Filling In: Syncopation, Pleasure and Distributed Embodiment in Groove

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Filling In: Syncopation, Pleasure and Distributed Embodiment in Groove DOI: 10.1111/musa.12082 MARIAA.G.WITEK FILLING IN:SYNCOPATION,PLEASURE AND DISTRIBUTED EMBODIMENT IN GROOVE On a live recording of the funk and soul saxophonist Maceo Parker’s ‘Shake Everything YouGot!’ from the album Life on Planet Groove (1992),1 the drummer, Kenwood Dennard, is allocated almost two minutes for his solo. After being accompanied by Parker’s rhythmic stuttering and shouting, Dennard is left to play the groove alone in what is now a classic marker of funk – the drum break.2 Dennard continues in the designated tempo (ca. 120 beats per minute) with a rhythmic pattern which is complex but rarely strays from the track’s regular 4/4 metre. The rhythm is heavily syncopated across the drum kit, but the audience captured on this live recording demonstrates its metrical certainty by clapping hands on every second beat (beats 2 and 4). At two points during the drum break, Dennard syncopates in such a way that beat 4 is entirely omitted, leaving only the audience’s clapping to signify the beat. Here, when the drums are silent, the listeners sound the beat with their body movements. The audience seems to find this deeply satisfying, as indicated by the sound of cheers following Dennard’s return on ‘the One’.3 Thus, even when we are listening to the recording, removed from the jazz club hosting the concert (Stadtgarten, Cologne, 1992), we can gain the same enjoyment of the audience’s momentary embodied expression of the beat. What is it about this scenario that feels so enjoyable? What is the significance of syncopation in groove, and to what extent can it explain the pleasure we experience through physical embodiment and synchronisation in dance- and movement-directed music? In this article, groove is analysed phenomenologically as a triangulation of rhythmic structure, embodiment and pleasure. A consideration of groove research and theories of extended mind and affective practice demonstrate how groove is distributed between mind, body and music. In this distribution, musical pleasure is not simply caused by sensory stimulation; rather, it emerges dynamically through the intertwining of the corporeal, material and psychological. Syncopation provides an example of the structural complexity of groove that opens up empty spaces in the rhythmic surface which invite the body to fill in through entrainment. By filling in temporal gaps, bodies extend into the musical structure, and the desire to fill the gaps and complete the groove affords a participatory pleasure. 138 Music Analysis, 36/i (2017) © 2016 The Author. Music Analysis © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA FILLING IN 139 Groove During the last few decades groove has attracted attention from a number of disciplines, most noticeably ethnomusicology, musicology and psychology. It is therefore surprising that the current definition of groove in the New Grove Dictionary Online, while highlighting some of its most significant aspects – most important, its effects on the body and degree of repetition – accepts some very vague descriptions of the relationship between the structural and the experiential: In the realm of jazz, a [groove is a] persistently repeated pattern. More broadly, Feld (1988), studying groove from an ethnomusicological perspective, defines it cautiously as “an unspecifiable but ordered sense of something that is sustained in a distinctive, regular and attractive way, working to draw the listener in”. Connections to dance are important, and the statement that a performance has, or achieves, a groove, usually means that it somehow compels the body to move. (Kernfeld 2009; emphasis added) Furthermore, it is unclear from this definition what the groove actually consists of. Is it the music itself, a property of the music, the experience of it or our behaviour towards it? Or is it perhaps a special combination of these factors? Together with Charles Keil, Steven Feld was one of the first to study groove as a musical concept. In Music Grooves (1994), they both directed broadly ethnomusicological approaches towards groove in cultures ranging from African American jazz communities to the Kaluli of New Guinea. In their writing, music is treated as an embodied process, and analyses are directed towards performances rather than works. In these analyses groove is found in microtemporal performance nuances, often described as ‘push’ or ‘pull’ against the main pulse, such that some notes slightly anticipate or are played shortly after time points on a strict metric grid.4 Keil coined the term ‘Participatory Discrepancies’ (PDs) to describe such nuances, claiming that small variations in timing promote participation and social interaction on the part of both performer and listener: ‘The power of music is in its participatory discrepancies. [ . ] Music, to be personally involving and socially valuable, must be “out of time” and “out of tune”’ (1987, p. 275). The term ‘engendered feeling’ is also employed, referring to the processual, emergent qualities of affect elicited by PDs in rhythm, qualities felt in the body (Keil and Feld 1994, p. 54). Such participatory views are now common in approaches to musical experience, and few maintain that audiences and listeners are ever passive respondents. Tia De Nora’s (2000) ethnography of aerobics classes and Judith Becker’s (2004) research on trancing, for example, both foreground the notion of human-music interaction. However, it is rare for this interaction to be analysed in detail. For example, Keil and Feld leave us wondering what it is about PDs that afford engendered feeling. What is participatory about the participatory discrepancies? Musicologists and phenomenologists studying groove have also focused on such ‘microtiming’, bringing us closer to clarifying how experience is related Music Analysis, 36/i (2017) © 2016 The Author. Music Analysis © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 140 MARIAA.G.WITEK to the musical structure. In her book on the funk grooves of James Brown and Parliament, Anne Danielsen (2006) treats microtiming as a way of creating forward motion and rhythmic interplay between textural layers. Together with repetition, metric displacement and cross-rhythm, such structures promote a ‘groove mode of listening’ (pp. 147–9), in which time is organised cyclically. While in ‘the state of being in a groove’, listeners are immersed in an ‘eternal present’ and an attentional equilibrium (p. 154). Because the groove is repetitive, listeners’ expectations with respect to events of rhythmic tension do not move forwards and backwards in protension and retention, but are directed inwards to what is happening at that moment in time. This does not mean, however, that groove is static. Inspired by Jacques Deleuze’s Difference and Repetition (1968), in which a productive and dynamic kind of repetition is identified, Danielsen (2006, pp. 162–4) describes how the groove is constantly moving; the same structural gestures are simply repeated time and time again. For Danielsen, groove is neither fully within the music nor within the listener, but emerges only in the relationship between the two.5 She distinguishes between figure and gesture, between the imagined and the sounding: the figure is the referential structure, while the gesture is the musical utterance that expresses the figure. A rhythmic pattern notated according to traditional metric categories (i.e. on a metric grid) is the figure, while the rhythmic pattern as performed, with all its articulations and microrhythmic nuances that cannot easily be transcribed, is the gesture. However, the figure is not simply a mental representation, abstracted from the perceived gesture, but a virtual component of it, actualised in its gestural form. In other words, there is always a mutual relationship between reference structure and sounding event, and structure is both virtual and real, in both the listener and the music.6 The relationship between musical structure and listener is equally important in Tiger Roholt’s (2014) more recent phenomenological analysis of groove. As in Keil and Feld’s (1994) work on PDs, it is ‘rhythmic nuance’, the microtemporal sophistication in rhythmic expression, that is the source of groove; Roholt’s arguments thus resemble those surrounding engendered feeling. He claims that, although the timing nuances are important, they must be experienced ‘engagedly’, not ‘analytically’, in order for groove to occur (p. 3). This ‘feeling’ of groove is inseparable from the body that feels it. Groove is to be understood by the body and requires body movement to be actualised. The kind of embodiment that Roholt finds in groove is an instance of the phenomenologist Maurice Merleau-Ponty’s (1945) ‘motor intentionality’, which describes an unconscious yet practical directness towards an object, a bodily knowing that feels more like ‘grasping’ than representing (Roholt 2014, pp. 96–103). According to Roholt, groove is the affective dimension of motor intentionality afforded by the tension between moving with the beat and the deviating rhythmic nuances (p. 105). He concludes that groove is only partly located in a recording, that it is ontologically incomplete and requires listeners’ embodied engagement. © 2016 The Author. Music Analysis, 36/i (2017) Music Analysis © 2016 John Wiley & Sons Ltd FILLING IN 141 Syncopation, Entrainment and Affect While many groove-based genres do indeed involve microtiming (e.g. jazz, funk, R & B and hip-hop), others do not. In particular, some forms of electronic dance music (e.g. house and techno) are not microrhythmically sophisticated in this way, but nonetheless ‘compel the body to move’, involve repetition and generally fit the New Grove definition of groove (Kernfeld 2009). After musicologists and behavioural psychologists had established the basic experiential effect of groove (Iyer 2002; Zbikowski 2004; and Madison 2006), leading to its psychological definition as a musical quality associated with a pleasurable desire to move (Janata, Tomic and Haberman 2012), researchers began examining which musical structures best fit this experience.
Recommended publications
  • THE ENVIRONMENTAL IMAGINATIONS of MOBY-DICK: TECHNOLOGY and VULNERABILITY in HUMAN/MORE- THAN-HUMAN RELATIONSHIPS Jensen A
    University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Graduate School Professional Papers 2019 THE ENVIRONMENTAL IMAGINATIONS OF MOBY-DICK: TECHNOLOGY AND VULNERABILITY IN HUMAN/MORE- THAN-HUMAN RELATIONSHIPS Jensen A. Lillquist University of Montana, Missoula Let us know how access to this document benefits ouy . Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Part of the Environmental Studies Commons, Literature in English, North America Commons, and the Other Philosophy Commons Recommended Citation Lillquist, Jensen A., "THE ENVIRONMENTAL IMAGINATIONS OF MOBY-DICK: TECHNOLOGY AND VULNERABILITY IN HUMAN/MORE-THAN-HUMAN RELATIONSHIPS" (2019). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 11320. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/11320 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE ENVIRONMENTAL IMAGINATIONS OF MOBY-DICK: TECHNOLOGY AND VULNERABILITY IN HUMAN/MORE-THAN-HUMAN RELATIONSHIPS By JENSEN ARTHUR LILLQUIST Bachelor of Arts in English Literature, The University of Montana, Missoula, MT, 2017 Thesis presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of: Master of Arts in English Literature, Ecocriticism The University of Montana Missoula, MT May 2019 Approved by: Scott Whittenburg, Dean of The Graduate School Graduate School Dr. Louise Economides, Co-Chair Department of English Dr. Katie Kane, Co-Chair Department of English Dr. Christopher Preston Department of Philosophy i Lillquist, Jensen, M.A., May 2019 English Literature The Environmental Imaginations of Moby-Dick: Technology and Vulnerability in Human/More- than-Human Relationships Co-Chairperson: Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Socio-Affective Distribution, Vibe, and Dance-Music Consciousness Provided by Philpapers
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE Feeling at one: Socio-affective distribution, vibe, and dance-music consciousness provided by PhilPapers Music and Consciousness 2: Worlds, Practices, Modalities music and Ruth Herbert, David Clarke, a nd Eric Clarke consciousness 2 WO'!" pr6, 111 Print publication date: 2019 Print ISBN-13: 9780198804352 Publish ed to Oxford Scholarship Online: July 2019 DOI: 10.1093/oso/9780198804352.001.0001 Feeling at one: Socio-affective distribution, vibe, and dance-music consciousness Maria A. G. Witek DOI: 10.1093/oso/9780198804352.003.0006 Abstract and Keywords In this ch apter, t he embodie d consciou s n ess of clu bbing a nd ravin g is consider ed throu g h t he theory of exten ded mind, accord ing t o w hich the mind is a distrib uted system w here brain , b ody, a nd environment play equa l p arts. Bu ildin g on the idea of music as affective at mosp here, a case is made for con sidering t he vibe of a da nce party as cognitively, socially, and affectively distrib uted . The chapter su g gests that p a rticipating in the vib e affords primary musical conscious ness- a kind of p re-refl exive st ate ch aracterized by affective an d bodily knowledge- and sp eculates ab out t he n eu ral correlates of clubbing an d raving by m eans of an a nalogy with b rain r esearch on p sychedelic sta tes.
    [Show full text]
  • Media Inquiries: Kristin Elise Phillips, Department of Communications November 2010 212-496-3419; [email protected]
    Media Inquiries: Kristin Elise Phillips, Department of Communications November 2010 212-496-3419; [email protected] www.amnh.org BRAIN: THE INSIDE STORY ON VIEW AT THE AMERICAN MUSEUM OF NATURAL HISTORY FROM NOVEMBER 20, 2010 THROUGH AUGUST 14, 2011 INTERACTIVE EXHIBITS SHOWCASE LATEST RESEARCH ABOUT THE HUMAN BRAIN EXHIBITION BEGINS INTERNATIONAL TOUR IN NOVEMBER 2011 The human brain—the result of millions of years of evolutionary history—uses molecular, chemical, and electrical signals to interpret information, weigh decisions, and learn at every stage of life. Drawing on 21st-century research and technology, Brain: The Inside Story offers visitors a new perspective and keen insight into their own brains through imaginative art, vivid brain-scan imaging, and dynamic interactive exhibits for all ages. The exhibition, which is on view at the American Museum of Natural History from Saturday, November 20, until August 14, 2011, brings visitors up to date on the latest in neuroscience, highlighting the brain’s surprising ability to rewire itself in response to experience, disability, or trauma, and showcases new technologies that researchers use to study the brain and treat conditions such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s. ―The human brain is the most complex and fascinating biological structure known, and we are delighted to explore its many facets in Brain: The Inside Story,‖ says Ellen V. Futter, President of the American Museum of Natural History. ―This exhibition illustrates how our remarkable brains work and what makes them so special, while featuring what is currently one of the hottest and most promising fields in science today—neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • Music, Trance, Religious Experience, and the Brain
    >> I'm Steve Mencher for the Library of Congress with another one of our Music and the Brain podcasts. Our guest today is Robin Sylvan. He's a researcher on the spiritual and religious dimensions of music and Founder and Director of the Sacred Center in Oakland, California, author of Traces of the Spirit, The Religious Dimension Of Popular Music and Transformation, the Spiritual and Religious and Dimensions of Global Rave Culture. And here's where I'd like to start today, which is talking about you and I'd like you to feel free to bring up your work, your research, your passions into this discussion, but let's use you as a starting point and tell me how old you are for example. >> Robin Sylvan: I am 51 years old. >> Okay because that sort of places you in the middle of some kind of musical nexus when we start to talk about music. >> Robin Sylvan: Okay. >> I read that you have maintained a daily Buddhist sitting meditation practice for about 25 years. >> Robin Sylvan: Yes, it's kind like part of my daily regimen of settling my mind and getting into a clear and grounded space before I begin the day, so that's been very important to me over the years. >> Is this a particular branch of Buddhism? >> Robin Sylvan: It's just a simple breathing practice in Tibetan tradition. It's called Shamatha but it's very similar to Zazen or it's just a very simple practice that's following your breath. Of course that sounds simpler than it is because the mind is constantly generating lots of stuff to distract you from that, but that's the essential nature of the practice.
    [Show full text]
  • Peak Experiences with Electronic Dance Music: Subjective Experiences, Physiological Responses, and Musical Characteristics of the Break Routine
    This is a repository copy of Peak experiences with electronic dance music: Subjective experiences, physiological responses, and musical characteristics of the break routine. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/145911/ Version: Published Version Article: Solberg, R.T. and Dibben, N. orcid.org/0000-0002-9250-5035 (2019) Peak experiences with electronic dance music: Subjective experiences, physiological responses, and musical characteristics of the break routine. Music Perception, 36 (4). pp. 371-389. ISSN 0730-7829 https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2019.36.4.371 Published as Ragnhild Torvanger Solberg, Nicola Dibben, Peak Experiences with Electronic Dance Music: Subjective Experiences, Physiological Responses, and Musical Characteristics of the Break Routine, Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, Vol. 36 No. 4, April 2019; (pp. 371-389). © 2019 by the Regents of the University of California. Copying and permissions notice: Authorization to copy this content beyond fair use (as specified in Sections 107 and 108 of the U. S. Copyright Law) for internal or personal use, or the internal or personal use of specific clients, is granted by the Regents of the University of California for libraries and other users, provided that they are registered with and pay the specified fee via Rightslink® or directly with the Copyright Clearance Center. Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version.
    [Show full text]
  • Electronic Dance Music and Culture in the Pacific Northwest
    Linfield University DigitalCommons@Linfield Senior Theses Student Scholarship & Creative Works Spring 2011 Electronic Dance Music and Culture in the Pacific Northwest Megan Myer Linfield College Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.linfield.edu/muscstud_theses Part of the Music Commons Recommended Citation Myer, Megan, "Electronic Dance Music and Culture in the Pacific Northwest" (2011). Senior Theses. 3. https://digitalcommons.linfield.edu/muscstud_theses/3 This Thesis (Open Access) is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It is brought to you for free via open access, courtesy of DigitalCommons@Linfield, with permission from the rights-holder(s). Your use of this Thesis (Open Access) must comply with the Terms of Use for material posted in DigitalCommons@Linfield, or with other stated terms (such as a Creative Commons license) indicated in the record and/or on the work itself. For more information, or if you have questions about permitted uses, please contact [email protected]. Electronic Dance Music and Culture in the Pacific Northwest By: Megan Myer M y e r | 2 Table of Contents Introduction … … … … … … … … … P. 3 Chapter 1: History of Electronic Music … … … … … … P. 4 Chapter 2: History of Electronic Dance Music … … … … P. 9 Chapter 3: Musical Genres … … … … … … … P. 12 Chapter 4: Culture … … … … … … … … P. 16 Definition of Key Terms … … … … … … … P. 22 Bibliography … … … … … … … … … P. 23 M y e r | 3 Megan Myer Dr. Joan Paddock/Liz Obert MUSC 490/EART 485 Spring 2011 Electronic Dance Music and Culture in the Pacific Northwest A Brief Introduction: "A digital multimedia exploration of Electronic Dance Music (EDM) history and theory; the Pacific Northwest rave experience of the 21st century." In today‟s world of music, everything is constantly changing alongside the progression of technology.
    [Show full text]
  • Socio-Affective Distribution, Vibe, and Dance-Music Consciousness
    Feeling at one: Socio-affective distribution, vibe, and dance-music consciousness Music and Consciousness 2: Worlds, Practices, Modalities music and Ruth Herbert, David Clarke, a nd Eric Clarke consciousness 2 WO'!" pr6, 111 Print publication date: 2019 Print ISBN-13: 9780198804352 Publish ed to Oxford Scholarship Online: July 2019 DOI: 10.1093/oso/9780198804352.001.0001 Feeling at one: Socio-affective distribution, vibe, and dance-music consciousness Maria A. G. Witek DOI: 10.1093/oso/9780198804352.003.0006 Abstract and Keywords In this ch apter, t he embodie d consciou s n ess of clu bbing a nd ravin g is consider ed throu g h t he theory of exten ded mind, accord ing t o w hich the mind is a distrib uted system w here brain , b ody, a nd environment play equa l p arts. Bu ildin g on the idea of music as affective at mosp here, a case is made for con sidering t he vibe of a da nce party as cognitively, socially, and affectively distrib uted . The chapter su g gests that p a rticipating in the vib e affords primary musical conscious ness- a kind of p re-refl exive st ate ch aracterized by affective an d bodily knowledge- and sp eculates ab out t he n eu ral correlates of clubbing an d raving by m eans of an a nalogy with b rain r esearch on p sychedelic sta tes. Keywords: dance music, clubbing, raving, vibe, extended mind, psychedelic Introduction I was at 'Unity II' rave this p ast weekend, a nd I h ad t he m ost wonderful experien ce.
    [Show full text]
  • A Study of Rave Culture
    LIGHT AND SOUND UNDERGROUND: A STUDY OF RAVE CULTURE A Senior Honors Thesis by SUMMER GIOIA HARRISON Submitted to the Office of Honors Programs k Academic Scholarships Texas ARM University in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the UNIVERSITY UNDERGRADUATE RESEARCH FELLOWS April 2002 Group: Art and Literature I I,IGHT AND SO( JND UNDERGROUND: A STUDY OF RAVE CULTURE A Senior Honors 'I'hesis by SUMMER GIOIA HARRISON Submitted to the Office of Honors Programs Sr. Academic Scholarships Texas AEcM I tniversity in partial fulfillment for the designation ol UNIVERSITY UNDERGRADUATE RL'SEARCI I I ELLOAV Approved as to style and content by: Fdvvard A. Funkhouser Dbuglas Brooks y (Fellows Advisor) (Executive Director) April 2002 Group: Art and Literature I ABSTRACT Light and Sound Underground: A Study of Rave Culture. (April 2002) Summer Gioia Harrison Department of English Texas A&M University Fellows Advisor: Dr. Douglas Brooks Department of English Pulsating colors flash, beat throbs deeper, deeper down, lift me up into this ecstasy: thc world of rave. Rave culture is a strikingly significant, subversive subculture of recent and current times. Unique to the subculture are: rave music, rave dance, rave fashion, as well as specific tools and machines (i.e. technology used), behaviors, rituals, mind-altering drugs, jargon, and above all, the ecstatic community events that are raves. A subculture must provide for its participants something that may well be unavailable to them from the dominant culture. This starts with a sense of belonging to a "group" that appeals to them rationally and aesthetically. Rave in particular provides inclusion in an international community, as well as senses of festivity, intensity, emotional release, and collective experience; these in turn invoke alternative ideas/modes of thought and behavior/ways of living.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring Psychedelic Trance and Electronic Dance Music in Modern Culture
    Exploring Psychedelic Trance and Electronic Dance Music in Modern Culture Emília Simão Portuguese Catholic University, Portugal Armando Malheiro da Silva University of Porto, Portugal Sérgio Tenreiro de Magalhães Portuguese Catholic University, Portugal Managing Director: Lindsay Johnston Managing Editor: Austin DeMarco Director of Intellectual Property & Contracts: Jan Travers Acquisitions Editor: Kayla Wolfe Production Editor: Christina Henning Cover Design: Jason Mull Published in the United States of America by Information Science Reference (an imprint of IGI Global) 701 E. Chocolate Avenue Hershey PA 17033 Tel: 717-533-8845 Fax: 717-533-8661 E-mail: [email protected] Web site: http://www.igi-global.com Copyright © 2015 by IGI Global. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored or distributed in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, without written permission from the publisher. Product or company names used in this set are for identification purposes only. Inclusion of the names of the products or companies does not indicate a claim of ownership by IGI Global of the trademark or registered trademark. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Exploring psychedelic trance and electronic dance music in modern culture / Emila Simao, Armando Malheiro de Silva, and Sergio Tenreiro de Magalhaes, editors. pages cm Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-1-4666-8665-6 (hardcover) -- ISBN 978-1-4666-8666-3 (ebook) 1. Trance. 2. Altered states of consciousness. 3. Underground dance music. I. Simao, Emilia, 1976- editor. BF1045.A48E97 2015 306.4’846--dc23 2015015529 British Cataloguing in Publication Data A Cataloguing in Publication record for this book is available from the British Library.
    [Show full text]
  • Press 29 April 2019 the Da Vincis of the Dancefloor – Meet the Artists Capturing Clubland
    The Guardian The Da Vincis of the dancefloor – meet the artists capturing clubland Sheryl Garratt 29 April 2019 The Da Vincis of the dancefloor – meet the artists capturing clubland Drawing a crowd … Denzil Forrester’s Night Flames, 2012, inspired by his love of dub reggae. Photograph: courtesy the artist and Stephen Friedman Gallery Why take a selfie in a sweaty club when you can buy a painting of your banging night out instead? We meet the ravers turning 3am euphoria into pulsating art It would be around midnight when Jah Shaka’s truck pulled up outside Phebes nightclub in east London, and his crew would start humping his big sound system speakers into the dark, cramped, low- ceilinged basement ready for an all-night dub reggae session. As the excitement built and the basement filled with people, Denzil Forrester would squeeze behind the long, narrow bar that ran down one side of the room, prop his A1 sketchbook on the counter … and start sketching. It wasn’t the easiest place to work. It was dark, hot and crowded, the air fogged with weed and tobacco smoke. Then, when Shaka fired up The Guardian The Da Vincis of the dancefloor – meet the artists capturing clubland Sheryl Garratt 29 April 2019 the system and the heavy bass kicked in, it would take a while for Forrester to adjust to the vibrations passing through his body. ‘By the end of the night I’d have about 40 pieces’ … Denzil Forrester. Photograph: Sean Drakes/LatinContent/Getty Images “I’d try and draw to the length of a record, so three to four minutes.
    [Show full text]
  • Detroit Techno – the Aesthetics of Music Without Stage and Performance Ekkehard Windrich
    Detroit Techno – The Aesthetics of Music without Stage and Performance Ekkehard Windrich May 2013 Master's Thesis Institute of Sonology Den Haag 2 Abstract This thesis examines the musical workings and perception of 1990s Detroit Techno as a mutual socio-musical process between producers, DJ and dancers. The way Techno is communicated is shown as a self-tuning system mainly based on non-symbolic, intrinsic musical functionality. Systems like these have no centre of attention and origination, which is reflected in the absence of stage and performance in early Techno. The social implications of Techno are discussed in context of multiplicity, a crucial term for the different compositional concepts both of Dick Raaijmakers and John Cage. Keywords: Auditory Scene Analysis, Cognitive Science, Dance Floor, Detroit Techno, Embodied Cognition, Movement, Multiplicity, Musical Communication, Sampling, Self-tuning system, Step Sequencer; 3 Acknowledgements I have to thank The Institute of Sonology for two equally dense as inspiring years. Probably there has been no other period of my life in which I learned so much about music. The artistic openness towards my project, despite its somewhat non-sonological character, was in every respect outstanding. Furthermore I want to thank my parents, my sister and my brother for their sustained mental and practical support and the substantial interest with which they follow my musical development. This thesis would not have been possible without Martin Retschitzegger and Michael 'Duke' Peter, our heated discussions about Techno and the many nights we spent in Berlin's clubs. Finally I thank Kathrin Grenzdörffer for the happiness I am still unable to grasp.
    [Show full text]