Previous research has indeed shown that the psychological individual differences that religiosity typically relates to low accept- may explain this variability. More precisely, ance of adult euthanasia, both within and it aims to investigate the religious, moral, across countries, be they religious or secular emotional, and cognitive characteristics that ­countries (for national studies: Aghababaei, distinguish law opponents from the societal 2014; Deak & Saroglou, 2015; Stolz et al., majority; or, in correlational terms, charac- 2015; for international studies in Europe teristics that are related to, and may predict and beyond: Cohen et al., 2006; Verbakel & (in a regression), high versus low disapproval Jaspers, 2010), as well as across time within of child euthanasia. There are three broad countries (Danyliv & O’Neill, 2015; Jaspers, questions of particular interest here. First, Lubbers, & De Graaf, 2007). Interestingly, does the disapproval of child euthanasia Catholic individuals and countries are less mostly/exclusively reflect ideological pref- accepting of euthanasia than Protestant indi- erences such as religious beliefs and moral viduals and countries, very likely because preferences, or does it (also) reflect subtle the former are generally more traditional ­psychological, cognitive and emotional, fac- (Verbakel & Jaspers, 2010). Note also that, tors? Moreover, are psychological character- across time, there is some polarization istics of moral opponents the same for adult in Europe, with most of Western Europe and child euthanasia, or does the minor status becoming more permissive and most of of the child play a role? Finally, does prosocial Eastern Europe becoming less permissive orientation underline the approval of child of ­euthanasia (Cohen, Van Landeghem, euthanasia for alleviating suffering or, on the Carpentier, & Deliens, 2014). These trends contrary, the disapproval of it for preserving can be explained as resulting from, respec- the innocents’ ? The present work tively, a growing secularization process in the is the first, to our knowledge, to examine the former countries, and the intensification of above questions, and it does so in Belgium, a traditional and identity affirmation-oriented society where child euthanasia is accepted by forms of religion in the latter countries fol- a majority of the population. Below we will lowing their exit from atheist communist present these moral and religious, as well as regimes. Overall, religions have become cognitive and emotional, dispositions and more liberal with regard to euthanasia develop the corresponding hypotheses. (Moulton, Hill, & Burdette, 2006), and one study conducted in the UK even reported Religious Ideology religious acceptance of euthanasia (Hains & We first expected disapproval of the ­Hulbert-Williams, 2013). Nevertheless, atten- legalization of child euthanasia to be typi- uation does not mean cessation; and the basic cal of people high in religiosity. Religious reason of religious opposition to euthanasia­ worldviews usually emphasize the idea mentioned above (the literal belief in God that the world and the life of each living as the supreme master of each being’s life) being are created by God, and God is solely remains central in religious faith. responsible for the termination of each life. Our expectation of religiousness being Endorsing (literally) this idea is thus likely to associated with a discomfort with child Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia 61 euthanasia is not simply an extension of Collectivistic Moral Orientation the theoretical and empirical evidence on Behind, but also beyond, the role of religious adult euthanasia, but is based on additional ideology, we also expected the disapproval and subtler psychological reasons. In fact, of child euthanasia to be related to a strong child euthanasia may represent a stronger endorsement of conservative-traditional moral- moral dilemma for the religious person than ity, that is, in terms of the Moral Foundations adult euthanasia, which typically implicates Theory (Haidt & Graham, 2007), collectivistic elderly persons. Given previous theory and moral foundations. These foundations include evidence attesting consistent links between loyalty to the group, respect for authority, religiosity and just-world beliefs (Jost et al., and purity, that is, valuing the preservation 2014), it may be that religious people find of the natural and sacred order of things and the termination of an “innocent” minor’s the world. These three values constitute what life particularly unjust. At the same time, Haidt and colleagues call “binding” morality, a the prosocial orientation – in attitudes, moral orientation favoring society over indi- ­values, and dispositions – of religious per- viduals. These values are highly endorsed in sons, at least toward ingroup members and collectivistic cultural contexts, and by conserv- non-immoral persons (Saroglou, 2006, 2013, ative people in individualistic cultural contexts for reviews), may, on the contrary, cause dis- (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009). comfort for religious persons in front of an Thus, disapproval of child euthanasia “innocent” child’s extreme suffering, and may stem from loyalty, that is, the strong could potentially allow them to be more endorsement by all members of society of open to the idea of euthanasia as the only the obligation to participate and contribute, solution to alleviate such suffering. Thus, through respective and reciprocal duties, child euthanasia may put the religious to the society’s existence, cohesion, and ­person in a strong moral dilemma where ­development: No member is allowed to sim- compassion toward a terribly and incurably ply be discharged. They may also disapprove suffering “innocent” child conflicts with the of this modern, liberal practice simply out of moral duty to preserve human life. Indeed, respect for traditional norms as expressed by research in recent years has suggested that, various sources of societal authority. Finally, generally, when moral deontology is in and importantly, purity is also likely to be conflict with prosocial concerns, religious involved, because terminating a person’s life, ­people may not be at ease with deciding in particular a child’s life, through medical what is best due to their endorsement of technology created by humans, can be per- both interpersonal and non-interpersonal ceived as violating the natural order of things morality (Deak & Saroglou, 2016; Johnson and the world. et al., 2016; Malka, Soto, Cohen, & Miller, Previous research has shown the role 2011). Nevertheless, the existing evidence played by the above-mentioned “binding” suggests that religiosity is typically more collectivistic morality in the opposition to strongly related to deontological morality the moral liberalization of various issues than interpersonal morality (Piazza & Sousa, related to sex, family, and individual life, 2014; Schmitt & Fuller, 2015). Consequently, such as divorce, , homosexuality, we favored the hypothesis that religios- and , as they interfere with the pres- ity would relate to opposition to child ervation of social groups (Deak & Saroglou, ­euthanasia. This would hold in Belgium, the 2015; Koleva, Graham, Iyer, Ditto, & Haidt, country where this study was conducted. 2012; Rottman, Kelemen, & Young, 2014). Although Belgium is a high scoring secular Two recent studies have shown that this Western country, it has a strong Catholic also includes adult euthanasia (Deak & ­tradition, which, as mentioned above, would Saroglou, 2015, purity and loyalty; Koleva favor the religious opposition hypothesis.1 et al., 2012, purity). 62 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia

Note that religiosity can be perceived to there is evidence showing that the former inherently include the endorsement of the morality may in some cases even come at three binding moral foundations (Graham & the detriment of interpersonal morality (e.g., Haidt, 2010), and has indeed been found to Piazza & Sousa, 2014; Van Pachterbeke, predict their strong endorsement, in particu- Freyer, & Saroglou, 2011). lar purity (Deak & Saroglou, 2016; Jonhson In favor of this hypothetical direction et al., 2016). Thus, it may be that religiosity (disapproval of child euthanasia is stronger and collectivistic morality share a significant among people low in prosociality), some pre- common variance in predicting the disap- vious research indicates that opposition to proval of child euthanasia. at least adult euthanasia is fueled solely by a strong emphasis on collectivist morality (in Other-Oriented Emotions and particular, loyalty and purity), and does not Motives reflect an emphasis on interpersonal moral- Beyond the role of religious ideology and ity (care and fairness) or empathy (Deak & conservative/collectivistic moral orientation, Saroglou, 2015; Koleva et al., 2012). This one can argue that other-orientated motives evidence could thus be extended to child may relate to the acceptance or disapproval euthanasia. of child euthanasia. Facing the possibility On the other hand, it can be argued that of alleviating extreme and hopeless suffer- the decision to end a child’s life, even if ing of a minor, by allowing or accelerating for good reasons, is difficult for someone his/her to occur, raises the fol- who is highly empathetic and ascribes high lowing question: do people with strong importance to the values care and fairness. ­other-oriented motives tend to favor Compared to an elderly person, a seriously ­euthanasia or disapprove of it? These proso- ill child has not yet experienced the rich- cial inclinations may involve values (endors- ness of life. Not surprisingly, people are less ing the importance of caring for and not favorable of child than adult euthanasia harming others), emotions (feeling empathy (Stolz et al., 2015), and after the loss of for another person’s unfortunate situation), a child is more complicated than other kinds and behavioral inclinations (actively show- of bereavement (e.g., Neidig & Dalgas-Pelish, ing selfless generosity). We investigated this 1991). Terminating a child’s life eliminates issue while being open to two competing even the smallest chance for him/her to hypotheses. have positive future experiences. Thus, the On the one hand, it can be argued that the disapproval of child euthanasia, in contrast disapproval of the legal possibility of child to adult euthanasia, may be stronger among euthanasia due to strong deontological, col- people with high other-oriented motives as lectivistic moral reasons (“we should not allow reflected in their prosocial values, emotions, or help, under any circumstances, individuals and behavior. to end their life”) also indicates weak other- In favor of this second hypothesized oriented dispositions in values, emotions, direction (disapproval of child euthanasia and behavior, because the other person’s is stronger among highly prosocial people),­ extreme suffering is neglected. Inversely, tol- some research indicates that, at least among erance of child euthanasia should denote a Iranian Muslims, agreeableness and self- capacity to empathize with others’ pain and reported altruism are positively associ- a strong endorsement of care-based moral- ated with the rejection of adult euthanasia ity. Indeed, though it has been argued that (Aghababei, 2014). Note that, however, in conservatives value both “binding”, collec- this study, this was also the case for con- tivistic morality and interpersonal morality, scientiousness, which suggests that the including the moral foundations of care and associations of both big five factors with fairness (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009), opposition to euthanasia may reflect social Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia 63 conformity- see Bègue et al., 2015- rather the disapproval of child euthanasia to be than necessarily empathy, compassion, and negatively related to openness to experience. valuing no harm. Another argument in favor However, it can be argued that the disap- of a positive link between prosocial disposi- proval of child euthanasia does not result tions and the disapproval of child euthanasia from a generalized low flexibility across all may be that strong concerns for children’s life domains, but from low flexibility in a capacity for discernment, typical among the specific domain, that is, existential questions skeptics (Bovens, 2015; Siegel et al., 2014), and worldviews. For instance, some research and subsequent fears of possible abuses of has shown that religious fundamentalists legalization (Karlsson, Strang, & Milberg, are not necessarily low in integrative com- 2007), reflect high perspective taking, an plexity of thought in general, but only with important component of empathy. respect to the religious/existential and the moral domains (Pancer, Jackson, Hunsberger, Socio-Cognitive Characteristics: Low Pratt, & Lea, 1995). Similarly, Conway et al. Flexibility (2016) recently found that conservatives are Beyond religious ideological, moral, and not necessarily simple-minded across all life emotional factors, the opposition to child domains: while liberals show higher com- euthanasia may be based on socio-cognitive plexity in certain domains (e.g., premarital factors indicating low cognitive flexibility. It sexuality, alcohol consumption, abortion), can be expected that such opposition reflects conservatives demonstrate higher complex- (1) a low openness to novelty and complex- ity in other domains (e.g., death penalty, ity, and thus low autonomy in thought, (2) open-door immigration, loud music). We a low relativism and readiness to question investigated this question by focusing on the one’s own beliefs and values, independently construct of existential quest. In line with the of their content, and (3) a strong tendency concept of religious quest, as elaborated by to use excessively generalized risk-based Batson et al. (1993), researchers have recently ­rhetoric. We investigated the above expec- defined existential quest as the readiness to tations through the use of three constructs, question one’s own beliefs and worldviews, respectively, openness to experience, exis- and to do so independently of their content, tential quest, and slippery slope thinking be it religious or not (Van Pachterbeke et al., style. Below we detail the rationale for our 2012). We thus hypothesized that opponents hypothesis for each of the three constructs. of child euthanasia would show low levels of Accepting the legalization of child existential quest, which was indeed found ­euthanasia is something both new and to be the case for adult euthanasia (Deak & complex: It requires acceptance of excep- Saroglou, 2015). tions to the “do not kill” rule and a more Finally, we expected the disapproval of nuanced understanding of this moral imper- child euthanasia to be strongly related to ative as not being exclusively conceivable the endorsement of a slippery slope think- in “black-and-white”­ terms. Initial evidence ing style. In general, slippery slope argu- shows that, at least among Iranian Muslims, ments are those where a premise, such as an opposition to euthanasia is related, among action or a reform, must be rejected as bad other things, to low openness to experience (or accepted as good) on the basis of simi- (Aghababei, 2014). Similarly, greater accept- lar cases with further consequences that are ance of euthanasia in European ­countries much more ­negative (or positive) (Jefferson, is associated with the belief in the right 2014; Walton, 2015). In the moral and social to self-determination (Cohen et al., 2006) domains, these kinds of arguments are typi- and the value placed on autonomy, at cally used to oppose a proposed reform by both the individual and the national levels linking it to a chain of disastrous conse- (Verbakel & Jaspers, 2010). We thus expected quences alleged to inevitably follow the 64 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia passage of a reform: “if we do A, at some dispositions: empathy, ­interpersonal moral point, the highly undesirable­ B will fol- principles, and behavioral intentional low” (Jefferson, 2014, p. 672). In ­particular, generosity). with regard to ­euthanasia, it is often feared that the acceptance of voluntary adult Method ­euthanasia for highly serious reasons Participants will gradually extend to an acceptance of The study was advertised on online social and euthanasia of networks as an investigation of opinions minors, as well as ­euthanasia for less seri- and attitudes concerning child euthanasia. ous reasons, or even for eugenic motives Data were collected in the spring of 2015, (see Jones, 2011; Verbakel & Jaspers, 2010). months after the Belgian euthanasia law was Psychological research has started to identify extended to minors. In total, 218 participants the mechanisms­ that contribute to the per- completed the questionnaire. Five people ceived strength of slippery slope arguments, were removed from the analyses based on such as a high similarity perceived between their answers to “catch-items” of the Moral the beginning and the end of a slippery slope Foundation Questionnaire (Graham et al., argument (Corner, Hahn, & Oaksford, 2011) 2011) that detect those who are not really or the use of inflammatory language induc- paying attention to the survey. The final ing anger (Quraishi et al., 2014). sample contained 213 young adults and Slippery slope arguments are typically older adults (83% women) aged 17 to 75 questionable at both the logical and empiri- years (M = 25.5; SD = 10.3). The majority cal levels for being, for instance, excessive, (79%) was Belgian, 14.2% were French, and illogical or unrealistic. Note though that they the remaining participants were from other are not necessarily, always, or totally falla- Western countries. (The non-Belgian partici- cious. In fact, they must be evaluated on a pants did not differ from the Belgian partici- case-by-case basis, both logically and empiri- pants on any of the measures.) Participants cally, especially within the context of social were mostly university students (76.4%). developments (Jefferson, 2014; Walton, Concerning religious affiliation, the sample 2015). For instance, conservative fears that consisted of 40% atheists or agnostics, 36% adult euthanasia would extend to minors Christians, 7% Jews, 5% Muslims, and 1% turned out to be true; and similar fears that Protestants; the remaining participants had the acceptance of gay and lesbian civil unions “other” affiliations. would progressively lead to an acceptance of gay and lesbian adoption also turned to be Measures true in several countries. Disapproval of child euthanasia At the beginning of the questionnaire, infor- Summary of the Hypotheses mation was provided on the Belgian legis- In summary, we expected the disapproval of lation of child euthanasia and the specific child euthanasia in Belgium – a Western secu- conditions in which euthanasia is legally per- larized country of Catholic tradition – where mitted. Participants read the following text: this practice has been legalized and is rather “On the 3rd of March 2014, Belgium became socially accepted but still debated, to be the first country to legalize euthanasia with- related to, and predicted (in a regression) by, out age limit for children and adolescents. ideological factors (religious beliefs and col- The latter, for euthanasia to be allowed, lectivistic moral orientation), socio-cognitive should have an incurable disease and face tendencies (low openness to experience and unbearable suffering. Before the decision, existential quest, and a high use of slippery a psychologist must ensure the child’s slope thinking style), and other-oriented capacity for discernment, that is to say, emotions and motives (high or low prosocial his/her ability to fully comprehend all the Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia 65 consequences of the decision. According to Spontaneous generosity. Participants the parliamentarians,­ the law on ­euthanasia were asked to indicate what they would do does not substitute . In all cases, if they won 100,000 Euros, specifying each a discussion among healthcare ­professionals, expenditure and the percentage of money as well as parental consent, is mandatory”. they would allocate to each (see Clobert & The degree of endorsement of, versus Saroglou, 2013). The total percentage of opposition to, the legalization of child money participants spontaneously allocated euthanasia was assessed with the following to others (e.g., family, friends, and chari- item: “How much do you agree with the legal- ties) instead of themselves was coded as a ization of child euthanasia?” Additionally, score of spontaneous prosocial behavioral the degree of endorsement of, versus opposi- intention. tion to, the specific legal conditions for child Existential quest. The Existential Quest euthanasia to be permitted was assessed by Scale (Van Pachterbeke, Keller, & Saroglou, the following item: “How much do you agree 2012) assesses flexibility in existential beliefs with the legal conditions under which child and worldviews, that is, valuing doubt and euthanasia is practiced?” Respondents rated being open to questioning and changing each item on a 6-point Likert scale ranging one’s own existential beliefs and worldviews. from 1 = not at all agree to 6 = totally agree. Sample items are: “Being able to doubt one’s For both items, scores were reversed so that convictions and reappraise them is a good higher scores denote opposition. Afterwards, quality” and “I know perfectly well what the participants were asked to give an argument goal of my life is (reverse)”. This is a 9-item to justify their response to the first question.­ measure with 7-point Likert scales, but we These spontaneous responses (some par- did not include two items referring specifi- ticipants provided one argument, some pro- cally to religion to avoid any overlap with vided more than one) were coded by two attitudes regarding religion (α = .66). independent coders to form broad categories Openness to experience. We used the 10 of the type of arguments used. items of the openness to experience subscale of the Big Five Inventory (John, Naumann, & Hypothesized correlates Soto, 2008; e.g., “you are creative and have Slippery slope thinking. We created four a lot of original ideas”) with response scale items to measure the belief that the mis- ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much) use of euthanasia, legalized for exceptional (α = 74). cases, would lead to a generalized practice Moral foundations. The 20-item Moral and the trivialization of the act of terminat- Foundations Questionnaire-short version ing a life: “Do you think that the legaliza- (Graham et al., 2011; our French transla- tion of child euthanasia crossed a line, and tion) was administered. This questionnaire that it will be misused?”; “Do you have the measures the endorsement of the five moral impression that this decision was made foundations, that is, care, fairness, loyalty, because of economic reasons rather than to authority, and purity (6-point Likert scales). prevent suffering?”; “Do you think it is pos- Following previous work (e.g., Napier & sible that, following this child euthanasia­ Luguri, 2013), we also combined the care law, there will be other laws concerning and the fairness items into a single variable euthanasia of the handicapped, people with that we called “interpersonal morality”, and dementia, the elderly, prisoners, …?”; “Do the items of authority, loyalty, and purity you think that this law on child euthanasia into a single score representing “collectivistic is just a step further in the growing disre- morality” (respective αs = .53 and .79). spect for human life?” Participants had to Empathy. Eight items, four for empathetic rate their answers on 7-point Likert scales concern and four for perspective taking, were (α = .83). selected from the Interpersonal Reactivity 66 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia

Index (Davis, 1983) to keep the questionnaire Results within a reasonable length (5-point Likert Means and standard deviations of the scales were used). A global score of empathy ­disapproval of child euthanasia and the disap- was computed by averaging the scores of the proval of the related legal conditions, as well eight items (α = .67). as of the hypothesized correlates are detailed Religiosity and spirituality. We adminis- in Table 1. On the basis of scores ≤ 3 on the tered three items measuring the importance 6-point Likert scale, it turned out that 71.8% of God in life, the importance of religion in of participants accepted, in various degrees, life, and the frequency of prayer (a typical child euthanasia and 79.3% found the legal index of religiosity), and one item measuring conditions acceptable. These two opinions the importance of spirituality in life (7-point were highly interrelated, r = .75, p < .001. Likert scales, except for the frequency of prayer In the open-ended question asking for a rated on a 5-point Likert scale). Given the justification of their approval or disapproval high intercorrelation between religiosity and of child euthanasia, participants sponta- spirituality, and that separately the two neously used various types of ­arguments. indexes did not provide additional informa- Half of the 213 participants approved of tion, we combined the four items into one child euthanasia for a prosocial motive, construct that we labeled “religiousness” that is, to end the severe suffering of seri- (α = .94).2 ously ill children.­ More than one fourth

Disapproval of Variables M SD Child Legal Euthanasia Conditions Disapproval of Child euthanasia 2.74 1.51 – – Legal conditions 2.55 1.62 – – Interpersonal morality 4.63 0.55 –.11 –.07 Care 4.38 0.71 –.08 –.09 Fairness 4.88 0.65 –.10 –.03 Collectivistic morality 3.40 0.72 .19** .16* Loyalty 3.29 0.77 .19** .17* Authority 3.43 0.87 –.01 –.06 Purity 3.47 1.03 .27*** .26*** Slippery slope thinking 3.02 1.45 .71*** .76*** Existential quest 5.04 0.90 –.25*** –.22** Openness to experience 3.56 0.59 .03 .03 Religiousness 2.74 1.94 .52*** .56*** Empathy 4.14 0.55 .10 .03 Spontaneous generosity (%) 23.78 26.83 .29** .26** Table 1: Means and Standard Deviations of All Measures and Coefficients of Correlations Between the Target Variables and the Hypothesized Correlates. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia 67 of participants (28%) used a principlistic Moreover, both indicators of disapproval were ­justification for their endorsement­ of child strongly and positively related to slippery slope euthanasia by defending the right to choose thinking as well as to religiousness. Finally, one’s own death. One fifth of participants both the disapproval of child ­euthanasia and accepted child euthanasia­ only if the legal the disapproval of its legal conditions were conditions are respected (20%). Finally, the negatively related to existential quest and posi- justification most commonly used against tively related to spontaneous generosity. child ­euthanasia was doubt regarding a Several of the hypothesized explanatory indi- child’s capacity to understand what death vidual differences were inter-correlated (see means and to make such an important deci- Table 2). Empathy was meaningfully related sion (16% of the total number of partici- (positively) to interpersonal morality and exis- pants). This justification was followed by the tential quest, but not to collectivistic morality opposition to legal “killing” (5%) and slip- and religiosity. On the contrary, spontaneous pery slope-like arguments (1.9%). generosity was unrelated to empathy, existen- We carried out correlational analyses on the tial quest, and interpersonal morality, but was relationships between the disapproval of child positively related to collectivistic morality and euthanasia and its legal conditions and the religiosity. Slippery slope thinking was found individual differences variables (see Table 1). to be in contrast with existential quest: the two Disapproval of both child ­euthanasia in gen- were negatively interrelated and were, respec- eral, and its legal conditions in particular, were tively, positively versus negatively associated positively related to collectivistic morality, with religiosity and its moral correlate, that particularly to the endorsement of the moral is, collectivistic morality. Finally, openness to foundations of loyalty and purity. No relation- experience was characteristic of those who did ship was found between either care or fairness not value authority as a moral foundation. (interpersonal morality) and the disapproval Given the above-mentioned inter-correla- of child euthanasia or its legal conditions. tions, it could be that some of these variables

Collect. Slippery Exist. Open- Religi- Empathy Spont. morality slope th. quest ness ousness generos. Interper. morality .10 –.11 .09 .02 –.12† .30*** .10 Care –.11 .04 .03 –.09 .22** .08 Fairness –.07 .11 .00 –.12† .27*** .08 Collect. morality .19** –.13† –.07 .31*** –.07 .25*** Loyalty .15* –.12† .08 .31*** –.01 .20** Authority –.01 –.08 –.21** .11 –.12† .08 Purity .30*** –.12† –.03 .32*** –.04 .32** Slippery slope think. –.18** .05 .53*** .01 .25*** Existential quest .09 –.14* .26*** –.08 Openness .07 .10 .06 Religiousness .02 .22** Empathy .08 Table 2: Correlations between Hypothesized Predictors of Disapproval of Child euthanasia and Legal Conditions. †p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. 68 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia share common variance in predicting, in a overlap with religiosity. Finally, gender and regression, the disapproval of child ­euthanasia. age did not impact the results. Overall, 35% For instance, religiousness and collectivistic of the variance was explained. morality have much in common, both theo- retically and empirically, and thus it is unclear Discussion whether opposition to child euthanasia is Euthanasia of minors is an emerging issue of predicted distinctly and additively by each of social and moral debate in Western countries,­ the two, or whether one of the two may be a with Belgium being the first country in the confound. Moreover, it is of interest to exam- world to legally allow, since 2014, volun- ine whether individual differences in the (dis) tary euthanasia of minors without any age approval of child euthanasia are exclusively limit. The present study, using a convenience due to the religious and moral ideologies adult sample, confirmed that the accept- people endorse, or also originate from subtler ance of child euthanasia in Belgium is high ­psychological, emotional and cognitive factors. (72%; mean disapproval = 2.74 on a 6-point To clarify these issues, we conducted Likert scale). This acceptance rate is similar a hierarchical regression analysis of the to that found in a survey conducted prior disapproval of child euthanasia on the to the law’s vote (74%, 2,714 Belgian adults; ­hypothesized correlates and predictors. In Laporte, 2013). However, it is slightly lower Step 1, religiousness was entered as a unique than that of adult euthanasia (86%) found in predictor, given past research indicating its a previous study in the same country (mean predominant role. In Step 2, collectivistic disapproval = 2.54 on the same 6-point scale; morality (loyalty combined with purity) was Deak & Saroglou, 2015; gender balanced added, to check for the religiosity-conserva- sample), a difference very likely due to the tive morality overlap. In Step 3, all other indi- child’s age status. The two major arguments vidual differences variables were additionally spontaneously provided by participants to entered: interpersonal morality (care and defend child euthanasia, that is, avoiding fairness), empathy, and spontaneous gener- suffering (50%) and the right to choose one’s osity, as well as openness to experience and own moment of death (28%), were the same existential quest. Slippery slope thinking was leading justifications in a previous study in not included because it was conceptually Belgium on adult euthanasia (Roelands et al., and empirically (r = .71) too proximal to the 2015). Nevertheless, in that study, the two disapproval of child euthanasia. At the final justifications were present in reverse propor- Step 4, age and gender were entered to con- tions, respectively 30% and 63%, likely due trol for their possible role as moderators or to the age of the target. confounds. All VIFs were ≤ 1.3, indicating no risk of multicollinearity. The Predominant Role of Religiousness The results (see Table 3) showed that reli- Importantly, from an individual differ- giousness was the strongest predictor of the ences perspective, though the acceptabil- disapproval of child euthanasia. Although ity of child euthanasia was high, there was slightly decreased after the inclusion of col- still a significant inter-individual variability. lectivistic morality and other variables, the Religiousness was the strongest correlate of effect of religiousness, remained the most negative attitudes toward child euthanasia. important. Beyond this effect, the disap- The effect (r = –.52) was similar in size to the proval of child euthanasia was additionally association between religiousness and the and distinctly predicted by high spontaneous disapproval of adult euthanasia in a previous generosity, high empathy, and low existential study in the same country (r = –.53; Deak & quest. Collectivistic morality was no longer Saroglou, 2015). Importantly, religiousness a significant predictor, very likely due to its remained a distinct and, at the same time Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia 69

β SE b* t-test 95% CI Step 1 Religiousness 0.40 0.05 .51 8.46*** [.30, .49] R2 = .26 Step 2 Religiousness 0.36 0.05 .47 7.27*** [.27, .46] Collectivistic morality 0.21 0.13 .11 1.67 [–.04, .45] R2 = .27; ∆2 = .01 Step 3 Religiousness 0.32 0.05 .41 6.37*** [.22, .41] Collectivistic morality 0.12 0.13 .06 0.96 [–.13, .37] Openness to experience –0.01 0.15 –.00 –0.03 [–.30, .29] Existential quest –0.31 0.10 –.19 –3.08** [–.51, –.11] Empathy 0.44 0.17 .16 2.58** [.10, .78] Spontaneous generosity 0.09 0.00 .17 2.83** [.01, .02] Interpersonal morality –0.29 0.17 –.11 –1.72 [–.62, .04] R2 = .35; ∆2 = .08 Step 4 Religiousness 0.32 0.05 .40 6.24*** [.22, .41] Collectivistic morality 0.12 0.13 .06 0.95 [–.13, .37] Openness to experience –0.03 0.15 –.01 –0.21 [–.33, .27] Existential quest –0.30 0.10 –.18 –2.93** [–.50, –.10] Empathy 0.47 0.18 .17 2.71** [.13, .82] Spontaneous generosity 0.01 0.00 .17 2.81** [.01, .02] Interpersonal morality –0.27 0.17 –.10 –1.59 [–.61, .07] Gender –0.21 0.25 –.05 –0.83 [–.69, .28] Age 0.00 0.01 .02 0.29 [–.02, .02] R2 = .35; ∆2 = .00 Table 3: Hierarchical Regression of Disapproval of Child Euthanasia on the Hypothesized Predictors. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Notes. Dfs = 208. Collectivistic morality includes loyalty and purity. the strongest, predictor of the disapproval religiousness), as well as the tendency for of child euthanasia, even after taking into low flexibility in worldviews. These results account, in the hierarchical multiple regres- confirm the key role of religion in the moral sion, the preferences for collectivistic moral- opposition to several domains of modern ity and ingroup prosociality (both related to moral liberalization (e.g., divorce, abortion, 70 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia gay marriage and adoption). Of interest to generalize to any domain, but is specifically note is that the role of religiousness in moral located within the ideological, existential- opposition has also been attested at the col- moral domain. Indeed, openness to expe- lective, country, level, with secularization rience, a broad personality factor, did not importantly diminishing society’s opposition relate to high or low acceptance; it was exis- to these issues (e.g., Cohen et al., 2014, and tential quest that predicted low opposition. Danilyv & O’Neill, 2015, for euthanasia; van This extends previous research showing that den Akker, van der Ploeg, & Scheepers, 2013, conservatives’ low flexibility does not gen- for homosexuality-related issues). eralize across all domains, but is specific to The distinct effect of religiousness, beyond certain ones (Conway et al., 2015; Pancer et that of moral foundations, can also be under- al., 1995). stood in terms of partial independence These results importantly replicate and between moral conviction and religious con- extend, from adult to child euthanasia, the viction, beyond their strong links (Skitka, results of the above-mentioned previous Bauman, & Lytle, 2009). Other research has Belgian study on the psychological cor- also shown that religious ideologies function relates and predictors of attitudes toward as sources of beliefs that influence moral judg- adult euthanasia (Deak & Saroglou, 2015). ment, and this independently from, or even in However, additional findings of the present opposition with, what humans consider to be study depart from those of the previous study universally moral (Turiel & Neff, 2000). This which was focused on similar hypothetical mainly concerns here the belief in God as the correlates of adult euthanasia in Belgium unique creator, and thus as the unique legiti- (Deak & Saroglou, 2015). In that study, proso- mate terminator of human life. The partial cial dispositions were found to be generally independence between religion and morality irrelevant in explaining the tolerance or dis- can also be seen from another perspective: approval of adult euthanasia. On the con- religious people may think morally independ- trary, in the present work focused on child ent from their religion. Indeed, an inspection, euthanasia, both empathy (in the regression) in the present data, of the answers provided and spontaneous generosity (in both the cor- by those affiliated with a religion showed that relational and regression analyses) turn out 37% (most often Jews, but also 25% of the to be associated with the disapproval of child Catholics) approved the legalization of child euthanasia. These findings provide initial evi- euthanasia (see also Roelands et al., 2015, for dence in favor of only one of the two possi- similar trends regarding adult euthanasia). ble directions developed in the introduction: People with prosocial dispositions – at least, The Additional Role of Moral, Cognitive, in terms of emotional and behavioral incli- and Emotional Factors nations – tend to disapprove the legalization In addition to the role of religiousness, other of euthanasia (possibly because of the horror results underlined the role of moral (in cor- of prescribing death to an “innocent” minor relations, not in the regression), as well even if for good reasons). The present results as cognitive and emotional factors. Those suggest that the alternative hypothesis, that who disapproved, compared to those who they should endorse it in order to alleviate accepted the legalization of child euthanasia,­ extreme suffering, may be wrong. tended to be lower in socio-cognitive flexibil- The fact that these findings are specific to ity on existential issues (existential quest) child euthanasia and do not seem to extend and higher in collectivistic moral orienta- to adult euthanasia precludes the inter- tion (loyalty and purity). Interpersonal moral pretation that prosocial people are against values (care and justice) were unrelated to euthanasia in general, and this for altruistic, moral opposition. The findings suggest that other-oriented, motives. It is the minor status the lower flexibility involved here may not of the target that makes, in the case of child Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia 71 euthanasia, the moral conflict between “do euthanasia in Belgium, it could be that the not kill” and “do not let the other suffering” role of respect for authority as a moral foun- even stronger, allowing prosocial concerns dation is unclear here: does this value lead to to play their role. One meaningful inter- respect of the current norm (permissiveness) pretation is that the present findings may or to attachment to the traditional norm highlight the role of some past evolutionary, (prohibition)? ingroup, prosocial motives for the preservation Finally, the slippery slope argumentation of offspring, kinship, and the human species. style turned out to be very highly correlated Interestingly, in the task we used to measure with the disapproval of euthanasia; and the participants’ inclination to generosity, the two shared the same correlates. This under- beneficiaries spontaneously mentioned were lines the decisive role of this thinking style mostly proximal people and ingroup mem- in shaping the non-acceptance of (child) bers (family, friends) and not charity organi- ­euthanasia—or, alternatively, in a posteriori zations and unknown people in need. In legitimizing moral opposition. To some addition, similar to what has been previously extent, these slippery slope arguments, even reported (Deak & Saroglou, 2015), a strong if excessive, may be realistic. For instance, the willingness to share hypothetical gains with legalization of adult euthanasia in Belgium, (proximal) others was, in the present study, where this study was conducted, was indeed related only to collectivistic, but not inter- followed, some years later, by the legaliza- personal morality, as well as to religiousness. tion of child euthanasia. Future research Religiousness is known to typically relate should investigate whether moral opposi- to both high reproduction (Pew Research tion is based not only on slippery slope argu- Center, 2015; Rowthorn, 2011) and a high ments that are specific to the moral issue level of prosociality mainly toward ingroup under study (i.e., child euthanasia), but also members (Galen, 2012; Saroglou, 2006). In on a more general style of slippery slope rea- sum, prosocial inclinations predicting oppo- soning that is content-free. sition to child euthanasia can be better inter- preted as indicating ingroup preservation Limitations and Future Directions motives rather than other-oriented concerns. The sample was predominantly female. It Also of interest to note is that it is unclear can thus not be guaranteed that the find- why, out of the three collectivistic moral ings would apply equally to men, particularly foundations, loyalty and purity, but not with respect to the results on empathy and authority, were related to the disapproval of generosity. However, given that most of the child euthanasia. This is, however, consistent results were very similar to those of a previ- with previous research on adult euthanasia ous study on adult euthanasia in the same (Deak & Saroglou, 2015; Koleva et al., 2012). country, where the gender was well-balanced Loyalty represents the moral imperative of and did not impact the results (Deak & reciprocity within the group and the soci- Saroglou, 2015), there are no strong reasons ety, thus making euthanasia, be it in child- to doubt the overall generalizability of the hood or late adulthood, appear as a kind of present findings across genders. Also, only treachery or as a non-assumption of one’s two items were used to measure the disap- own responsibilities. Purity represents the proval of the legalization of child ­euthanasia. need for (literal) respect and preservation of Nevertheless, given the theoretical and the physical and sacred order of things; thus, empirical continuity of the present findings any intervention to terminate an individu- with previous research, the results can be al’s life is prohibited. Respect for authority viewed with confidence. may be less relevant for understanding the Finally, an idea for future research could (dis)approval of euthanasia. Alternatively, be to examine the importance of just-world given the high acceptance rate of (child) beliefs in causing a lower acceptability 72 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia of child, compared to adult, euthanasia. ­present study, death anxiety turned out Contrary to seriously ill persons of old-age to be entirely unrelated to the disap- who are naturally closer to death, children proval of child euthanasia and thus will and adolescents have not yet fully experi- not be further discussed. enced life. The option to terminate their life may thus seem to be more unfair and unjust, Competing Interests in turn making the decision more difficult The authors have no competing interests to to make. This is not only because someone declare. will die (high tribute) without having lived ­his/her life (no benefit), but also because References minors may be perceived as too innocent Aghababaei, N. (2014). Attitudes towards to experience a death they do not deserve. euthanasia in Iran: The role of altruism. Given well-established empirical links Journal of Medical Ethics, 40, 173–176. between religion and just-world beliefs (Jost DOI: https://doi.org/10.1136/medeth- et al., 2014), this kind of belief, typical of the ics-2012-101004 theodicy problem, may also significantly fuel Bègue, L., Beauvois, J.-L., Courbet, D., religious opposition to child euthanasia. Oberlé, D., Lepage, J. & Duke, A. A. (2015). Personality predicts obedience Notes in a Milgram paradigm. Journal of Per- 1 Regarding religious moral opposition, sonality, 83, 299–306. DOI: https://doi. another interesting comparison is that org/10.1111/jopy.12104 between abortion and child euthanasia. Bovens, L. (2015). Child euthanasia: Should In both cases, an important concern, from we just not talk about it? Journal­ of a religious perspective, is to not inter- Medical Ethics, 41, 630–634. DOI: vene in God’s power regarding human https://doi.org/10.1136/medeth- life, especially that of an “innocent” non- ics-2014-102329 adult, the fetus also being considered a Bryan, J. W. & Freed, F. W. (1993). Abortion human person. However, abortion dif- research: Attitudes, sexual behavior, and fers from child euthanasia. The religious problems in a community college popu- opposition to the former may reflect, at lation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, least indirectly, some additional punitive 22, 1–22. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/ tendency toward sexually liberal women BF01537900 when perceived as unwilling to assume Clobert, M. & Saroglou, V. (2013). their responsibilities with regard to the ­Intercultural non-conscious influences: consequences of their sexual acts (see, Prosocial effects of Buddhist priming on e.g., Bryan & Freed, 1993). ­Westerners of Christian tradition. Inter- 2 We additionally included a measure of national Journal of Intercultural Rela- death anxiety (Templer, 1970). Since the tions, 37, 459–466. DOI: https://doi. termination of a child’s life is a particu- org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2012.10.001 larly disturbing event implying stronger Cohen, J., Marcoux, I., Bilsen, J., and more complex grieving processes, Deboosere, P., van der Wal, G. & we investigated whether people with Deliens, L. (2006). European public high death anxiety may tend to be acceptance of euthanasia: Socio-demo- reluctant toward child euthanasia. For graphic and cultural factors associated instance, death reminders have been with the acceptance of euthanasia­ found to increase people’s attachment in 33 European countries. Social to the social group, family, and offspring ­Science and Medicine, 63, 743–756. (Echebarria Echabe & Saioa Perez, 2016; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socsci- Fritsche et al., 2007). However, in the med.2006.01.026 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia 73

Cohen, J., Van Landeghem, P., Carpentier, N. Fritsche, I., Jonas, E., Fischer, P., Koranyi, N., & Deliens, L. (2014). Public acceptance of Berger, N. & Fleischmann, B. (2007). euthanasia in Europe: A survey study in and the desire for off- 47 countries. International Journal of Pub- spring. Journal of Experimental Social lic Health, 59, 143–156. DOI: https://doi. Psychology­ , 43, 753–762. DOI: https:// org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.01.026 doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.003 Conway, L. G., III., Gornick, L. J., Houck, S. C., Galen, L. W. (2012). Does religious belief pro- Anderson, C., Stockert, J., Sessoms, D. & mote prosociality? A critical examination. McCue, K. (2016). Are conservatives really Psychological Bulletin, 138, 876–906. more simple-minded than liberals?: The DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028251 domain-specificity of complex thinking. Gill, R. (Ed.). (1998). Euthanasia and the Political Psychology, 37, 777–798. DOI: churches: Christian ethics in dialogue. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12304 London, UK: Cassell. Corner, A., Hahn, U. & Oaksford, M. (2011). Graham, J. & Haidt, J. (2010). Beyond beliefs: The psychological mechanism of the slip- Religions bind individuals into moral com- pery slope argument. Journal of Memory munities. Personality and Social Psychol- and Language, 64, 133–152. DOI: https:// ogy Review, 14, 140–150. DOI: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.10.002 doi.org/10.1177/1088868309353415 Danyliv, A. & O’Neill, C. (2015). Attitudes Graham, J., Haidt, J. & Nosek, B. (2009). towards legalising physician provided Liberals and conservatives use differ- euthanasia in Britain: The role of reli- ent sets of moral foundations. Journal gion over time. Social Science and Medi- of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, cine, 128, 52–56. DOI: https://doi. 1029–1046. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.12.030 org/10.1037/a0015141 Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., differences in empathy: Evidence for a Koleva, S. & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping multidimensional approach. Journal of the moral domain. Journal of Personal- Personality and Social Psychology, 44, ity and Social Psychology, 101, 366–385. 113–126. DOI: https://doi. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021847 org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113 Grell, O. P. & Cunningham, A. (Eds.). (2007). Deak, C. & Saroglou, V. (2015). Opposing Medicine and religion in Enlightenment abortion, gay adoption, euthanasia, and Europe. Hamphsire, England: Routledge. suicide: Compassionate openness or Haidt, J. & Graham, J. (2007). When moral- self-centered moral rigorism? Archive for ity opposes justice: Conservatives have the Psychology of Religion, 37, 267–294. moral intuitions that liberals may not DOI: https://doi.org/10.1163/15736121- recognize. Social Justice Research, 20, 12341309 98–116. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/ Deak, C. & Saroglou, V. (2016). Valu- s11211-007-0034-z ing care protects religiosity from the Hains, C-A. M. & Hulbert-Williams, N. J. antisocial consequences of imper- (2013). Attitudes toward euthanasia and sonal deontology. Journal of Empirical physician-: A study of the ­Theology, 29, 171–189. DOI: https://doi. multivariate effects of healthcare train- org/10.1163/15709256-12341339 ing, patient characteristics, religion Echebarria Echabe, A. & Saioa Perez, S. and locus of control. Journal of Medical (2016). The impact of different proce- Ethics, 39, 713–716. DOI: https://doi. dures to arouse mortality awareness on org/10.1136/medethics-2012-100729 various worldview dimensions. European Jaspers, E., Lubbers, M. & De Graaf, N. D. Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 392–399. (2007). ‘Horrors of Holland’: Explaining DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2144 attitude change towards euthanasia 74 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia

and homosexuals in the Netherlands, Laporte, C. (2013, October 2). Vous acceptez 1970–1998. International Journal of Pub- l’euthanasie des enfants [You accept lic Opinion Research, 19, 452–473. DOI: ­children’s euthanasia]. La Libre Belgique. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edm029 Retrieved from http://www.lalibre.be/ Jefferson, A. (2014). Slippery slope actu/belgique/vous-acceptez-l-eutha- arguments. Philosophy Compass, 9, nasie-des-enfants-524b967a35703e- 672–680. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/ ef3a0c3589. phc3.12161 Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Cohen, A. B. & John, O. P., Naumann, L. P. & Soto, C. J. Miller, D. T. (2011). Religiosity and social (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative welfare: Competing influences of cultural Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, meas- conservatism and prosocial value orienta- urement, and conceptual issues. In: John, tion. Journal of Personality, 79, 763–792. O. P., Robins, R. W. & Pervin, L. A., (Eds.), DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- Handbook of personality: Theory and 6494.2011.00705.x research (2nd ed., pp. 114–158). New York, McDougall, J. F. & Gorman, M. (Eds.). NY: Guilford Press. (2007). Euthanasia: A reference handbook Johnson, K. A., Hook, J. N., Davis, D. E., (2nd ed.). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC Clio. Van Tongeren, D. R., Sandage, S. J. Moulton, B. E., Hill, T. D. & Burdette, A. & Crabtre, S. A. (2016). Moral founda- (2006). Religion and trends in euthanasia tion priorities reflect U.S. Christians’ attitudes among U.S. adults, 1977–2004. individual differences in religiosity. Per- Sociological Forum, 21, 249–272. DOI: sonality and Individual Differences, 100, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11206-006- 56–61. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 9015-5 paid.2015.12.037 Neidig, J. R. & Dalgas-Pelish, P. (1991). Jones, D. A. (2011). Is there a logical slippery Parental grieving and perceptions regard- slope from voluntary to nonvoluntary ing health care professionals’ interven- euthanasia? Kennedy Institute of Ethics tions. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Journal, 21, 379–404. DOI: https://doi. Nursing, 14, 179–191. DOI: https://doi. org/10.1353/ken.2011.0018 org/10.3109/01460869109014497 Jost, J. T., Hawkins, C. B., Nosek, B. A., Pancer, S. M., Jackson, L. M., Hunsberger,­ B., Hennes, E. P., Stern, C., Gosling, S. D. & Pratt, M. & Lea, J. (1995). Religious ortho- Graham, J. (2014). Belief in a just God doxy and the complexity of thought about (and a just society): A system justifica- religious and nonreligious issues. Journal tion perspective on religious ideology. of Personality, 63, 213–232. DOI: https:// Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1995. Psychology, 34, 56–81. DOI: https://doi. tb00808.x org/10.1037/a0033220 Pew Research Center (2015). The future of Karlsson, M., Strang, P. & Milberg, A. world religions: Population growth projec- (2007). Attitudes toward euthanasia tions, 2010–2050. Retrieved from http:// among Swedish medical students. Pallia- www.pewforum.org/files/2015/03/ tive Medicine, 21, 615–622. DOI: https:// PF_15.04.02_ProjectionsFullReport.pdf. doi.org/10.1177/0269216307081940 Piazza, J. & Sousa, P. (2014). Religios- Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., ity, political orientation, and con- Ditto, P. H. & Haidt, J. (2012). Tracing sequentialist moral thinking. Social the threads: How five moral concerns Psychological and Personality ­Science, (especially purity) help explain culture 5, 334–342. DOI: https://doi. war attitudes. Journal of Research in Per- org/10.1177/1948550613492826 sonality, 46, 184–194. DOI: https://doi. Quraishi, S. & Oaksford, M. (2014). Emo- org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.006 tion as an argumentative strategy: How Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia 75

induced mood affects the evaluation religious convictions as constraints on def- of neutral and inflammatory slippery erence to authority. Journal of Personality­ slope arguments. In: Blanchette, I., (Ed.), and Social Psychology, 97, 567–578. DOI: Emotion and reasoning: Current issues https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015998 in thinking and reasoning (pp. 95–118). Stolz, E., Burkert, N., Großschädl, F., Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Rásky, E., Stronegger, W. J. & Freidl, W. Roelands, M., Van den Block, L., Geurts, S., (2015). Determinants of public attitudes Deliens, L. & Cohen, J. (2015). Attitudes of towards euthanasia in adults and phy- Belgian students of medicine, ­philosophy, sician-assisted death in neonates in and law toward euthanasia and the condi- Austria: A national survey. PLoS ONE, tions for its acceptance. Death Studies­ , 39, 10, Article e0124320. DOI: https://doi. 139–150. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/ org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124320 07481187.2014.920433 Templer, D. I. (1970). The construction and Rottman, J., Kelemen, D. & Young, L. validation of a death anxiety scale. Jour- (2014). Tainting the soul: Purity concerns nal of General Psychology, 82, 165–167. predict moral judgments of suicide. Cog- DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309 nition, 130, 217–226. DOI: https://doi. .1970.9920634 org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.007 Turiel, E. & Neff, K. (2000). Religion, cul- Rowthorn, R. (2011). Religion, fertility ture, and beliefs about reality in moral and genes: A dual inheritance model. reasoning. In: Rosengren, K. S., Johnson, Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 278, C. N. & Harris, P. L., (Eds.), Imagining the 2519–2527. DOI: https://doi. impossible: Magical, scientific, and reli- org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2504 gious thinking in children (pp. 269–304). Saroglou, V. (2006, Spring). Religion’s Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University role in prosocial behavior: Myth or Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/ reality? ­Psychology of Religion Newslet- CBO9780511571381.011 ter: ­American Psychological Association van den Akker, H., van der Ploeg, R. & ­Division 36, 31, 1–8. Scheepers, P. (2013). Disapproval of Saroglou, V. (2013). Religion, spirituality, homosexuality: Comparative research and altruism. In: Pargament, K. I., Exline, on individual and national determinants J. J. & Jones, J. W., (Eds.), APA Handbook of disapproval of homosexuality in 20 of psychology, religion and spirituality European­ countries. International Journal (Vol. 1, pp. 439–457). Washington, DC: of Public Opinion Research, 25, 64–86. DOI: American Psychological Association. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edr058 https://doi.org/10.1037/14045-024 Van Pachterbeke, M., Freyer, C. & Schmitt, D. P. & Fuller, R. C. (2015). On Saroglou, V. (2011). When authoritari- the varieties of sexual experience: Cross- anism meets religion: Sacrificing oth- cultural links between religiosity and ers in the name of abstract deontology. human mating strategies. Psychology of European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, Religion and Spirituality, 7, 314–326. DOI: 898–903. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/ https://doi.org/10.1037/rel0000036 ejsp.834 Siegel, A. M., Sisti, D. A. & Caplan, A. L. Van Pachterbeke, M., Keller, J. & (2014). Pediatric euthanasia in Belgium: Saroglou, V. (2012). Flexibility in existen- Disturbing developments. The Journal tial beliefs and worldviews: Introducing and of American Medical Association, 311, measuring existential quest. Journal of Indi- 1963–1964. DOI: https://doi. vidual Differences, 33, 2–16. DOI: https:// org/10.1001/jama.2014.4257 doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000056 Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W. & Lytle, B. L. Verbakel, E. & Jaspers, E. (2010). A com- (2009). Limits on legitimacy: Moral and parative study on permissiveness toward 76 Deak and Saroglou: Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia

euthanasia: Religiosity, slippery slope, ­mortality salience on faith-based medical autonomy, and death with dignity. Pub- refusals. Journal of Personality and Social lic Opinion Quarterly, 74, 109–139. DOI: ­Psychology, 97, 334–350. DOI: https:// https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfp074 doi.org/10.1037/a0015545 Vess, M., Arndt, J., Cox, C. R., Routledge, Walton, D. (2015). The basic slippery C. & Goldenberg, J. L. (2009). Exploring slope argument. Informal Logic, 35, the existential function of religion:­ The 273–311. DOI: https://doi.org/10.22329/ effect of religious fundamentalism and il.v35i3.4286

How to cite this article: Deak, C. and Saroglou, V. (2017). Terminating a Child’s Life? Religious, Moral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors Underlying Non-Acceptance of Child Euthanasia. Psychologica Belgica, 57(1), pp. 59–76, DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.341

Submitted: 20 April 2016 Accepted: 28 October 2016 Published: 26 April 2017

Copyright: © 2017 The Author(s). This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (CC-BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. See http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Psychologica Belgica is a peer-reviewed open access journal OPEN ACCESS published by Ubiquity Press.