A STUDY of the PREVALENCE of MENSTRUAL SYNCHRONY in the Dorlyiitories of BALL STATE U1tiversiry

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A STUDY of the PREVALENCE of MENSTRUAL SYNCHRONY in the Dorlyiitories of BALL STATE U1tiversiry A STUDY OF THE PREVALENCE OF MENSTRUAL SYNCHRONY IN THE DORlYiITORIES OF BALL STATE U1TIVERSIry An Honors Thesis (ID 499) by Kandice Welch Thesis Director Dr. Anne Cartwright Ball State University Muncie, Indiana May, 1978 Sf(.c \1 ·1\\eSI~> L ..') :.t1lti . -z.!t 11'6 \I \,\)1./5,'), ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author wishes to express her appreciation for her thesis director, Dr. Anne Cartwright, for the valuable, but sometimes frustrating learning experience of writing an acceptable scientific paper. I would also like to thank the teachers Nho allowed me to canvas their classes for volunteers, and the subjects themselves wi thout ~Thom there would be no paper. The author "t'Tould like to acknowledge Dr. McComish for the use of his calculator. Special thanks are in order for my indispensable husband, Mike. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS • • • . ii LIST OF APPE~IDICES . .. iv CHAPTER I. INTRODUCTION......... • • 1 II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE • . 2 III. MATERIALS AND METHODS . 14 IV. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION • . 17 - V. SUMMARY A~ID CONCLUSIONS . 22 LITERATURE CITED • • • • • • • • • . 23 iii LIST OF APPENDICES Page 1. Ball State University assurance and certifica- tion of revie't'j of a single project involving human subjects · · · · · • · · · • • · • · · • · 25 2. Packet of forms used in study • · · • • · • · · 28 3. Ba clc ground information on control subjects · • · 34 4. Background information on experimental subjel~ts 35 5. First day of menses for control subjects, August through April • · · • · • · • · · · • • • 36 6. First day of menses for experimental subjects, August through April · • • · · · • • · · · • • · 37 7. Differences between onset of menses for sub- - ject pairs • • · · · · • · · • • · · • • · • • • 38 8. t-test results · · · · · · • · · · · · • • • • · 39 iv INTRODUCTION The purpose of this study was to determine l'rhether a relationship existed betw"een two ';'mmen rooming together in a Ball State University dormitory and the timing of their menstrual cycles. NcClintock (1971) condu.cted a similar study in an eastern all women college and found that within a period of four monthes the time of menses for roommates and close friends began to synchronize. The present study differed from that of McClintock i,n that the basic environnent was a co-educational instituti.on. Similar reproductive phenomenona to that observed - by HcClintock have been observed among other mammals, and the causes are believed to be pheromonal in nature. Pheromones are odors which carry messages from one individ­ ual to another. The 'ltvord itself is derived from the Greek pherein, to transfer; hormon, to excite. There is much current speculation on the possible role of pheromones in both nonhuman and human primate behavior. REVIEW OF THE RELATED LITERATURE Karlson and Luscher (1959:55) defined pheromones as "substances which are secreted to the outside by an ind­ ividual and received by a second individual of the same species, in which they release a specific reaction, for example, a definite behaviour or a developmental process." Law and Regnier (1971) in their review article su'bdi vided pheromones into two divisions; releasers and prim1ers. Releasers had an immediate effect on the behavior of the recipient whereas primers had a delayed effect. 'rhese definitions were originally applied to insect pheromones. Pheromones were first discovered in 1896 when it was found that live female moths in cardboard traps could be used to attract male moths (Comfort, 1972). Sinee that time many different insect pheromones l'fi th many different functions have been identified. Law and Regnier (1971) cited examples of recruitment pheromones to increase foraging efficiency and alarm pheromones which sig;naled flight or aggression both of which were found in social insects. The sting apparatus of bees contained alarm pheromones which induced other bees to sting at the same location. Ants were also often attracted by alarm pher­ omones. The queen honeybee secreted a pheromone 'I'1'hich prevented the development of worker ovaries. 3 The majority of the studies on insect pheromones concentrated on those compounds vThich were responsible for sexual attraction. Many compounds which have the same attractive effect have been synthesized, and they may prove vA.luable in controlling harmful insects. Since pheromones are interspecific they do not have the broad spectrum effect of most insecticides which destroy both the useful as well as the harmful insects (Comfort, 1972). At this time pheromones have not proven to be feasible in controlling heavy insect infestations; how"ever, they have been useful in the control of light infestations. Pheromones have also been useful as an assay tool for measuring the size of insect populations, and the communica­ tion disruption effect of pheromones has been studied as a possible method of regulating population growth. Communica­ tion disruption consisted of spraying large doses of the male attractant pheromone over infested areas during the breeding season in order to confuse the males in their attempts to find the true females (Marx, 1973). The effect of olfactory stimuli such as the attraction of males to estrous females, have been observed in mammals for centuries. However, it was not until the early 1950's when olfactory influence on the reproductive cyclE~s of laboratory mice were observed that mammalian pheromones attracted serious scientific interest. Lee and Boot (1956) found that when female mice were grouped together in the absence of a male, many of the mice 4 became anestrous. These mice also exhibited a high rate of pseudopregnancies. Lee and .~ suspected that the stimulus was olfactory in nature, for neither phenomenon oecurred among mice in which the olfactory bulb had been removed. A few years after the discovery of what is now known as the Lee-Boot effect, Whitten (1959) found that grouped anestrous female mice would come into estrous when exposed to a male mouse or his urine. Furthermore, if all of the mice were exposed to this stimulus at the same time, the presence of the male served to synchronize their estrous cycles. A third pheromonal effect on the reproductj.ve pro­ cess in mice was discovered by Bruce (1959). When pregnant females were exposed to an unfamiliar male or his urine they reabsorbed their developing fetuses. The sexual maturation of mice may also be tnflu­ enced by pheromones. Drict~mer (1977) discovered that the first estrous of young female mice was delayed by exposure to either grouped females or their urine. Physical contact and social interaction with grouped females produeed no more delay in the sexual maturation of the young than did exposure to the urine of the grouped females alonE~. Another study discovered that there was an andr6gen­ dependent urinary pheromone in male mice which aceelerated sexual maturation in young females (Lombardi, 197(,). This pheromone appeared to be suppressed in both juvenile and - adult males when they were placed in a subordinatE~ social 5 - role to a . more dominant male. Production of this pheromone may be suppressed by the presence of pregnant or lactating females. Lombardi suggested that in high density populations the intense male-male interactions and the presence of preg­ nant and lactating females may have partially accounted for suppressed fertility in populations where males are abundant. Pheromones may have played a role in developing mother-young bonds in some rodents. r101 tz and LE~idahl (1977) suggested that in lactating female rats the prolactin in some manner altered the composition of the bile. This alteration effected the chemistry of the cecum of the rat which resulted in production of a maternal pheromone. This pheromone was secreted with the female's feces. After receiving injections of bile taken from lactatine~ female rats, males were also able to release this young··attractant pheromone. Doty (1976) reviewed several studies which indicated that pheromones may have played a role in the nursing activity, ,nest location, and clumping behavior of rodent young. Doty (1976) cited many other examples of the role of pheromones in non-primate mammalian behavior, both among domestic and feral animal populations. Many un~llates had special scent gland which were used to mark off terri­ tory. Odor might also have been important in the identifica­ tion of offspring by many female ungulates. The presence of male pigs ~vas able to hasten sexual development in prepubertal 6 females although it has hot been shmm whether the stimulus ~ras olfactory in nature. There TTaS also data that the pre­ sence of a ram was able to induce synchronized estrous in female sheep and that this stimulus was at least partially olfactory (Morgan et al., 1972). Michael and Keverne (1968) :round that anosmic male rhesus monkeys could not distinguish between attractive estrogenized females and unatrractive ovariectomi.zed females. Later (1970) these authors discovered that the previously unattractive oVariectomized females could be made attractive to normal ma,les when their genital areas were pai.nted with the washings from the vagin3.s of estrogenized females. They postulated that there were attractive chemicals ln the vaginal secretions of the estrogenized females ~.J'hich stim­ ulated both male interest and overt male sexual behavior (mounting and ejaculation) even though the ovariE~ctomized female l.;as not aroused and i'.J'as not giving the appropriate behavioral cues. Keverne and ~1ichael (1970) called these hypothesized chemicals "copulins." Copulins were isolated from female monkeys in 1971 (Michael et a1., 1971). They proved to be short chain aliphatic acids and were tentatively identified as acetic, propionic, isobutyric, isovaleric, and isocaprioc aCids. In tests with four males these aliphatic acids, when separated from the vaginal secretions and applied to oVariectomized females, caused a substantial increase in overt male sexual behavior. 7 The question still remains unanswered of how large a part learning plays in these reproductive behaviors. Goldfoot, et a1. (1978) attempted to repeat l\1ichael's exp­ eriments using sexually experienced males which he made anosmic.
Recommended publications
  • Re-Cycling the Menstrual Cycle: a Multidisciplinary Reinterpretation of Menstruation
    Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Master's Theses Graduate College 12-1998 Re-Cycling the Menstrual Cycle: A Multidisciplinary Reinterpretation of Menstruation Heather H. Rea Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses Part of the Anthropology Commons Recommended Citation Rea, Heather H., "Re-Cycling the Menstrual Cycle: A Multidisciplinary Reinterpretation of Menstruation" (1998). Master's Theses. 3942. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/masters_theses/3942 This Masters Thesis-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. RE-CYCLING THE MENSTRUAL CYCLE: A MULTIDISCIPLINARY REINTERPRETATION OF MENSTRUATION by Heather H. Rea A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of The Graduate College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts Department of Anthropology Western Michigan University Kalamazoo, Michigan December 1998 Copyright by Heather H. Rea 1998 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank my thesis committee, Dr. Robert Anemone, Dr. David Karowe, and Dr. Erika Loeffler. Without their combined patience, insights, and senses of humor, this thesis would not have been completed. I would especially like to thank Dr. Loeffler who has been a supportive and inspirational boss, teacher, and friend throughout my graduate work. I would like to thank Marc Rea who suffered through the early stages of this thesis and my graduate work. He also suffered with me through our long-lost-psychotic-puppy's diaper-wearing first cycle of heat which inspired everything.
    [Show full text]
  • Female Reproductive Strategies and the Ovarian Cycle in Hamadryas
    Femalereproductivestrategiesandtheovarian cycleinhamadryasbaboons AthesissubmittedinpartialrequirementoftheMaster’sDegreeof ScienceinConservationBiologyatVictoriaUniversityofWellington by R.E.Tobler ABSTRACT: Thisthesisexaminestherelationshipbetweensexualbehaviourandthe ovarian cycle in a group-living primate, Papio h. hamadryas . Of particular interest is whetherfemalesmodifytheirovariancycleinamannerthatisexpectedincreasetheir reproductivesuccess.Thestudywasconductedonacaptivecolonywheretheresident males (RM) had been vasectomised prior to start of the study resulting in all mature femalesundergoingrepeatedovariancyclingthroughoutthestudyperiod.Thismadethe analysisofsexualbehaviourrelativetofinescalechangesintheovariancyclepossible. One year of ovarian cycle data and 280 hours of behavioural data was collected via observationalsamplingduringthestudy.RMvasectomisationdidnotalterthearchetypal one male unit social structure nor the typical socio-spatial organisation of wild hamadryas populations. Females were found to be more promiscuous than in wild populations, however, presumably because of the confounding effect that the high number of simultaneously cycling females had on RM herding (Chapter 1). RMs dominatedcopulationsovertheoptimalconceptiveperiodoftheovariancycle,whilethe majorityofextra-OMUcopulationsoccurredoutsidethisperiodandwererarelysolicited by females. This pattern supports a dual paternity concentration/paternity confusion strategy,andnotfemalechoiceorfertilityinsurancestrategies(Chapter2).Femaleswere not found to synchronise
    [Show full text]
  • Loss of Oestrus and Concealed Ovulation in Human Evolution
    Current Anthropology Volume 40, Number 3, June 1999 1999 by The Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research. All rights reserved 0011-3204/99/4003-0001 $2.50 The rich literature on the sexual behaviour of the Ho- mininae (Australopithecus and Homo) emphasizes the Loss of Oestrus and uniqueness in this regard of Homo sapiens and attri- butes considerable importance to the role of sexual be- haviour in the process of hominization and even in the Concealed Ovulation evolutionary success of the species (e.g., Morris 1967, Alexander and Noonan 1979, Lovejoy 1981, Kourtovik in Human Evolution 1983, Parker 1987, Turke 1988). The absence of signs of ovulation and the accompanying so-called constant receptivity in human females are considered fundamen- tal. The uniqueness of their emergence in the evolution The Case against the Sexual- of the Hominoidea is assumed on the basis of the chim- panzee model. This assumption has produced many Selection Hypothesis hypotheses as to the evolutionary causes of various forms of sexual signaling and behaviour allegedly char- acteristic of the Plio/Pleistocene evolution of the Ho- by Bogusøaw Pawøowski mininae. The adaptive advantage of constant receptiv- ity has been suggested to be greater cooperation within the group and reduction of competitiveness among males (Etkin 1963, Pfeiffer 1969, Fox 1972, Daniels 1983), the emergence of monogamy (Etkin 1954, Morris The assumption that absence of oestrus and of manifestations of 1967, Lovejoy 1981), the intensi®cation of paternal be- ovulation is speci®c to humans has given rise to various propos- als of a role for selection pressures in the evolution of these fea- haviour (Alexander and Noonan 1979; Symons 1979; tures in the form of sexual selection or other behavioural adapta- Strassmann 1981; Turke 1984, 1988), the possibility of tions.
    [Show full text]
  • Menstrual Synchrony: Fact Or Artifact?
    Menstrual Synchrony: Fact or Artifact? Anna Ziomkiewicz Polish Academy of Sciences Although more than thirty years of intensive investigation have passed since McClintock first published results on menstrual synchrony, there is still no conclu- sive evidence for the existence of this phenomenon. Indeed, a growing body of null- result studies, critiques of menstrual synchrony studies, and the lack of convincing evolutionary explanations bring into question the existence of this phenomenon. This paper presents results of a study conducted over five consecutive months in Polish student dormitories. In 18 pairs and 21 triples of college-age women, men- strual synchrony was not found. Social interactions, considered the most important factor mediating the effect of menstrual synchrony, was unrelated to any difference in menstrual cycle onsets. Initial menstrual onset difference was influenced by woman's body mass and menstrual cycle irregularity. These results provide further evidence that women do not synchronize their menstrual cycles. KEY WORDS: Menstrual cycle; Menstrual synchrony; Social interactions artha McClintock (197 l) first reported the apparent influence of social inter- M actions on menstrual cycles of women living in a college dormitory. In that study, 135 women, age 17 to 22, living in single or double rooms, were asked to recall (a) their last and second to last menstrual period three times during the school year, (b) time spent each week in the company of men, and (c) the list of girls they consider their best friends. Statistically significant effects of menstrual synchrony were reported for pairs of friends and groups of friends, which subsequently led to more than thirty years of intensive interest in the phenomenon of menstrual syn- chrony and its mechanisms.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Umwelten in a Changing World
    Tartu Semiotics Library 18 Tartu Tartu Semiotics Library 18 Animal umwelten in a changing world: Zoosemiotic perspectives represents a clear and concise review of zoosemiotics, present- ing theories, models and methods, and providing interesting examples of human–animal interactions. The reader is invited to explore the umwelten of animals in a successful attempt to retrieve the relationship of people with animals: a cornerstone of the past common evolutionary processes. The twelve chapters, which cover recent developments in zoosemiotics and much more, inspire the reader to think about the human condition and about ways to recover our lost contact with the animal world. Written in a clear, concise style, this collection of articles creates a wonderful bridge between Timo Maran, Morten Tønnessen, human and animal worlds. It represents a holistic approach Kristin Armstrong Oma, rich with suggestions for how to educate people to face the dynamic relationships with nature within the conceptual Laura Kiiroja, Riin Magnus, framework of the umwelt, providing stimulus and opportuni- Nelly Mäekivi, Silver Rattasepp, ties to develop new studies in zoosemiotics. Professor Almo Farina, CHANGING WORLD A IN UMWELTEN ANIMAL Paul Thibault, Kadri Tüür University of Urbino “Carlo Bo” This important book offers the first coherent gathering of perspectives on the way animals are communicating with each ANIMAL UMWELTEN other and with us as environmental change requires increasing adaptation. Produced by a young generation of zoosemiotics scholars engaged in international research programs at Tartu, IN A CHANGING this work introduces an exciting research field linking the biological sciences with the humanities. Its key premises are that all animals participate in a dynamic web of meanings WORLD: and signs in their own distinctive styles, and all animal spe- cies have distinctive cultures.
    [Show full text]
  • Between Species: Choreographing Human And
    BETWEEN SPECIES: CHOREOGRAPHING HUMAN AND NONHUMAN BODIES JONATHAN OSBORN A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY GRADUATE PROGRAM IN DANCE STUDIES YORK UNIVERSITY TORONTO, ONTARIO MAY, 2019 ã Jonathan Osborn, 2019 Abstract BETWEEN SPECIES: CHOREOGRAPHING HUMAN AND NONHUMAN BODIES is a dissertation project informed by practice-led and practice-based modes of engagement, which approaches the space of the zoo as a multispecies, choreographic, affective assemblage. Drawing from critical scholarship in dance literature, zoo studies, human-animal studies, posthuman philosophy, and experiential/somatic field studies, this work utilizes choreographic engagement, with the topography and inhabitants of the Toronto Zoo and the Berlin Zoologischer Garten, to investigate the potential for kinaesthetic exchanges between human and nonhuman subjects. In tracing these exchanges, BETWEEN SPECIES documents the creation of the zoomorphic choreographic works ARK and ARCHE and creatively mediates on: more-than-human choreography; the curatorial paradigms, embodied practices, and forms of zoological gardens; the staging of human and nonhuman bodies and bodies of knowledge; the resonances and dissonances between ethological research and dance ethnography; and, the anthropocentric constitution of the field of dance studies. ii Dedication Dedicated to the glowing memory of my nana, Patricia Maltby, who, through her relentless love and fervent belief in my potential, elegantly willed me into another phase of life, while she passed, with dignity and calm, into another realm of existence. iii Acknowledgements I would like to thank my phenomenal supervisor Dr. Barbara Sellers-Young and my amazing committee members Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • A Critical Review of Menstrual Synchrony Research
    psychon~uroendocrinology. Vok 17. No. 6. pp. 565-591, 1992 0306-4530/92 $5.00+0.00 Printed in Great Britain Pergamon Press I.~d. A CRITICAL REVIEW OF MENSTRUAL SYNCHRONY RESEARCH H. CLYDE WILSON Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri - Columbia, Columbia, Missouri, U.S.A. (Received 23 July 1990; in final form 25 July 1992) SUMMARY Two experiments and three studies reported a significant level of menstrual synchrony after subjects had been treated with applications of axillary extract from a donor subject or after subjects have spent time together. Four studies failed to replicate these results. A comparison of the studies shows the only consistent difference is that those studies not finding menstrual synchrony reported problems with subjects who had irregular cycle lengths, while those finding menstrual synchrony reported no such problems. All experiments and studies were based on the methods and research design introduced by McClintock (1971). Three errors are inherent in research based on her model: (1) an implicit assumption that differences between menses onsets of randomly paired subjects vary randomly over consecutive onsets, (2) an incorrect procedure for determining the initial onset absolute difference between subjects, and (3) exclusion of subjects or some onsets of subjects who do not have the number of onsets specified by the research design. All of these errors increase the probability of finding menstrual synchrony in a sample. One or more of these errors occurred in the experiments and studies reporting synchrony; no significant levels of menstrual synchrony occur when these errors are corrected. Menstrual synchrony is not demonstrated in any of the experiments or studies.
    [Show full text]
  • PDF Available Via Open Access
    You Mate, I Mate: Macaque Females Synchronize Sex not Cycles Ines Fu¨ rtbauer1,2,3*, Roger Mundry4, Michael Heistermann3, Oliver Schu¨ lke2,5, Julia Ostner1,2 1 Primate Social Evolution Group, Courant Research Centre Evolution of Social Behaviour, Georg-August-University Go¨ttingen, Go¨ttingen, Germany, 2 Integrative Primate Socio-Ecology Group, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany, 3 Reproductive Biology Unit, German Primate Center, Go¨ttingen, Germany, 4 Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany, 5 Courant Research Centre Evolution of Social Behaviour, Georg-August-University Go¨ttingen, Go¨ttingen, Germany Abstract Extended female sexuality in species living in multimale-multifemale groups appears to enhance benefits from multiple males. Mating with many males, however, requires a low female monopolizability, which is affected by the spatiotemporal distribution of receptive females. Ovarian cycle synchrony potentially promotes overlapping receptivity if fertile and receptive periods are tightly linked. In primates, however, mating is often decoupled from hormonal control, hence reducing the need for synchronizing ovarian events. Here, we test the alternative hypothesis that females behaviorally coordinate their receptivity while simultaneously investigating ovarian cycle synchrony in wild, seasonal Assamese macaques (Macaca assamensis), a promiscuous species with extremely extended female sexuality. Using fecal hormone analysis to assess ovarian activity we show that fertile phases are randomly distributed, and that dyadic spatial proximity does not affect their distribution. We present evidence for mating synchrony, i.e., the occurrence of the females’ receptivity was significantly associated with the proportion of other females mating on a given day. Our results suggest social facilitation of mating synchrony, which explains (i) the high number of simultaneously receptive females, and (ii) the low male mating skew in this species.
    [Show full text]
  • The Human Symbolic Revolution: a Darwinian Account
    Chris Knight, Camilla Power & Ian Watts, The Human Symbolic Revolution: A Darwinian Account. In Cambridge Archaeological Journal 5:1 (1995), pp. 75-114 75 The Human Symbolic Revolution: A Darwinian Account Chris Knight, Camilla Power & Ian Watts By 50,000 years ago, the effects of a ‘symbolic explosion’ — an efflorescence of human art, song, dance and ritual — were rippling across the globe. Applied to archaeological evidence, standard neo-Darwinian theory offers new understandings of this improbable event. The present article defines ‘symbolism’, models quasi-ritual behaviour in late archaic Homo sapiens, extends the argument to the emergence of anatomically modern humans and concludes with preliminary tests against archaeological, ethnographic and rock art data. The brain constructs from sensory inputs its own illusory version of the world outside. Unlike the real world, the illusory one is egocentric, its coordinates measured from the ‘here’ and ‘now’ of individual experience. Environmental features are picked out and interpreted; schemata for recognizing feeding and/or mating opportunities may be disproportionately developed. While cognition in all animals is distorted by such motivational bias, among humans a further distorting process occurs. Through exposure to ritual, art and other external memory stores, every individual constructs, in addition to the cognitive map just described, a personalized copy of a communal map, access to which defines membership of a symbolic community. Among modern humans, all behaviour and all cognition occurs in the context of this additional map — ‘a web of beliefs, rules, and values that gives all things and all actions symbolic cultural meaning’ (Chase 1994). Implanted by external public pressure, the motivational bias of such communal cognition is sociocentric, countering the bias of egocentric vision.
    [Show full text]
  • Female Proto-Symbolic Strategies
    Camilla Power & Leslie Aiello, 1997. Female Proto-symbolic Strategies. In Lori D Hager (ed), Women in Human Evolution. Routledge: London & New York. ISBN 0-415-10834-9 153 8 FEMALE PROTO-SYMBOLIC STRATEGIES Camilla Power and Leslie Aiello How did humans become the unique symbolic culture-bearing species that we are? One of the most exciting challenges for paleoanthropology is to understand this extraordinary event. Few convincing arguments have been put forward to explain the evolution of symbolic cognition on a Darwinian basis; no theory has ventured predictions which are testable against symbolic data from both the archaeological and the ethnographic records. Modern paleoanthropology dates from the 1966 “Man the Hunter” conference (Lee and DeVore 1968). Among the hunter-gatherer ethnographers, primatologists, paleontologists and archaeologists present, a consensus emerged that the evolution of humans was the evolution of the hunter-gatherer adaptation. At the time, evidence for the basic elements of the hunter-gatherer lifestyle — centered on food-sharing and a home base — was identified early in the fossil and archaeological record (Isaac 1971). A unilinear, gradualist model developed which proposed co-evolution of increasing amounts of meat in the hominid diet, increasing levels of co-operation between males in the hunt, and the development of cognition, language, and symbolic culture on that basis. Inspired by the rise of feminist consciousness in the 1970s, women scientists challenged the overt sexism of this account by advancing the parallel model of “Woman the Gatherer” (Tanner 1981). Invaluable for switching attention to female strategies, “Woman the Gatherer” arose at a time when the newly emergent discipline of sociobiology was offering the prospect of a female-centered methodology for evolutionary accounts (cf.
    [Show full text]
  • Recent Advances in Animal Behaviour
    Proc. Indian Acad. Sci. (Anim. Sci.), Vol. 94, No.3, June 1985, pp. 173-186 . .:© Printed in India. Recent advances in animal behaviour KM ALEXANDER Department of Zoology, University of Kerala, Kariavattom, Trivandrum 695581, India Abstract. Ethology, a fast developing field of animal sciences has considerable relevance in animal husbandry, agriculture, control ofanimal populations, pest control, medicine, wildlife biology,etc. It has made vast strides of progress during the past fewdecades and some of these trends are reviewed. Communication signals playa salient role in sociobiology of animal groups. Animals deploy visual, acoustic, tactile and olfactory signals during their social interactions. Among these, olfactory cues have certain specific advantages over the other modes concerned. Recently considerable attention has been focussed on chemical signals in animals, especially those of economically important forms such as insects, fishes and mammals. Regarding insects, sex pheromones, aphrodisiacs, trail markers, aggregating and alerting pheromones have been isolated in various insectan orders. The factors controlling sex pheromone behaviour and impact of pheromones on control of insect population have been elaborated. Investigations on chemical cues of lower vertebrates indicate that fishes, amphibians and reptiles deploy them in their social interactions. Pheromones modulate the schooling, reproductive and alarm response behaviour in fishes. Among mammals, urine, fecal pellets, saliva and secretions of specialised skin glands function as sources of olfactory cues. Data on histophysiology, and ultrastructure of specialisedskin glands, biochemistry oftheirsecretions have been collected. Osmetrichia, scent marking patterns and fiehmen responses and their hormonal control have been elucidated. The neuroendocrinological basis of scent marking has been made explicit. Relatively only very few of the mammalian pheromones have been isolated.
    [Show full text]
  • Finding Useful Concepts of Representation in Cognitive Neuroscience: a New Tactic for Addressing Dynamical Critiques of Representational Models of Cognition, Action, and Perception
    Finding Useful Concepts of Representation in Cognitive Neuroscience: A new tactic for addressing dynamical critiques of representational models of cognition, action and perception A dissertation submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Cincinnati in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Philosophy of the McMicken College of Arts and Science by Jonathan Martin 1 Abstract Since Timothy van Gelder's 1995 paper “What Might Cognition be if not Computation?” there has been a growing distrust of representational accounts of cognition, most notably from proponents of relatively new programs like dynamical systems theory and ecological/embodied approaches to cognition. Some advocates of these programs have argued that the concept of “representation” is deeply flawed – does little explanatory work, prejudices what is researched and how results are interpreted, and loses sight of the complex interactions that hold between agents and their environments. In this dissertation, I will not try to challenge these claims. Instead, I will argue that responses from defenders of representational modeling often fail to adequately meet these challenges, underestimate their insights, and have a tendency to react by redefining “representation” in such a way that it loses its explanatory significance. Although I believe that the ultimate shape and utility of representational models of cognition will only be revealed in the course of doing science, I will advocate a new tack for defending representational modeling. I begin by acknowledging that representational terminology is indeed used inconsistently, and sometimes without warrant or explanatory power. Next, I examine cases from the cognitive neuroscience to try to show that we can distinguish between explanations whose representational features are closely tied to their explanatory ambitions and those that are not.
    [Show full text]