The Psycholinguistics of Signed and Spoken Languages: How Biology Affects Processing Karen Emmorey

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Psycholinguistics of Signed and Spoken Languages: How Biology Affects Processing Karen Emmorey 43-Gaskell-Chap43 3/11/07 10:53 AM Page 703 CHAPTER 43 The psycholinguistics of signed and spoken languages: how biology affects processing Karen Emmorey inguistic research over the last few decades consists of dynamic and constantly changing has revealed substantial similarities forms rather than static symbols. Further, nei- Lbetween the structure of signed and spo- ther sign language nor spoken language comes ken languages (for reviews see Emmorey, 2002; pre-segmented into words and sentences for the Sandler and Lillo-Martin, 2006). These similari- perceiver. The production of writing, although ties provide a strong basis for cross-modality performed by the hand, differs substantially comparisons, and also bring to light linguistic from sign language production because writing universals that hold for all human languages. In derives its structure from a separate system (the addition, however, biology-based distinctions orthography of a spoken language). In contrast between sign and speech are important, and can to written language, sign and speech are both be exploited to discover how the input–output primary language systems, acquired during systems of language impact online language pro- infancy and early childhood without formal cessing and affect the neurocognitive underpin- instruction. nings of language comprehension and production. For example, do the distinct perceptual and pro- ductive systems of signed and spoken languages 43.1 Sign perception and exert differing constraints on the nature of lin- guistic processing? Recent investigations have visual processing suggested that the modality in which a language Although non-signers may interpret the visual is expressed can impact the psychological mech- signed signal simply as a collection of rapid hand anisms required to decode and produce the lin- and arm motions, signers quickly extract com- guistic signal. This chapter explores what aspects plex meaning from the incoming visual signal. of language processing appear to be universal to Similarly, speakers extract meaning from a rap- all human languages and what aspects are idly changing acoustic stream, if they know the affected by the particular characteristics of audi- language. Listeners and viewers are able to auto- tion vs. vision, or by the differing constraints on matically parse an incoming auditory or visual manual versus oral articulation. linguistic signal by virtue of stored internal rep- Sign language processing is appropriately resentations. Speech perception involves seg- compared to speech processing, rather than to mentation of speech sounds into phonemic units. reading, because unlike written text, which can be For signed languages, a first question is whether characterized as “visual language,”sign language signs actually exhibit sublexical linguistic structure 43-Gaskell-Chap43 3/11/07 10:53 AM Page 704 704 · CHAPTER 43 The psycholinguistics of signed and spoken languages that could be used by a parser to segment visual (Brentari, 1998; Corina and Sandler, 1993; Wilbur, signed input. Is it possible to have a phonology 1993). The syllable is a unit of structure that is that is not based on sound? below the level of the word but above the level of the segment, and is required to explain phono- 43.1.1 Phonology in a language logical form and patterning within a word. Although sign phonologists disagree about pre- without sound cisely how sign syllables should be characterized, Several decades of linguistic research has shown there is general agreement that a sign syllable that signed languages, like spoken languages, have must contain a movement of some type. In ASL, a level of structure in which meaningless elements several phonological constraints have been are combined in rule-governed ways to create identified that must refer to the syllable. For meaningful forms (e.g. Stokoe, 1960; Battison, example, only certain movement sequences are 1978; Sandler, 1986; Brentari, 1998). For spoken allowed in bisyllabic (two-movement) signs: languages, these elements are oral gestures that circle + straight movements are permitted, but create sounds. For signed languages, manual and straight + circle movements are not (Uyechi, facial gestural units are combined to create 1994). Although a straight + circle movement distinct signs. The discovery that sign languages sequence is ill-formed as a single sign, it is well- exhibit phonological structure was ground- formed when it occurs in a phrase. Thus, the con- breaking because it demonstrated that signs are straint on movement sequences needs to refer to not holistic pantomimes lacking internal organ- a level smaller than the word (the constraint does ization. Furthermore, this discovery showed that not hold across word boundaries), but larger human languages universally develop a level of than the segment. Within Sandler’s (1986) Hand meaningless linguistic structure and a system Tier model, signed segments consist of Movements that organizes this structure. and Locations, somewhat akin to Vowels and Briefly, signs are composed of three basic Consonants. phonological parameters: hand configuration, However, syllables in signed language differ location (place of articulation), and movement. from syllables in spoken language because there Orientation of the hand/arm is another con- is little evidence for internal structure within the trasting parameter, but many theories represent signed syllable. Spoken syllables can be divided orientation as a sub-component of hand config- into onsets (usually, the first consonant or con- uration or movement, rather than as a basic sonant cluster) and rhymes (the vowel and final phonological element. Figure 43.1 provides consonants). Such internal structure does not illustrations of minimal pairs from LIS (Lingua appear to be present for sign syllables, although Italiana dei Segni, Italian Sign Language). The some linguists have argued for weight distinc- top part of the figure illustrates two LIS signs tions, i.e. “heavy” vs. “light” syllables, based on that differ only in hand configuration. Not all differences in movement types. Because of the sign languages share the same hand configura- lack of internal structure, there do not appear to tion inventory. For example, the “t” hand config- be processes such as resyllabification in sign lan- uration in American Sign Language (the thumb guages (e.g. a segment from one syllable becomes is inserted between the index and middle fingers part of another syllable). These facts are impor- of a fist) is not found in European sign languages. tant, given the emphasis that speech production Chinese Sign Language contains a hand config- models place on syllabification as a separate uration formed with an open hand with all fin- processing stage (e.g. Levelt et al., 1999). gers extended except for the ring finger, which is Syllabification processes and/or the use of bent—this hand configuration does not occur a syllabary may be specific to phonological in American Sign Language (ASL). In addition, encoding for speech production. The syllable signs can differ according to where they are likely serves as an organizational unit for speech, made on the body or face. Figure 43.1B illus- providing a structural frame for multisegmental trates two LIS signs that differ only their place of words. For example, MacNeilage (1998) argues articulation, and these different locations do not that the oscillation of the mandible creates a add meaning to the signs. Signs can also differ frame around which syllable production can be minimally in orientation, as illustrated in Figure organized. Meier (2000; 2002) points out that 43.1C. Finally, movement is another contrasting signing differs dramatically from speaking category that distinguishes minimally between because signing does not involve a single, pre- signs, as shown in Figure 43.1D. dominant oscillator, akin to the mandible. Rather, In addition to segment-like units, syllables have signs can have movement that is restricted to just also been argued to exist in signed languages about any joint of the arm. Thus, sign production 43-Gaskell-Chap43 3/11/07 10:53 AM Page 705 Sign perception and visual processing · 705 (a) BICICLETTA (“bicycle”) CAMBIARE (“change”) (b) AFFITTO (“rent”) STIPENDIO (“salary”) (c) PAPA (“father”) UOMO (“man”) (d) FORMAGGIO (“cheese”) SASSO (“stone”) Figure 43.1 Examples of minimal pairs in Lingua Italiana dei Signi, LIS (Italian Sign Language) (A) signs that contrast in hand configuration; (B) signs that contrast in place of articulation (location); (C) signs that contrast in orientation; (D) signs that contrast in movement. Illustrations from V. Volterra (ed.), La lingua italiana dei segni. Bologna: Il Mulino, 1987 (new edn 2004) Copyright © Virginia Volterra and Elena Radutzky. Reprinted with permission. 43-Gaskell-Chap43 3/11/07 10:53 AM Page 706 706 · CHAPTER 43 The psycholinguistics of signed and spoken languages may not be constrained to fit within a frame place of articulation (Emmorey et al., 2003; Baker imposed by a single articulator. Further, multi- et al., 2005). This result is consistent with previ- segmental signs (e.g. signs with more than three ous studies which found that deaf and hearing segments) are relatively rare, regardless of how participants exhibit similar perceptual groupings signed segments are defined (see Brentari, 1998). and confusability matrices for hand configura- In contrast, syllabification processes for speech tion and for place of articulation (Lane et al., production may serve a critical framing
Recommended publications
  • Language Development Language Development
    Language Development rom their very first cries, human beings communicate with the world around them. Infants communicate through sounds (crying and cooing) and through body lan- guage (pointing and other gestures). However, sometime between 8 and 18 months Fof age, a major developmental milestone occurs when infants begin to use words to speak. Words are symbolic representations; that is, when a child says “table,” we understand that the word represents the object. Language can be defined as a system of symbols that is used to communicate. Although language is used to communicate with others, we may also talk to ourselves and use words in our thinking. The words we use can influence the way we think about and understand our experiences. After defining some basic aspects of language that we use throughout the chapter, we describe some of the theories that are used to explain the amazing process by which we Language9 A system of understand and produce language. We then look at the brain’s role in processing and pro- symbols that is used to ducing language. After a description of the stages of language development—from a baby’s communicate with others or first cries through the slang used by teenagers—we look at the topic of bilingualism. We in our thinking. examine how learning to speak more than one language affects a child’s language develop- ment and how our educational system is trying to accommodate the increasing number of bilingual children in the classroom. Finally, we end the chapter with information about disorders that can interfere with children’s language development.
    [Show full text]
  • Assessing the Bimodal Bilingual Language Skills of Young Deaf Children
    ANZCED/APCD Conference CHRISTCHURCH, NZ 7-10 July 2016 Assessing the bimodal bilingual language skills of young deaf children Elizabeth Levesque PhD What we’ll talk about today Bilingual First Language Acquisition Bimodal bilingualism Bimodal bilingual assessment Measuring parental input Assessment tools Bilingual First Language Acquisition Bilingual literature generally refers to children’s acquisition of two languages as simultaneous or sequential bilingualism (McLaughlin, 1978) Simultaneous: occurring when a child is exposed to both languages within the first three years of life (not be confused with simultaneous communication: speaking and signing at the same time) Sequential: occurs when the second language is acquired after the child’s first three years of life Routes to bilingualism for young children One parent-one language Mixed language use by each person One language used at home, the other at school Designated times, e.g. signing at bath and bed time Language mixing, blending (Lanza, 1992; Vihman & McLaughlin, 1982) Bimodal bilingualism Refers to the use of two language modalities: Vocal: speech Visual-gestural: sign, gesture, non-manual features (Emmorey, Borinstein, & Thompson, 2005) Equal proficiency in both languages across a range of contexts is uncommon Balanced bilingualism: attainment of reasonable competence in both languages to support effective communication with a range of interlocutors (Genesee & Nicoladis, 2006; Grosjean, 2008; Hakuta, 1990) Dispelling the myths….. Infants’ first signs are acquired earlier than first words No significant difference in the emergence of first signs and words - developmental milestones are met within similar timeframes (Johnston & Schembri, 2007) Slight sign language advantage at the one-word stage, perhaps due to features being more visible and contrastive than speech (Meier & Newport,1990) Another myth….
    [Show full text]
  • Theories of Language Acquisitionq Susan Goldin-Meadow, University of Chicago, Departments of Psychology and Comparative Human Development, Chicago, IL, USA
    Theories of Language Acquisitionq Susan Goldin-Meadow, University of Chicago, Departments of Psychology and Comparative Human Development, Chicago, IL, USA © 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Theoretical Accounts of Language-Learning 1 Behaviorist Accounts 1 Nativist Accounts 2 Social/Cognitive Accounts 3 Connectionist Accounts 3 Constrained Learning 4 Constrained Invention 5 Is Language Innate? 7 Innateness Defined as Genetic Encoding 7 Innateness Defined as Developmental Resilience 7 Language Is Not a Unitary Phenomenon 8 References 8 The simplest technique to study the process of language-learning is to do nothing more than watch and listen as children talk. In the earliest studies, researcher parents made diaries of their own child’s utterances (e.g., Stern and Stern, 1907; Leopold, 1939–1949). The diarist’s goal was to write down all of the new utterances that the child produced. Diary studies were later replaced by audio and video samples of talk from a number of children, usually over a period of years. The most famous of these modern studies is Roger Brown’s (1973) longitudinal recordings of Adam, Eve, and Sarah. Because transcribing and analyzing child talk is so labor-intensive, each individual language acquisition study typically focuses on a small number of children, often interacting with their primary caregiver at home. However, advances in computer technology have made it possible for researchers to share their transcripts of child talk via the computerized Child Language Data Exchange System (CHILDES, https://childes.talkbank.org). Because this system makes available many transcripts collected by different researchers, a single researcher can now call upon data collected from spontaneous interactions in naturally occurring situations across a wide range of languages, and thus test the robustness of descriptions based on a small sample.
    [Show full text]
  • What It Is and How to Teach It. Center for Applied Linguistics, W
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 433 722 FL 025 979 AUTHOR Burkart, Grace Stovall TITLE Spoken Language: What It Is and How To Teach It. INSTITUTION Center for Applied Linguistics, Washington, DC. SPONS AGENCY Center for International Education (ED), Washington, DC. PUB DATE 1998-12-00 NOTE 33p.; In its: Out of Class Learning Experiences and Students' Perceptions of Their Impact on English Conversation Skills; see FL 025 966. CONTRACT PO17A50050 -95A PUB TYPE Guides Classroom Teacher (052) EDRS PRICE MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Class Activities; Classroom Techniques; *College Instruction; Communicative Competence (Languages); Curriculum Design; Discourse Analysis; Graduate Students; Higher Education; *Interpersonal Communication; *Language Patterns; Language Teachers; *Oral Language; *Second Language Instruction; Skill Development; *Speech Skills; Teacher Education; Teaching Assistants IDENTIFIERS Turn Taking ABSTRACT An instructional module designed to help prepare college-level teaching assistants (TAs) for their duties in second language instruction is presented. The module focuses on the teaching of oral language skills in a beginning or intermediate language course. The first section examines features of spoken language, including the range of uses of spoken language, the concepts of communicative competence and communicative efficiency, distinctions between transactional and interactional talk, formulas in spoken language, and typical spoken language episodes. The second section describes specific approaches and classroom techniques for building language learners' speaking skills, including teaching spoken language for communication, finding a balance between control and spontaneity, outlining a curriculum, using class activities to bridge the gap between language knowledge and language use, and activities teaching both. Questions for classroom discussion are dispersed throughout the text. Contains 30 references.
    [Show full text]
  • Characteristics of Spoken and Written Communication in the Opening and Closing Sections of Instant Messaging
    Portland State University PDXScholar Dissertations and Theses Dissertations and Theses Fall 1-23-2014 Characteristics of Spoken and Written Communication in the Opening and Closing Sections of Instant Messaging Kenta Nishimaki Portland State University Follow this and additional works at: https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds Part of the Interpersonal and Small Group Communication Commons, and the Other Communication Commons Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Nishimaki, Kenta, "Characteristics of Spoken and Written Communication in the Opening and Closing Sections of Instant Messaging" (2014). Dissertations and Theses. Paper 1548. https://doi.org/10.15760/etd.1547 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of PDXScholar. Please contact us if we can make this document more accessible: [email protected]. Characteristics of Spoken and Written Communication in the Opening and Closing Sections of Instant Messaging by Kenta Nishimaki A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the Requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Japanese Thesis Committee: Suwako Watanabe, Chair Patricia J. Wetzel Emiko Konomi Portland State University 2013 ABSTRACT This study examines opening and closing segments in instant messaging (IM) and demonstrates how openings and closings differ between oral conversation and instant messaging as well as the factors that account for the difference. Many researchers have discussed the differences and similarities between spoken and written languages. Tannen (1980) claims that spoken and written languages are not distinct categories and there is a continuum between them.
    [Show full text]
  • Access to Assessments
    GUIDANCE FOR AWARDING BODIES Access to External Assessment for D/deaf Candidates Version 3 – December 2006 1 CONTENTS Page 1. Introduction 4 2. About the D/deaf Candidate 5 3. Making Reasonable Adjustments 7 4. Range of Reasonable Adjustments to the Assessment of D/deaf Candidates 8 A Changes to assessment conditions 8 A1 Extra time A2 Accommodation A3 Early opening of papers B Use of mechanical, electronic and technological aids 9 B1 Assistive technology B2 BSL/English dictionaries/glossaries C Modification to the presentation of assessment material 10 C1 Language modified assessment material a) Modified written paper b) Assessment material in British Sign Language C2 Modified or standard written paper with modification through ‘live’ presentation C3 Modified oral assessments a) Listening tests (language exam) and assessment material in audio format b) Listening tests (subjects other than languages) D Alternative ways of presenting responses 18 D1 Responses in British Sign Language to written questions D2 Responses in spoken English to written questions D3 Responses to oral assessments a) Speaking tests (language exam) b) Speaking/oral tests (subject exams other than languages) 2 E Use of access facilitators 20 E1 Reader E2 Scribe E3 Oral Language Modifier E4 BSL/English Interpreter E5 Transcriber E6 Lipspeakers, Note-takers, Speech to Text Reporters, Cued Speech Transliterators APPENDIX Summary of Reasonable Adjustments for D/deaf Candidates 31 Signature would like to thank everyone who contributed to this guidance, including members of the British Association of Teachers of the Deaf (BATOD), teachers of D/deaf students and other specialist staff at specific schools, colleges and universities.
    [Show full text]
  • How Infant Speech Perception Contributes to Language Acquisition Judit Gervain* and Janet F
    Language and Linguistics Compass 2/6 (2008): 1149–1170, 10.1111/j.1749-818x.2008.00089.x How Infant Speech Perception Contributes to Language Acquisition Judit Gervain* and Janet F. Werker University of British Columbia Abstract Perceiving the acoustic signal as a sequence of meaningful linguistic representations is a challenging task, which infants seem to accomplish effortlessly, despite the fact that they do not have a fully developed knowledge of language. The present article takes an integrative approach to infant speech perception, emphasizing how young learners’ perception of speech helps them acquire abstract structural properties of language. We introduce what is known about infants’ perception of language at birth. Then, we will discuss how perception develops during the first 2 years of life and describe some general perceptual mechanisms whose importance for speech perception and language acquisition has recently been established. To conclude, we discuss the implications of these empirical findings for language acquisition. 1. Introduction As part of our everyday life, we routinely interact with children and adults, women and men, as well as speakers using a dialect different from our own. We might talk to them face-to-face or on the phone, in a quiet room or on a busy street. Although the speech signal we receive in these situations can be physically very different (e.g., men have a lower-pitched voice than women or children), we usually have little difficulty understanding what our interlocutors say. Yet, this is no easy task, because the mapping from the acoustic signal to the sounds or words of a language is not straightforward.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 2 Sign Language Types
    Chapter 2 Sign language types This chapter defines four different sign language types, based on the infor- mation available in the respective sources. Before introducing the types of sign languages, I first report on the diachronic developments in the field of typological sign language research that gave rise to the distinction of the various sign language types. Sign language research started about five decades ago in the United States of America mainly due to the pioneering work of Stokoe (2005 [1960]), Klima and Bellugi (1979), and Poizner, Klima and Bellugi (1987) on American Sign Language (ASL). Gradually linguists in other countries, mainly in Europe, became interested in sign language research and started analyzing European sign languages e.g. British Sign Language (BSL), Swedish Sign Language (SSL), Sign Language of the Netherlands (NGT) and German Sign Language (DGS). Most of the in-depth linguistic descrip- tions have been based on Western sign languages. Therefore, it has long been assumed that some fundamental levels of linguistic structure, such as spatial morphology and syntax, operate identically in all sign languages. Recent studies, however, have discovered some important variations in spatial organization in some previously unknown sign languages (Washabaugh, 1986; Nyst, 2007; Marsaja, 2008; Padden, Meir, Aronoff, & Sandler, 2010). In the context of growing interest in non-Western sign languages towards the end of the 1990s and more recently, there have been efforts towards developing a typology of sign languages (Zeshan, 2004ab, 2008, 2011b; Schuit, Baker, & Pfau, 2011). Although it has been repeatedly emphasized in the literature that the sign language research still has too little data on sign languages other than those of national deaf communities, based in Western or Asian cultures (Zeshan, 2008).
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Origins of Language
    Cambridge University Press 978-1-108-49945-3 — The Study of Language George Yule Excerpt More Information 1 The Origins of Language The first person to set foot on the continent of Australia was a woman named Warramurrungunji. She emerged from the sea onto an island off northern Australia, and then headed inland, creating children and putting each one in a specific place. As she moved across the landscape, Warramurrungunji told each child, “I am putting you here. This is the language you should talk! This is your language!” Erard (2016) This origin story from the Iwaidja people of Australia, illustrated in the painting above, offers an explanation of not only where language came from, but also why there are so many different languages. Among the English-speaking people, there have been multiple attempts to provide a comparable explanation, but not much proof to support any of them. Instead of a belief in a single mythical earth mother, we have a variety of possible beliefs, all fairly speculative. We simply don’t have a definitive answer to the question of how language originated. We do know that the ability to produce sound and simple vocal patterning (a hum versus a grunt, for example) appears to be in an ancient part of the brain that we share with all vertebrates, including fish, frogs, birds and other mammals. But that isn’t human language. We suspect that some type of spoken language must have developed between 100,000 and 50,000 years ago, well before written language (about 5,000 years ago).
    [Show full text]
  • Linguistic Access to Education for Deaf Pupils and Students in Scotland
    BRITISH SIGN LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTIC ACCESS WORKING GROUP SCOPING STUDY: LINGUISTIC ACCESS TO EDUCATION FOR DEAF PUPILS AND STUDENTS IN SCOTLAND © Crown copyright 2009 ISBN: 978-0-7559-5849-8 (Web only) This document is also available on the Scottish Government website: www.scotland.gov.uk RR Donnelley B56660 02/09 www.scotland.gov.uk BRITISH SIGN LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTIC ACCESS WORKING GROUP SCOPING STUDY: LINGUISTIC ACCESS TO EDUCATION FOR DEAF PUPILS AND STUDENTS IN SCOTLAND The Scottish Government, Edinburgh 2008 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We are very grateful to the many individuals who have contributed their time and commitment to this study. Appendix 2 lists the main organisations who have taken part. The views expressed in this report are those of the researcher and do not necessarily represent those of the Scottish Government or Scottish officials. © Crown copyright 2009 ISBN: 978-0-7559-5849-8 (Web only) The Scottish Government St Andrew’s House Edinburgh EH1 3DG Produced for the Scottish Government by RR Donnelley B56660 02/09 Published by the Scottish Government, February, 2009 CONTENTS 1. INTRODUCTION (including glossary of abbreviations) 1 2. METHODS 4 I Statistical information I Qualitative information I Documentary information THE LINGUISTIC ACCESS CONTEXT FOR DEAF PUPILS AND STUDENTS IN SCOTLAND 3. LANGUAGE APPROACHES USED WITH DEAF PUPILS IN SCOTLAND 7 I Monolingual approaches I Bilingual approaches I ‘No specific policy’ I Regional variation 4. LINGUISTIC ACCESS FOR DEAF STUDENTS IN FURTHER AND HIGHER EDUCATION 11 THE SCHOOL SECTOR: LINGUISTIC ACCESS TO EDUCATION FOR DEAF PUPILS NUMBERS OF PUPILS AND PROFESSIONALS 5. THE NUMBER OF DEAF PUPILS 13 I Scottish Government Statistics I ADPS statistics I Presenting statistics on the population of deaf pupils I Statistics relating to educational attainment I Recommendations 6.
    [Show full text]
  • American Sign Language
    U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES ∙ National Institutes of Health NIDCD Fact Sheet | Hearing and Balance American Sign Language What is American Sign Language? American Sign Language (ASL) is a complete, natural language that has the same linguistic properties as spoken languages, with grammar that differs from English. ASL is expressed by movements of the hands and face. It is the primary language of many North Americans who are deaf and hard of hearing, and is used by many hearing people as well. Is sign language the same in other countries? There is no universal sign language. Different sign languages are used in different countries or regions. For example, British Sign Language (BSL) is a different A young boy signs “I love you.” language from ASL, and Americans who know ASL may not understand BSL. Some countries adopt features of ASL in their sign languages. LSF are distinct languages. While they still contain some Where did ASL originate? similar signs, they can no longer be understood by each other’s users. No person or committee invented ASL. The exact beginnings of ASL are not clear, but some suggest that it How does ASL compare with spoken arose more than 200 years ago from the intermixing of language? local sign languages and French Sign Language (LSF, or Langue des Signes Française). Today’s ASL includes some ASL is a language completely separate and distinct elements of LSF plus the original local sign languages; from English. It contains all the fundamental features over time, these have melded and changed into a rich, of language, with its own rules for pronunciation, word complex, and mature language.
    [Show full text]
  • Ed 304 810 Author Title Institution Spons Agency
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 304 810 EC 212 300 AUTHOR Stewart, David A. TITLE A Model Communication and Language Policy for Total Communication Programs for the Hearing Impaired. Occasional Paper No. 125. INSTITUTION Michigan State Univ., East Lansing. Inst. for Research on Teaching. SPONS AGENCY Special Education Programs (ED/OSERS), Wa3hington, DC. PUB DATE Sep 88 GRANT G008730145 NOTE 25p. AVAILABLE FROM Institute for Research on Teaching, College of Education, Michigan State University, 252 Erickson Hall, East Lansing, MI 48825 ($3.00). PUB TYPE Viewpoints (120) -- Reports - Descriptive (141) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC01 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS American Sign Language; *Educational Policy; Elementary Secondary Education; *Hearing Impairments; Interpersonal Communication; Intervention; Manual Communication; Models; School Policy; *Sign Language; *Teacher Behavior; *Teaching Methods; *Total Communication ABSTRACT This paper argues that current practices in total communication classrooms have basically assigned the responsibility of communication to hearing-impaired students wIlo must adapt to the variation in communication behaviors displayed by each of their teachers. The paper advocates use of a model communication and language policy designed to implement consistent linguistic input in the instruction of hearing-impaired students in total communication programs. The consistent use of a modified form of Signed English as the primary sign system and the use of American Sign Language as an intervention tool form the basis for establishing consistent linguistic input in the classroom. Following a rationale for adoption of such a policy, an appendix contains the communication and language policy that was developed for the Lansing (Michigan) School District's hearing-impaired programs. Two other appendixes describe the characteristics of modified signed English and provide a brief perspective of pidgin signs.
    [Show full text]