EMDR and the Anxiety Disorders: Exploring the Current Status

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

EMDR and the Anxiety Disorders: Exploring the Current Status EMDR and the Anxiety Disorders: Exploring the Current Status Ad de Jongh University of Amsterdam and Vrije Universiteit, The Netherlands Erik ten Broeke Visie, Deventer, The Netherlands Based on the assumptions of Shapiro’s adaptive information-processing model, it could be argued that a large proportion of people suffering from an anxiety disorder would benefi t from eye movement desen- sitization and reprocessing (EMDR). This article provides an overview of the current empirical evidence on the application of EMDR for the anxiety disorders spectrum other than posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Reviewing the existing literature, it is disappointing to fi nd that 20 years after its introduction, support for the effi cacy of EMDR for other conditions than PTSD is still scarce. Randomized outcome research is limited to panic disorder with agoraphobia and spider phobia. The results suggest that EMDR is generally more effective than no-treatment control conditions or nonspecifi c interventions but less ef- fective than existing evidence-based (i.e., exposure-based) interventions. However, since these studies were based on incomplete protocols and limited treatment courses, questions about the relative effi cacy of EMDR for the treatment of anxiety disorders remain largely unanswered. Keywords: eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR); anxiety disorders; specifi c phobia; panic disorder he past three decades have witnessed signifi - At the end of the 1980s, the fi rst publications of a cant advances and growth in research on anxi- newly developed method, eye movement desensitiza- ety disorders (Boschen, 2008). Growth has T tion and reprocessing (EMDR), emerged. This began been strong for its treatment for which a wide array with Shapiro’s (1989) study on the effects of EMDR of behavioral and cognitive interventions have been on a single traumatic memory. After its publication, proposed and investigated. With regard to the effec- it took years before the fi rst randomized clinical trial tiveness of these procedures, the results of a recent was published that supported the notion that EMDR comprehensive meta-analysis demonstrated not only was capable of treating full-blown posttraumatic stress that cognitive behavioral interventions are particu- disorder (PTSD) within a limited number of sessions larly effi cacious but also that there are marked differ- (Wilson, Becker, & Tinker, 1995). In the years there- ences in treatment responsiveness among the different after, many other studies followed, and now, 20 years types of anxiety disorders (Hofmann & Smits, 2008). after the fi rst EMDR publication, there is enough evi- Treatment effects appear particularly large for acute dence to conclude that the method is an established, stress disorder (ASD) and obsessive-compulsive dis- time-limited treatment for PTSD (Bisson et al., 2007). orders (OCDs) but are less strong for panic disorder Based on the notion that EMDR is capable of re- and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). The authors solving disturbing memories of traumatic events that conclude that there is still considerable room for are critical in the development and maintenance of improvement in the treatment of anxiety disorders PTSD, it could be asserted that other types of anxiety (Hofmann & Smits, 2008). disorders that developed following a distressing event Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 3, Number 3, 2009 133 © 2009 EMDR International Association DOI: 10.1891/1933-3196.3.3.133 would also be responsive to EMDR. This notion is at From a theoretical point of view, EMDR could play the basis of Shapiro’s adaptive information-processing a role in the treatment of panic disorder, as there are (AIP) model, a framework that is considered to be indications that panic disorder and agoraphobia often helpful to therapists when developing a problem start after a stressful life event (Kleiner & Marshall, formulation in terms of a relationship between 1987). In addition, there is evidence to suggest that memories of disturbing events and clients’ current many panic patients suffer from PTSD-like symptoms anxiety symptoms and the use of EMDR for resolv- as a result of their fi rst panic attack (McNally & Lukach, ing these memories (Solomon & Shapiro, 2008). 1992). Indeed, since the development of EMDR, a large Beside incidental reports of therapists suggesting number of articles have appeared, suggesting a posi- that EMDR can be successfully applied to panic disor- tive effect of EMDR on the symptoms of individuals der with agoraphobia (de Jongh & ten Broeke, 1996; suffering from various anxiety-related conditions. Be- Fernandez & Faretta, 2007), not many studies have cause anxiety symptoms are common in patients who been published, and those that have been conducted use primary and secondary mental health services, it were carried out by the same research group (Feske & is important to evaluate the potential of EMDR as a Goldstein, 1997; Goldstein, de Beurs, Chambless, & contribution to the treatment of anxiety disorders. Wilson, 2001; Goldstein & Feske, 1994). Their fi rst This article provides an overview of the current publication that concerned a series of seven persons empirical evidence on the application of EMDR for diagnosed with panic disorder yielded promising anxiety disorders beyond the typical traumatically results (Goldstein & Feske, 1994). It was found that induced conditions like PTSD and ASD. First, the most patients benefi ted signifi cantly from EMDR, key symptoms of each condition within the spectrum with reductions in number of panic attacks, sever- of anxiety disorders (i.e., panic disorder with and with- ity of anticipatory anxiety, and occurrence of general out agoraphobia, social phobia, specifi c phobia, OCD, symptoms of distress. In a subsequent randomized and GAD) are initially reviewed, as are the assumptions controlled study of the same researchers using a simi- as to why this condition may be suitable for the appli- lar treatment protocol, the effectiveness of EMDR cation of EMDR. Next, the available research is sum- was compared with a protocol in which the eye move- marized. Finally, some general conclusions are drawn. ments were omitted and a wait-list condition (Feske & Goldstein, 1997; see Table 1). EMDR appeared to be Panic Disorder With or Without more effective than both no-treatment and an EMDR Agoraphobia protocol, in which the eye movements were lacking, on a number of self-report measures. However, at The central clinical features of panic disorder are the the 3-month follow-up, these differences could not be panic attacks, the associated feelings of losing con- maintained. Four years later, the same research group trol, and the fact that these are recurrent (American published another randomized clinical trial for panic Psychiatric Association, 2000). Many panic patients disorder with agoraphobia (Goldstein et al., 2001) in suffer from agoraphobia, as they tend to avoid situa- which the effectiveness of EMDR was compared with tions from which escape might be diffi cult or in which a relaxation condition used as an attention-placebo help may not be available in the event of having a control condition (association and relaxation therapy panic attack (e.g., being outside the home alone). [ART]). The use of ART in this study appeared to be Strictly speaking, there is no indication for EMDR very similar to EMDR with the exception of EMDR’s in the treatment of panic disorder because there are distraction/bilateral stimulation component. Patients already evidence-based psychological interventions in the ART condition received 30 to 45 minutes of available with cognitive-behavioral therapy being the progressive muscle relaxation training, after which most important. A number of treatment approaches they were asked to describe the scene of their most have been established in controlled trials as being frightening panic-related memory in detail, to close effective, including exposure treatment, reduction their eyes, and to speak out loud as they followed of avoidance, and cognitive treatments focusing on themselves to associate to this image (“let come up the modifi cation of panic attacks (National Institute whatever comes up”) for about 45 to 60 minutes of for Clinical Excellence, 2004). However, the long- association. As to the results, it appeared that the cli- term outcome of these studies appears to be limited, ents in the EMDR condition did not fare better on any and the vast majority of panic disorder patients need of the measures than those who were asked to just prolonged additional treatment (van Balkom, de associate on the target memory. Conversely, EMDR Beurs, Koele, Lange, & van Dyck, 1996). appeared to be signifi cantly superior to the wait-list 134 Journal of EMDR Practice and Research, Volume 3, Number 3, 2009 de Jongh and ten Broeke TABLE 1. Randomized Clinical Trials Pertaining to EMDR’s Effectiveness on the Area of Anxiety Disorders No. (and duration of Sample sessions in Authors Year Treatment (n) Characteristics minutes) Outcome Variables Effects Panic Disorder With Agoraphobia Feske and 1997 1. EMDR (15) Panic disorder 1, 3, 4: fi vea a. Social concerns 1 > 2 (a–f ) Goldstein 2. Wait list (12) with almost all (one 120-minute and general 3 > 4 (b, d, f ) and having and four anxiety 3-month 3. EMDR (18) agoraphobia 90-minute b. Anticipated follow-up: 4. EMDR with sessions) panic and coping 1 = 2 eye movements c. Physical concerns 3 = 4 omitted (18) d. General anxiety and fear of panic e. Panic frequency f. Variety of secondary measures Goldstein, 2001 1. EMDR (18) Outpatients 1–2: four a. Panic/agorapho- 1 = 2 (a–d) de Beurs, 2. Attention- applying for (90-minute bia severity 1 > 3 (a–b) Chambless, placebo, ART treatment; panic sessions) b. Diary 1 = 3 (c–d) and Wilson (13) disorder with c. Frequency of Same results 3. Wait list (14) agoraphobia anxious for 1-month (based on cognitions follow-up Structured d. Frequency of Clinical panic attacks Interview for DSM Disorders) Specifi c Phobia Muris and 1997 1.
Recommended publications
  • SCHIZOPHRENIA and OTHER PSYCHOTIC DISORDERS of EARLY ONSET Jean Starling & Isabelle Feijo
    IACAPAP Textbook of Child and Adolescent Mental Health Chapter OTHER DISORDERS H.5 SCHIZOPHRENIA AND OTHER PSYCHOTIC DISORDERS OF EARLY ONSET Jean Starling & Isabelle Feijo Jean Starling FRANZCP, MPH Child and adolescent psychiatrist, Director, Walker Unit, Concord Centre for Mental Health, Sydney, and senior clinical lecturer, Discipline of Psychiatry, Sydney Medical School, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia Conflict of interest: none declared Isabelle Feijo FRANZCP Psychiatrist, Walker Unit, Concord Centre for Mental Health, Sydney, Australia and specialist in child and adolescent psychiatry and psychotherapy, Swiss Medical Association Conflict of interest: none declared Jackson Acknowledgement: thanks to Pollock; Polly Kwan who vetted the untitled. Cantonese websites This publication is intended for professionals training or practising in mental health and not for the general public. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the Editor or IACAPAP. This publication seeks to describe the best treatments and practices based on the scientific evidence available at the time of writing as evaluated by the authors and may change as a result of new research. Readers need to apply this knowledge to patients in accordance with the guidelines and laws of their country of practice. Some medications may not be available in some countries and readers should consult the specific drug information since not all dosages and unwanted effects are mentioned. Organizations, publications and websites are cited or linked to illustrate issues or as a source of further information. This does not mean that authors, the Editor or IACAPAP endorse their content or recommendations, which should be critically assessed by the reader.
    [Show full text]
  • “What About Bob?” an Analysis of Gendered Mental Illness in a Mainstream Film Comedy
    “What About Bob?” An Analysis of Gendered Mental Illness in a Mainstream Film Comedy A Thesis Presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in the College of Graduate Studies of Northeast Ohio Medical University. Anna Plummer M.D. Medical Ethics and Humanities 2020 Thesis Committee: Dr. Julie Aultman (Advisor) Dr. Rachel Bracken Brian Harrell Copyright Anna Plummer 2020 ABSTRACT Mental illness has been a subject of fictional film since the early 20th century and continues to be a popular trope in mainstream movies. Portrayals of affected individuals in movies tend to be inaccurate and largely stigmatizing, negatively influencing public perception of mental illness. Recent research suggests that gender stereotypes and mental illness intersect, such that some mental illnesses are perceived as “masculine” and others as “feminine.” This notion may further stigmatize such disorders in individuals, as well as falsely inflate observed gender disparities in certain mental illnesses. Since gendered mental illness is a newly identified concept, little research has been performed exploring the way stereotypical gendered mental illness is depicted in mainstream film. This paper analyzes the movie What About Bob? to show that comedic film perpetuates stigma surrounding feminine mental illness in men and identifies the need for further study of gendered mental illness in movies to ascertain the effect such depictions have on the observed gender disparities in prevalence of certain mental disorders, as well as offers a proposal for coursework for film and medical students. i ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This paper would not have been possible without Dr. Aultman, whose teaching inspired me to pursue further education in Medical Ethics and Humanities, and whose guidance has been invaluable not only for this project, but also for addressing ethical issues in the clinic.
    [Show full text]
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • White Matter Abnormalities in Adults with Bipolar Disorder Type-II
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN White matter abnormalities in adults with bipolar disorder type‑II and unipolar depression Anna Manelis1*, Adriane Soehner1, Yaroslav O. Halchenko2, Skye Satz1, Rachel Ragozzino1, Mora Lucero1, Holly A. Swartz1, Mary L. Phillips1 & Amelia Versace1 Discerning distinct neurobiological characteristics of related mood disorders such as bipolar disorder type‑II (BD‑II) and unipolar depression (UD) is challenging due to overlapping symptoms and patterns of disruption in brain regions. More than 60% of individuals with UD experience subthreshold hypomanic symptoms such as elevated mood, irritability, and increased activity. Previous studies linked bipolar disorder to widespread white matter abnormalities. However, no published work has compared white matter microstructure in individuals with BD‑II vs. UD vs. healthy controls (HC), or examined the relationship between spectrum (dimensional) measures of hypomania and white matter microstructure across those individuals. This study aimed to examine fractional anisotropy (FA), radial difusivity (RD), axial difusivity (AD), and mean difusivity (MD) across BD‑II, UD, and HC groups in the white matter tracts identifed by the XTRACT tool in FSL. Individuals with BD‑II (n = 18), UD (n = 23), and HC (n = 24) underwent Difusion Weighted Imaging. The categorical approach revealed decreased FA and increased RD in BD‑II and UD vs. HC across multiple tracts. While BD‑II had signifcantly lower FA and higher RD values than UD in the anterior part of the left arcuate fasciculus, UD had signifcantly lower FA and higher RD values than BD‑II in the area of intersections between the right arcuate, inferior fronto‑occipital and uncinate fasciculi and forceps minor.
    [Show full text]
  • Characteristics of the Regulation of the Surprise Emotion Chuanlin Zhu1, Ping Li2, Zhao Zhang2, Dianzhi Liu1 & Wenbo Luo2,3
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Characteristics of the regulation of the surprise emotion Chuanlin Zhu1, Ping Li2, Zhao Zhang2, Dianzhi Liu1 & Wenbo Luo2,3 This study aimed to assess the characteristics of the regulation of the emotion of surprise. Event-related Received: 27 June 2017 potentials (ERPs) of college students when using cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression to Accepted: 11 April 2019 regulate their surprise level were recorded. Diferent contexts were presented to participants, followed Published: xx xx xxxx by the image of surprised facial expression; subsequently, using a 9-point scale, participants were asked to rate the intensity of their emotional experience. The behavioral results suggest that individuals’ surprise level could be reduced by using both expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal, in basic and complex conditions. The ERP results showed that (1) the N170 amplitudes were larger in complex than basic contexts, and those elicited by using expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal showed no signifcant diferences, suggesting that emotion regulation did not occur at this stage; (2) the LPC amplitudes elicited by using cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression were smaller than those elicited by free viewing in both context conditions, suggesting that the late stage of individuals’ processing of surprised faces was infuenced by emotion regulation. This study found that conscious emotional regulation occurred in the late stages when individuals processed surprise, and the regulation efects of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression were equivalent. When we pass an important test, some of us dance out of excitement; when we are informed about the sudden death of a loved one, we feel extremely sad.
    [Show full text]
  • Included Diagnosis List
    Press TAB to Diagnosis Diagnosis Description Code F20.0 Paranoid Schizophrenia F20.1 Disorganized Schizophrenia F20.2 Catatonic Schizophrenia F20.3 Undifferentiated Schizophrenia F20.5 Residual Schizophrenia F20.81 Schizophreniform Disorder F20.89 Other Schizophrenia F20.9 Schizophrenia, Unspecified F21 Schizotypal Disorder F22 Delusional Disorder F23 Brief Psychotic Disorder F24 Shared Psychotic Disorder F25.0 Schizoaffective Disorder, Bipolar Type F25.1 Schizoaffective Disorder, Depressive Type F25.8 Other Schizoaffective Disorders F25.9 Schizoaffective Disorder, Unspecified F28 Other Psychotic Disorder Not Due to a Substance or Known Physiological Condition F29 Unspecified Psychosis Not Due to a Substance or Known Physiological Condition F30.10 Manic Episode Without Psychotic Symptoms, Unspecified F30.11 Manic Episode Without Psychotic Symptoms, Mild F30.12 Manic Episode Without Psychotic Symptoms, Moderate F30.13 Manic Episode, Severe, Without Psychotic Symptoms F30.2 Manic Episode, Severe, With Psychotic Symptoms F30.3 Manic Episode in Partial Remission F30.4 Manic Episode in Full Remission F30.8 Other Manic Episodes F30.9 Manic Episode, Unspecified F31.0 Bipolar Disorder, Current Episode Hypomanic F31.10 Bipolar Disorder, Current Episode Manic, Without Psychotic features, Unspecified F31.11 Bipolar Disorder, Current Episode Manic, Without Psychotic Features, Mild F31.12 Bipolar Disorder, Current Episode Manic, Without Psychotic Features, Moderate F31.13 Bipolar Disorder, Current Episode Manic, Without Psychotic Features, Severe F31.2
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Classification Based on Biophysical Signals and Machine Learning Techniques
    S S symmetry Article Emotion Classification Based on Biophysical Signals and Machine Learning Techniques Oana Bălan 1,* , Gabriela Moise 2 , Livia Petrescu 3 , Alin Moldoveanu 1 , Marius Leordeanu 1 and Florica Moldoveanu 1 1 Faculty of Automatic Control and Computers, University POLITEHNICA of Bucharest, Bucharest 060042, Romania; [email protected] (A.M.); [email protected] (M.L.); fl[email protected] (F.M.) 2 Department of Computer Science, Information Technology, Mathematics and Physics (ITIMF), Petroleum-Gas University of Ploiesti, Ploiesti 100680, Romania; [email protected] 3 Faculty of Biology, University of Bucharest, Bucharest 030014, Romania; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +40722276571 Received: 12 November 2019; Accepted: 18 December 2019; Published: 20 December 2019 Abstract: Emotions constitute an indispensable component of our everyday life. They consist of conscious mental reactions towards objects or situations and are associated with various physiological, behavioral, and cognitive changes. In this paper, we propose a comparative analysis between different machine learning and deep learning techniques, with and without feature selection, for binarily classifying the six basic emotions, namely anger, disgust, fear, joy, sadness, and surprise, into two symmetrical categorical classes (emotion and no emotion), using the physiological recordings and subjective ratings of valence, arousal, and dominance from the DEAP (Dataset for Emotion Analysis using EEG, Physiological and Video Signals) database. The results showed that the maximum classification accuracies for each emotion were: anger: 98.02%, joy:100%, surprise: 96%, disgust: 95%, fear: 90.75%, and sadness: 90.08%. In the case of four emotions (anger, disgust, fear, and sadness), the classification accuracies were higher without feature selection.
    [Show full text]
  • Association Between Neuroticism and Risk of Incident Cardiovascular Disease in the UK Biobank Cohort
    Association between neuroticism and risk of incident cardiovascular disease in the UK Biobank cohort Master Degree Project in Systems Biology Two years Level, 120 ECTS Submitted by: Hira Shahid Email: [email protected] Supervisor: Helgi Schioth Co.supervisor: Gull Rukh Examiner: Diana Tilevik Master Degree Project in Systems Biology Abstract Myocardial infarction (MI) and stroke are the major causes of cardiovascular related morbidities and mortalities around the world. The prevalence of cardiovascular diseases has been increased in last decades and it is vital need of time to investigate this global problem with focus on risk population stratification. The aim of the present study is to investigate the association between individualized personality trait that is neuroticism and risk of MI and stroke has been investigated in a large population-based cohort of UK biobank.375,713 individuals (mean age: 56.24 ± 8.06) were investigated in this longitudinal study and were followed up for seven years to assess the association between neuroticism and risk of MI and stroke incidence. The neuroticism score was assessed by a 12-item questionnaire at baseline, while information related to MI and stroke events was either collected from hospital records and death registries or was self-reported by the participants. Cox proportional hazard regression adjusted for age, gender, BMI, socioeconomic status, lifestyle factors and medical histories for hypertension, diabetes and depression was used. All statistical analyses were performed using R software. In fully adjusted model, a one standard deviation increase in neuroticism score was associated with 1.05-fold increased risk for MI. (HR=1.047(1.009-1.087), p=0.015).
    [Show full text]
  • EMOTION CLASSIFICATION of COVERED FACES 1 WHO CAN READ YOUR FACIAL EXPRESSION? a Comparison of Humans and Machine Learning Class
    journal name manuscript No. (will be inserted by the editor) 1 A Comparison of Humans and Machine Learning 2 Classifiers Detecting Emotion from Faces of People 3 with Different Coverings 4 Harisu Abdullahi Shehu · Will N. 5 Browne · Hedwig Eisenbarth 6 7 Received: 9th August 2021 / Accepted: date 8 Abstract Partial face coverings such as sunglasses and face masks uninten- 9 tionally obscure facial expressions, causing a loss of accuracy when humans 10 and computer systems attempt to categorise emotion. Experimental Psychol- 11 ogy would benefit from understanding how computational systems differ from 12 humans when categorising emotions as automated recognition systems become 13 available. We investigated the impact of sunglasses and different face masks on 14 the ability to categorize emotional facial expressions in humans and computer 15 systems to directly compare accuracy in a naturalistic context. Computer sys- 16 tems, represented by the VGG19 deep learning algorithm, and humans assessed 17 images of people with varying emotional facial expressions and with four dif- 18 ferent types of coverings, i.e. unmasked, with a mask covering lower-face, a 19 partial mask with transparent mouth window, and with sunglasses. Further- Harisu Abdullahi Shehu (Corresponding author) School of Engineering and Computer Science, Victoria University of Wellington, 6012 Wellington E-mail: [email protected] ORCID: 0000-0002-9689-3290 Will N. Browne School of Electrical Engineering and Robotics, Queensland University of Technology, Bris- bane QLD 4000, Australia Tel: +61 45 2468 148 E-mail: [email protected] ORCID: 0000-0001-8979-2224 Hedwig Eisenbarth School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, 6012 Wellington Tel: +64 4 463 9541 E-mail: [email protected] ORCID: 0000-0002-0521-2630 2 Shehu H.
    [Show full text]
  • Common Pitfalls in ERP for OCD
    Common Pitfalls in ERP for OCD ©Justin K. Hughes, MA, LPC & Molly Martinez, PhD Thank you for being here sufferers, support, family, professionals. 2 Common Pitfalls in ERP for OCD ©Justin K. Hughes, MA, LPC & Molly Martinez, PhD HELLO! Justin Hughes, MA, LPC Molly Martinez, PhD Owner, Dallas Counseling, PLLC Clinical Psychologist Clinician, Writer, Speaker Specialists in OCD & Anxiety Recovery (SOAR) www.justinkhughes.com www.soartogether.net Dallas, TX Richardson, TX ©Justin K. Hughes, MA, LPC & Molly Martinez, PhD 4 Learning Objectives 1) Overview OCD and ERP 2) Identify roadblocks to effective ERP 3) Identify solutions to address these common pitfalls 5 Want these slides right now??? www.justinkhughes.com/ocd 6 PART ONE: Review The Basics You Probably Know... ● What OCD is ● What ERP is ● That ERP is the PART ONE: gold-standard of Review the evidence-based treatment Basics for OCD 8 For a PRIZE... What is the average amount of symptom reduction after a trial of ERP for OCD? 9 60-70%!! (Abramowitz & Jacoby, 2015; Foa et al., 2010) 10 You Might Know... ● Compulsions function by reducing distress via: ○ Reassurance PART ONE: ○ Avoidance Review the ● ERP is hard Basics ● ERP requires planning ● ERP requires adjustment ● ERP doesn’t always work as expected 11 PART TWO: ERP Pitfalls & Solutions ● Fear-Related Issues ○ Therapists’ Fears ○ Not Addressing the Core Fear ○ Clients’ Fear of Distress ● Covert Compulsive Behaviors ○ Reassurance ○ Mental Compulsions PART TWO: ○ Distraction ● Treatment Plan(ning) Problems ERP Pitfalls & ○ Treatment Compliance Solutions ○ Not Going Far Enough ○ Not Working with Family ○ Wrong Form of Exposure ○ Unrealistic Expectations (Extinction Burst)Medication Myths/Misconceptions ○ Detours & Comorbidities ○ Therapy “Dosing” ○ No Relapse Prevention Plan 13 Fear-Related Pitfalls & Solutions For a PRIZE..
    [Show full text]
  • Anxiety Disorders
    Anxiety Disorders Anxiety Disorders Overview n What is anxiety? n Categories of anxiety disorders n Generalized Anxiety Disorder n Panic Disorder n Specific Phobia/Social Phobia n Obsessive Compulsive Disorder n DSM-IV diagnoses n Treatments Anxiety n Probably experienced some of this during the exam n What does it feel like? n When is it a “disorder?” n What is the difference between fear and anxiety? n Anxiety defined as n uneasiness stemming from the anticipation of danger n Fear defined as n a reaction to a specific threat from the real, physical world 1 Anxiety n Anxiety is an evolutionary useful feeling n part of the flight or fight response n useful to have a response of energizing to get out of a situation n sometimes this gets in our way n feels not so adaptive Anxiety n Diagnosing n For a person to be diagnosed as having anxiety, the anxiety must be out of proportion to the perceived threat n The anxiety is recognized by the individual seeking treatment to be excessive or unreasonable Anxiety Disorders n Several types/different diagnoses n Based on formal (topographical) features n Should be distinct n Will see much overlap within a category (e.g. within Anxiety) n Also see overlap between other categories (e.g., OCD and eating disorders) 2 Anxiety Disorders n Six disorders: n Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) n Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) n Phobias n Panic disorder ________________________________ n Acute stress disorder n Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) GAD n One of the most
    [Show full text]
  • Surprise: a Belief Or an Emotion? in V.S
    Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This chapter was originally published in the book Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 202 published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who know you, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial From: Barbara Mellers, Katrina Fincher, Caitlin Drummond and Michelle Bigony, Surprise: A belief or an emotion? In V.S. Chandrasekhar Pammi and Narayanan Srinivasan, editors: Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 202, Amsterdam: The Netherlands, 2013, pp. 3-19. ISBN: 978-0-444-62604-2 © Copyright 2013 Elsevier B.V. Elsevier Author's personal copy CHAPTER Surprise: A belief or an emotion? 1 Barbara Mellers1, Katrina Fincher, Caitlin Drummond, Michelle Bigony Department of Psychology, Solomon Labs, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA 1Corresponding author. Tel.: þ1-215-7466540, Fax: þ1-215-8987301, e-mail address: [email protected] Abstract Surprise is a fundamental link between cognition and emotion. It is shaped by cognitive as- sessments of likelihood, intuition, and superstition, and it in turn shapes hedonic experiences.
    [Show full text]