The Democracy of the Genes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Democracy of the Genes news and views aged adults, the discrepancy had been thought to reflect age-related increases in The democracy of the heritability of IQ. This hypothesis is supported by research on octogenarian twins published earlier this year6, as well as by the genes studies of other familial pairings7. Devlin et al. offer, and provide statistical support for, an alternative account of this Matt McGue anomaly. Specifically, they hypothesize that The genetic heritability of IQ remains highly contentious. A new analysis failure to consider the shared prenatal envi- shows that genetic influences may be weaker, and prenatal ronment of twins results in an overestima- environmental influences greater, than previously appreciated. tion of IQ heritability in studies of twins reared apart, but would not bias heritability ir Francis Galton’s contributions in distinguish additive gene effects, which are estimates in studies of twins reared together, 1869 alone were enough to ensure that independent of genetic background and thus where shared prenatal effects are cancelled Shis scientific legacy would be a lasting shared by parents and their offspring, from out in the comparison of monozygotic one. In that year, he not only helped in non-additive gene effects, which are depen- and dizygotic twin similarity. When shared founding this journal, but also published the dent on genetic background and thus not prenatal environments were included in the first empirical investigation of the inheri- shared by parents and offspring. Narrow- statistical model that Devlin et al. fitted to tance of human achievementl, a study that is sense heritability is the proportion of a the IQ correlations, they accounted for 20% generally credited with framing the modern trait’s variance that is attributable to additive of IQ similarity among twins but only 5% nature–nurture debate. On page 468 of this gene effects; broad-sense heritability is the among non-twin siblings. The former esti- issue2, Devlin, Daniels and Roeder report a proportion attributable to both additive mate is particularly remarkable given that statistical analysis of more than 200 familial and non-additive gene effects. twins, and especially monozygotic twins, can IQ correlations, the most recent in a long Herrnstein and Murray4 based their argu- experience radically different intrauterine line of empirical investigations aimed at ments on an IQ heritability of 60%; Devlin et environments even though they share the resolving issues that were raised by Galton. al. report a broad-sense heritability estimate womb at the same time8. Because it is associated with a wide range of 48% and a narrow-sense heritability of Devlin and colleagues’ report supports of social effects, including educational and only 34%. Although a narrow-sense heri- the view that the main environmental influ- occupational success, poverty and even tability of this magnitude does not preclude ences on IQ occur early in life. If indeed they delinquency3, IQ has long been of interest to dysgenic trends (albeit at a slower rate than do, improved cognitive functioning might behavioural and social scientists. But these Herrnstein and Murray projected), it is cer- be an unexpected benefit of public health associations have also challenged cherished tainly too low to support the establishment of initiatives aimed at improving maternal beliefs about the nature of social achieve- a high-IQ caste. Over the long term, unless nutrition and reducing prenatal exposure to ment. If social status is influenced by IQ, both the narrow-sense heritability and the toxins. Nonetheless, it is important to recog- which is in turn substantially inherited, then rate of intermarriage is very high, genes for nize that the evidence of Devlin et al., like social standing will, in part, be a function of quantitative traits such as IQ are essentially that supporting the influence of early intel- one’s genetic endowment. Hard work may be democratic; by the third or fourth genera- lectual stimulation on synapse formation no guarantee of success, unless one has also tion, descendants of gifted individuals are and subsequent human intellectual perfor- received a lucky draw in the genetic lottery. not much more likely to be gifted than are mance, is indirect and as yet unreplicated. The general uneasiness over IQ reached descendants of ordinary people. Caution is certainly warranted. In large- new heights with the publication of The Bell Although their paper is entitled “The scale studies where pre- or perinatal influ- Curve4, by Herrnstein and Murray, in 1994. heritability of IQ”, Devlin and colleagues’ ences on IQ have been assessed directly, little The authors described declines in the popu- most important finding probably concerns evidence for any strong effect has been lation’s genetic potential for high IQ (that is, the nurture rather than nature of IQ. Behav- found9. Perhaps this early work lacked dysgenic trends), because of higher fecun- ioural geneticists had previously pointed out precise assessments of prenatal exposures, or dity among the poorly educated relative to an inconsistency in the familial IQ correla- perhaps the effect of any single prenatal the well educated; and they also forecast the tions. From data on twins that have been factor is relatively minor and thus difficult to establishment of a caste-like cognitive élite, reared together, the heritability of IQ is detect. Even though it does not establish the which would be maintained through inter- estimated to be about 50%; but from twins effect of any specific prenatal factor, Devlin marriage and the (genetic) transmission of reared apart, it is estimated at about 70% (ref. and colleagues’ statistical analysis implies high IQs from parents to offspring. In short, 5). Because IQ correlations for twins reared that a relatively sizable portion of IQ vari- they argued that Western (and in particular together are based primarily on adolescents ability can be attributed to the aggregate US) society was becoming a two-class or children, whereas correlations for twins effect of the prenatal environment, and so system, where the cognitively limited masses reared apart are based primarily on middle- provides a rationale for reconsidering these would be ruled by a relatively small and earlier studies. reproductively isolated cognitive élite. That the IQ debate now centres on Devlin and colleagues’ findings will lead whether IQ is 50% or 70% heritable is a to a reconsideration of these most dire con- That the debate now remarkable indication of how the nature– clusions from The Bell Curve. The likelihood nurture debate has shifted over the past two of dysgenic trends occurring, as well as the centres on whether IQ is decades. The anti-hereditarian position that ability of parents genetically to reproduce 50% or 70% heritable is a there are no genetic influences on IQ has desired traits in their children, both depend crumbled for want of any empirical data that on the strength of parent–offspring trans- remarkable indication of would support such a radical view. Equally mission, which in turn depends on the how the nature–nurture remarkable is the increasingly dominant narrow-sense, rather than the broad-sense view that the major environmental influ- (total) heritability. Quantitative geneticists question has shifted ences on IQ occur within the first few years of NATURE VOL 388 31 JULY 1997 | | Nature © Macmillan Publishers Ltd 1997 417 news and views life, or in the womb, and directly affect the 1. Galton, F. Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry into its Laws and bonate production peaked in subpolar and development of the brain. Research on the Consequences (Macmillan, London, 1869). subtropical oceans (R. Schneider, Univ. 2. Devlin, B., Daniels, M. & Roeder, K. Nature 388, 468–471 nature and nurture of IQ is converging on (1997). Bremen), reflecting a temperature-related the view that human intellectual ability has 3. Neisser, U. et al. Am. Psychol. 51, 77–101 (1996). shift in plankton ecology from diatoms to a strong, but malleable, biological basis — a 4. Herrnstein, R. J. & Murray, C. The Bell Curve: Intelligence and calcareous plankton. Class Structure in American Life (Free Press, New York, 1994). convergence that Galton would, no doubt, 5. Plomin, R. & Loehlin, J. C. Behav. Genet. 19, 331–342 (1989). In shallow-water environments, the have found quite congenial. 6. McClearn, G. E. et al. Science 276, 1560–1563 (1997). evolution of several major reef systems (such Matt McGue is in the Department of Psychology 7. McGue, M., Bouchard, T. J. Jr, Iacono, W. G. & Lykken, D. T. in Nature, Nurture and Psychology (eds Plomin, R. & McClearn, as the Great Barrier Reef) accompanied and the Institute of Human Genetics, 75 East River G. E.) 59–76 (Am. Psychol. Assoc., Washington DC, 1993). increased reef carbonate production (A. Road, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, 8. Price, B. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2, 293–352 (1950). Droxler, Rice Univ.; P. Davies, Sydney Univ.). 9. Broman, S. H., Nichols, P. L. & Kennedy, W. A. Preschool IQ: Minnesota 55455, USA. Prenatal and Early Developmental Correlates (Erlbaum, The production of carbonate in continental e-mail: [email protected] Hillsdale, NJ, 1975). shelves and in mid-latitude open-ocean environments may partly explain the Palaeoclimatology extreme dissolution of carbonate sediments during MIS 11 throughout ocean basins A warm future in the past such as the Indian and Pacific3. (In the oceans’ carbonate cycle, biological forma- William R. Howard tion of hard body parts from carbonate out- strips river input of the component ions, and o understand the impact of a possibly followed by early returns to glacial conditions dissolution from the seabed makes up the warmer future climate, geologists are (Fig. 1). difference. In other words, to produce more Tsearching the past for warmer-than- Many other aspects of the climate were carbonate in one place you must dissolve present interglacial intervals — examples of strange. In deep-sea records of MIS 11, sub- more elsewhere.) what we may expect in a world with more polar waters in both hemispheres were at Perhaps barrier-reef tracts were estab- greenhouse gases than ours1.
Recommended publications
  • Cognitive Ability and Fertility Amongst Swedish Men. Evidence from 18 Cohorts of Military Conscription
    Max-Planck-Institut für demografische Forschung Max Planck Institute for Demographic Research Konrad-Zuse-Strasse 1 D-18057 Rostock Germany Tel +49 (0) 3 81 20 81 - 0 Fax +49 (0) 3 81 20 81 - 202 www.demogr.mpg.de MPIDR Working Paper WP 2017-020 l December 2017 Cognitive ability and fertility amongst Swedish men. Evidence from 18 cohorts of military conscription Martin Kolk l [email protected] Kieron Barclay l [email protected] © Copyright is held by the authors. Working papers of the Max Planck Institute for Demographic Research receive only limited review. Views or opinions expressed in working papers are attributable to the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the Institute. Cognitive ability and fertility amongst Swedish men. Evidence from 18 cohorts of military conscription Martin Kolk1 and Kieron Barclay2 Abstract: We examine the relationship between cognitive ability and childbearing patterns in contemporary Sweden using administrative register data. The topic has a long history in the social sciences and has been the topic of a large number of studies, many arguing for a negative gradient between intelligence and fertility. We link fertility histories to military conscription tests with intelligences scores for all Swedish born men born 1951 to 1967. We find an overall positive relationship between intelligence scores and fertility and that is consistent across our cohorts. The relationship is most pronounced for transition to a first child, and that men with the lowest categories of IQ-scores have the fewest children. Using fixed effects models we additionally control for all factors that are shared across siblings, and after such adjustments we find a stronger positive relationship between IQ and fertility.
    [Show full text]
  • Intelligence, Genetics Of: Heritability and Causation
    Intelligence, Genetics of: Heritability and Causation a risk factor for Alzheimers-disease. Psychological Medicine 25: 223–9 Skodak M, Skeels H M 1949 A final follow-up on one hundred adopted children. Journal of Genetic Psychology 75: 84–125 Wilson R S 1983 The Louisville Twin Study: Developmental synchronies in behavior. Child Deelopment 54: 298–316 R. Plomin Intelligence, Genetics of: Heritability and Causation In theory, the ‘genetics of intelligence’ describes the extent to which, and manner by which, an individual’s Figure 1 mental abilities are influenced by his or her genes. In The frequency distribution for an idealized IQ test. practice, there is a lot of debate and no universally Raw scores from a test are rescaled to fit the shape of a accepted description of the genetics of intelligence. normal curve with mean at 100 and standard deviation Intelligence is regarded as a complex trait, meaning of 15. Consequently, when the test is devised, 95 that many genetic and environmental factors are percent of the population will have IQ scores that fall thought to influence it. Nevertheless, we know little between 70 and 130 by construction about the number or nature of genes that influence cognitive abilities. We know even less about how the different aspects of intelligence interact with one expression of these genes depends on other genes or on another and that these interactions generate the environmental factors. To complicate matters even observed correlations (Hunt 1997). For example, further, there are numerous facets of intelligence, each strength in one area of intelligence (e.g., memory) of which may be influenced in different ways by genes might compensate for weakness in another area (e.g., and the environment.
    [Show full text]
  • S Intelligence?
    Philosophical Psychology, 2018 VOL. 31, NO. 2, 232–252 https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2017.1401706 What Kind of Kind IIss Intelligence? Davide Serpico Department of Classics, Philosophy and History, University of Genoa, Italy AbstractAbstract: The model of human intelligence that is most widely adopted derives from psychometrics and behavioral genetics. This standard approach conceives intelligence as a general cognitive ability that is genetically highly heritable and describable using quantitative traits analysis. The paper analyzes intelligence within the debate on natural kinds and contends that the general intelligence conceptualization does not carve psychological nature at its joints. Moreover, I argue that this model assumes an essentialist perspective. As an alternative, I consider an HPC theory of intelligence and evaluate how it deals with essentialism and with intuitions coming from cognitive science. Finally, I highlight some concerns about the HPC model as well, and conclude by suggesting that it is unnecessary to treat intelligence as a kind in any sense. Keywords : Intelligence; IQ; g Factor; Natural Kinds; Homeostatic Property Cluster; Essentialism; Psychometrics; Behavioral Genetics; Heritability Introduction Although the concept of intelligence is shrouded in controversy, it is nonetheless widely used both in psychological and in folk settings. Several theories have been proposed in order to clarify whether intelligence can be conceived as a general ability or only as a bundle of distinct cognitive phenomena. Several criticisms have been raised against a strong interpretation of genetic data based on the well-known intelligence quotient (IQ). Several attempts have been made to find more comprehensive definitions of intelligence. After a century of research, there is still extensive debate going on about the status of intelligence.
    [Show full text]
  • Future Efforts in Flynn Effect Research: Balancing Reductionism with Holism
    J. Intell. 2014, 2, 122-155; doi:10.3390/jintelligence2040122 OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Article Future Efforts in Flynn Effect Research: Balancing Reductionism with Holism Michael A. Mingroni Newark, Delaware, USA; E-Mail: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-302-753-3533 External Editor: Joseph L. Rodgers Received: 13 February 2014: in revised form 2 October 2014 / Accepted: 2 October 2014 / Published: 15 October 2014 Abstract: After nearly thirty years of concerted effort by many investigators, the cause or causes of the secular gains in IQ test scores, known as the Flynn effect, remain elusive. In this target article, I offer six suggestions as to how we might proceed in our efforts to solve this intractable mystery. The suggestions are as follows: (1) compare parents to children; (2) consider other traits and conditions; (3) compare siblings; (4) conduct more and better intervention programs; (5) use subtest profile data in context; and (6) quantify the potential contribution of heterosis. This last section contains new simulations of the process of heterosis, which provide a plausible scenario whereby rapid secular changes in multiple genetically influenced traits are possible. If there is any theme to the present paper, it is that future study designs should be simpler and more highly focused, coordinating multiple studies on single populations. Keywords: Flynn effect; intelligence; secular trend; heterosis 1. Introduction It has been nearly thirty years since James Flynn brought widespread attention to the occurrence of rapid gains in IQ test scores [1,2]. However, in honestly assessing the situation today one would have to conclude that we are not much closer to identifying the cause than we were three decades ago.
    [Show full text]
  • Hans Eysenck's Theory of Intelligence, and What It Reveals About Him
    Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 116–127 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Hans Eysenck's theory of intelligence, and what it reveals about him Linda S. Gottfredson School of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA article info abstract Article history: Hans Eysenck was a highly analytical, objective, independent-minded experimentalist. He personified the biolog- Received 18 March 2016 ical perspective of the Galton–Spearman ‘London School of Psychology’, which he led for many decades. His first Accepted 6 April 2016 (1939) and last publications (1998) were on intelligence. Returning to the topic in the 1960s, he formulated, tested, and promulgated the theory that general intelligence (g) is a biological phenomenon with broad social Keywords: consequences. I examine the status of Eysenck's theory, advances in the field, and social reactions to them during Hans J. Eysenck – – London School of Psychology the 1960s 1970s, 1980s 1990s, and since 2000. My perspective is that of a sociologist who, in testing alternative Intelligence theories of social inequality, was drawn inexorably into the intelligence literature, policy debates over fairness in g factor employee selection, and first-hand observation of the sort of controversies he experienced. Eysenck's 1979 and Intelligence theory 1998 textbooks on intelligence mark developments in his theory and supporting evidence during the first two Biological psychology periods. They exhibit considerable knowledge about the philosophy and history of science, and the nature of sci- Scientificcontroversy entific controversy. Advances in intelligence since 2000, in particular, from neuroimaging and molecular genetics, Social inequality vindicate his biological perspective.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Ability William T
    Forthcoming in The New Palgrave Dictionary of Economics, Steve Durlauf ed. Cognitive Ability William T. Dickens Senior Fellow The Brookings Institution ABSTRACT Modern psychological theory views cognitive ability as multidimensional while acknowledging that the many different abilities are themselves positively correlated. This positive correlation across abilities has led most psychometricians to accept the reality of a general cognitive ability that is reflected in the full scale score on major tests of cognitive ability or IQ. This entry provides an introduction to the history of cognitive testing and some of its major controversies. Evidence supporting the validity of measures of cognitive ability is presented and the nature and implications of group differences is discussed along with evidence on its malleability. Some people obviously and consistently understand new concepts quicker, solve unfamiliar problems faster, see relationships that others don’t and are more knowledgeable about a wider range of topics than others. We call such people smart, bright, quick, or intelligent. Psychologists have developed tests to measure this trait. Originally called IQ tests (for Intelligence Quotient because the measures were constructed as the ratio of mental age to chronological age multiplied by 100) that name has fallen out of favor. Instead, such tests are now often referred to as tests of cognitive ability. Although the term IQ is still sometimes used to refer to what such tests measure, none construct a ratio. History Spearman (1904) first popularized the observation that individuals who do well on one type of mental task also tend to do well on many others. For example, people who are good at recognizing patterns in sequences of abstract drawings are also good at quickly arranging pictures in order to tell a story, telling what three dimensional shapes drawn in two dimensions will look like when rotated, tend to have large vocabularies and good reading comprehension, and are quick at arithmetic.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic Foundations of Human Intelligence
    Edinburgh Research Explorer Genetic foundations of human intelligence Citation for published version: Deary, IJ, Johnson, W & Houlihan, LM 2009, 'Genetic foundations of human intelligence', Human Genetics, vol. 126, no. 1, pp. 215-232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Human Genetics Publisher Rights Statement: Deary, I. J., Johnson, W. & Houlihan, L. M. Jul 2009, "Genetic foundations of human intelligence", in Human Genetics. 126, 1, p. 215-232.© Springer-Verlag, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4. The final publication is available at link.springer.com General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 24. Sep. 2021 Genetic foundations of human intelligence I. J. Deary, W. Johnson, L. M. Houlihan I. J. Deary, W. Johnson, L. M. Houlihan Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, Scotland, UK.
    [Show full text]
  • The Wilson Effect: the Increase in Heritability of IQ with Age
    Twin Research and Human Genetics Volume 16 Number 5 pp. 923–930 C The Authors 2013 doi:10.1017/thg.2013.54 The Wilson Effect: The Increase in Heritability of IQ With Age Thomas J. Bouchard Jr. Psychology Department, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, USA Ronald Wilson presented the first clear and compelling evidence that the heritability of IQ increases with age. We propose to call the phenomenon ‘The Wilson Effect’ and we document the effect diagrammatically with key twin and adoption studies, including twins reared apart, that have been carried out at various ages and in a large number of different settings. The results show that the heritability of IQ reaches an asymptote at about 0.80 at 18–20 years of age and continuing at that level well into adulthood. In the aggregate, the studies also confirm that shared environmental influence decreases across age, approximating about 0.10 at 18–20 years of age and continuing at that level into adulthood. These conclusions apply to the Westernized industrial democracies in which most of the studies have been carried out. Keywords IQ, age, intelligence, heritability, twins, adoptees Monozygotic twins became increasingly concordant Figure 1 from Wilson (1983) is a plot of kin correlations with age and also paralleled each other for the spurts across age. The data were gathered by the justly famous and lags in development. Dizygotic twins became less Louisville Longitudinal Twin Study. The plot illustrates the concordant with age and eventually matched their sin- changes in kin correlations that mediate the effect; rela- gleton siblings as closely as one another.
    [Show full text]
  • The General Intelligence Factor Exploring Intelligence 25 Copyright 1998 Scientific American, Inc
    Despite some popular The General assertions, a single factor for intelligence, called g, can be measured with IQ Intelligence tests and does predict Factor success in life by Linda S. Gottfredson No subject in psychology has pro- downplayed or ignored. This misrepresen- mental tests are often designed to mea- voked more intense public controversy tation reflects a clash between a deeply sure specific domains of cognition—ver- than the study of human intelligence. felt ideal and a stubborn reality. The ideal, bal fluency, say, or mathematical skill, From its beginning, research on how implicit in many popular critiques of spatial visualization or memory—people and why people differ in overall mental intelligence research, is that all people are who do well on one kind of test tend to ability has fallen prey to political and born equally able and that social inequali- do well on the others, and people who social agendas that obscure or distort ty results only from the exercise of unjust do poorly generally do so across the even the most well-established scientific privilege. The reality is that Mother board. This overlap, or intercorrelation, findings. Journalists, too, often present a Nature is no egalitarian. People are in fact suggests that all such tests measure view of intelligence research that is unequal in intellectual potential—and some global element of intellectual abil- exactly the opposite of what most intel- they are born that way, just as they are ity as well as specific cognitive skills. In ligence experts believe. For these and born with different potentials for height, recent decades, psychologists have other reasons, public understanding of physical attractiveness, artistic flair, ath- devoted much effort to isolating that intelligence falls far short of public con- letic prowess and other traits.
    [Show full text]
  • Dysgenic Fertility for Intelligence and Education in Taiwan
    Intelligence 63 (2017) 29–32 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/intell Dysgenic fertility for intelligence and education in Taiwan ⁎ MARK Hsin-Yi Chena, , Yung-Hua Chenb, Yung-Kun Liaoc, Hsin-Ping Chend, Richard Lynne a Department of Special Education, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan, ROC b Chinese Behavioral Science Corporation, Taiwan, ROC c Department of Special Education, National Dong-Hwa University, Hualien, Taiwan, ROC d Department of Economics, National Chengchi University, Taipei, Taiwan, ROC e University of Ulster, Coleraine, Ireland ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: This study investigated the current trend of dysgenic fertility in Taiwan. Data on 680 adults aged 35 to 90 years Taiwan from the Taiwan WAIS-IV norming sample and 980 children aged 2.5 to 7 years from the Taiwan WPPSI-IV Dysgenic fertility norming sample were examined to investigate the relationships between intelligence, education, and fertility. IQ Results revealed that education and intelligence were negatively correlated with fertility, and that the WAIS-IV correlations were stronger for females. The genotypic intelligence is estimated to decline by approximately WPPSI-IV 1.19 IQ points per generation and the decline is much stronger for the younger adult cohort (1.46 IQ points) than for the older adult cohort (1.02 IQ points). 1. Introduction Ginsburg, & Coe, 2007; Sundet, Barlaug, & Torjussen, 2004; Teasdale & Owen, 2000, 2005, 2008; Woodley & Meisenberg, 2013). The average Eighty years ago, Runquist (1936) reported a secular increase in intelligence in a number of economically developed countries is intelligence. In the decades that followed, numerous studies reported declining (Lynn, 2011; Meisenberg & Woodley, 2013).
    [Show full text]
  • Taking a Balanced View of IQ
    news and views the possibility of mental processes that are not strictly reducible to brain processes. Symmetry as In between are the colonizers from cogni- tive psychology, who betray their insecurity by adopting the term ‘cognitive scientists’. destiny — taking a They propose to explain away — rather than to explain — the brute facts of variabili- ty in cognitive capacity. They supply the algorithms for solving problems which keep balanced view of IQ the sky pilots on their toes. For about 825 years they have been promising to account for variability in IQ in terms of cognitive- Steve Blinkhorn processing models. (The first positive results Genetic vulnerability during development may be responsible for are still eagerly awaited.) environmental effects on intelligence. Taken with a study showing that Results, processes, substratum — the the heritability of IQ does not change with age, could these results mean three foci of interest that divide those who that nature and nurture are no longer in opposition? study human intelligence. But somewhere in there is a mind–brain barrier, a conceptual he study of human intelligence has of heredity should become proportionately and philosophical divide that stands in the always had its feet in biology and its less. But if the Swedish study is to be believed, way of progress. The artificial-intelligence Thead in the clouds — the recent environmental influences on IQ have their and psychometric researchers may set prob- encounter between Gary Kasparov and effects early. This is hardly controversial lems and recognize the results as showing the Deeper Blue showed what can be achieved in stuff and, indeed, potential factors (such as external hallmarks of intelligence, but they confronting human intelligence with meth- atmospheric lead, maternal nutrition and have little to say about how to make people ods that have no plausible claim to mimic its fetal alcohol syndrome), which indicate a more intelligent, or overcome handicaps to cognitive processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Review of the Evidence: Intelligence
    Chapter 7 Review of the evidence: intelligence Genetics and human behaviour: the ethical context CHAPTER 7 Review of the evidence: intelligence1 Background 7.1 The first tests that aimed to measure intelligence were developed at the start of the twentieth century. The Simon–Binet test was commissioned by the French government to REVIEW OF THE EVIDENCE: INTELLIGENCE identify children who would not benefit from ordinary schooling because of low intelligence. It was subsequently adapted by researchers in the US to measure average and above-average intelligence as well. Other tests have been developed in different contexts than education, for example in military recruitment. The use of such tests has often aroused controversy and criticism, particularly when combined with claims about the biological basis of variation in test scores. There have been several prominent examples of claims that differences in IQ between racial groups are due to genetic factors rather than social or environmental ones and, further, that this ought to inform social policy. While it is often claimed that on average, black individuals score slightly lower on IQ tests than white individuals, who in turn score lower than people from East Asia, there are also studies which show that, if black individuals and white individuals are closely matched on socioeconomic status, the differences in IQ are substantially reduced.2 The potential abuse of information about genetic influences on behaviour is discussed further in Chapters 13- 15. Here, it suffices to observe that research into genetic influences on intelligence has been associated, historically, with significant concerns about misuse of the information and unfair discrimination.
    [Show full text]