The Phonetics-Phonology Interface: a Brief Orientation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Phonetics-Phonology Interface: a Brief Orientation Bruce Hayes LabPhon 7 UCLA 1 July, 2000 The Phonetics-Phonology Interface: A Brief Orientation 1. Primary Bifurcation • Phonetically guided phonological research • Phonologically-guided phonetic research PHONETICALLY GUIDED PHONOLOGICAL RESEARCH 2. Functionalist Perspective The sound patterns of language are an instance of “good design,” implementing principles of • Ease of articulation • Distinctness of contrasting forms in perception 3. Examples • Aerodynamic explanation of why various languages that allow geminates, and allow voiced obstruents, don’t allow geminate voiced obstruents (Ohala 1983). • Steriade’s (1993) explanation for why retroflexes are characteristically limited to postvocalic position: tongue-tip sliding during closure, with resulting superiority of VC formant transitions. 4. The “Who’s in Charge?” Question • What is the mechanism whereby phonological patterns are rendered phonetically natural? At least five answers (I-V): 5. I. “Accidents Plus Phonological Reinterpretation” (Ohala 1981) • Phonetic realization is essentially a physical process, with random (not teleological) variation. • Variation is misinterpreted and restructured within the phonological system, which is otherwise insensitive to phonetic principles. • Example: interpretation of coarticulation with /b/ in [bØ] as rounding, hence shift to [bu]. 6. II: Teleological Phonetics, Arbitrary Phonology (Blevins 1999) • The system of phonetic realization is exquisitely designed to balance considerations of articulation and perception (work of Bjorn Lindblom) The Phonetics-Phonology Interface: A Brief Orientation p. 2 • The phonological system is not “well designed” for phonetic naturalness, but is the result of historical restructuring of natural phonetic phenomena. (Blevins and Garrett 1998) • Example: the very strange syllable canon of Romagnolo Italian (Baroni 1999), resulting from an entirely expected series of phonetic changes. 7. Phonetically-Driven Phonology • The principles of phonology do embody functional phonetic criteria. • This has resulted in part from the widespread adoption of constraint-based approaches in phonology such as Optimality Theory (Prince and Smolensky 1993), which force the issue of what is a principled phonological constraint. • III: Approaches Based on Highly-Detailed Representations ¾ Phonological representations are not schematic and categorical, but very rich, embodying phonetics in a direct and vivid way: they include formant transitions, consonant releases, etc. Work of Steriade (1993), Pierrehumbert/Beckman/Ladd (in press), Flemming (1995), Boersma (1998), Kirchner (1998), others. ¾ Example: near-neutralizing final devoicing in German etc., as neutralization of some, but not all, of the many features that together manifest the intuitive opposition [voice] (Steriade 1999). • IV: Approaches Based on “Conventional” Representations + Learning ¾ Phonological representations involve categories and are not fully concrete. ¾ Children, monitoring their own sense of articulatory and perceptual difficulty, use this information to fabricate phonological constraints that embody phonetic criteria (Hayes 1999). ¾ Idea: account for the characteristic symmetry of phonology in the face of asymmetries and trade-offs in its phonetic base. ¾ Example: avoidance phenomena; e.g. child knows but systematically avoids words with phonological configurations that are phonetically difficult. • V: Innatist Approaches ¾ Our genes specify an inventory of constraints that embody innate knowledge of what is phonetically natural. ¾ There is little overlap (to my knowledge) between {scholars who take strong innatist views} and {scholars interested in phonetic explanation of phonological phenomena}. p. 3 Hayes PHONOLOGICALLY-GUIDED PHONETIC RESEARCH 8. Goal • Describe phonetic phenomena using a formalized phonological theory as a basis. Phonology: ¾ Serves as an organizing principle for phonetic implementation ¾ Interfaces phonetics to other linguistic domains. 9. Example: The Phonological Hypotheses of ToBIology References: Bruce 1977, Pierrehumbert 1980, Pierrehumbert & Beckman 1988, Ladd 1996 • Pitch contours are not unanalyzable gestalts, but derive from sequential representations: ¾ Phonological tones, occurring at linguistically significant locations ¾ Phonetic tonal targets, which interpret tones in pitch/time ¾ Phonetic interpolation between targets to form whole contours. • There is a hierarchy of levels of phonological phrasing; derivable by partly language- specific principles/constraints. Bases for phrasing: syntax, focus, phrase length, speaking rate. • Docking sites for tone: lexically-specified moras (Japanese), stressed syllables (English), left and right edges in the phrasal hierarchy (most/all languages). 10. Converging Evidence for the ToBIological Approach • Studies of the relative pitch scaling of tones under differing degrees of emphasis indicate they obey mathematically-statable laws; with better fits for tonal targets than tonal changes (Liberman and Pierrehumbert 1984). • Studies of the alignment of contours against phonetic material (Arvaniti et al. 1998, Ladd et al. 1999) have found cases in which alignment is target-governed (as opposed to constant-slope, constant-duration, etc.). • The tone sequences are not arbitrary, but constrained; describable by a tonal grammar (Pierrehumbert 1980: 29; Hayes and Lahiri 1991: 81). • Segmental sandhi processes are blocked at various junctural locations. These blocking effects diagnose a hierarchy that is the same hierarchy as the intonationally-diagnosed one (Hayes and Lahiri 1991, Jun 1996). • Tones/tone sequences can be analyzed as morphemes that have identifiable pragmatic content (Gussenhoven 1984, Pierrehumbert and Hirschberg 1987). • The tones are often distributed in ways that are iconic with respect to morphosyntax: e.g. in Bengali the rightmost L* … HP sequence marks the beginning and end of a focused constituent. The Phonetics-Phonology Interface: A Brief Orientation p. 4 11. Methodological Creed: Converging Evidence is a Good Thing! • Structure that is not directly observable is crucial to scientific understanding of language, but • Precisely because it is unobservable, we should insist on converging sources of evidence before taking such structures too seriously… 12. A Case Where the Evidence Didn’t Converge: The Moraic Theory of Weight References: Hyman (1985), McCarthy and Prince (1986), Hayes (1989) • Claim: all languages with heavy/light distinctions pick a criterion of weight: CVC, CVÛ vs. CV; CVÛ, CVR1 vs. CVO, CV; others. • This serves as a structural organizing principle: all the processes of the language that refer to weight refer to the same weight criterion (because they refer to structure, and structure encodes only that criterion). • Gordon (1999): surveyed ca. 400 languages, looking for cases where more than one process refers to weight. • Results: ¾ There is no more agreement (cross-process, within-language) than would be expected by chance. ¾ But there are significant connections between process and weight criterion (e.g. tone: CVÛ, CVR heavy), connections that have a plausible phonetic explanation. 13. The Merits of Converging Evidence • The obvious point of avoiding circularity: “CVÛ, CVR syllables heavy in this language because they can host contour tones; and they can host contour tones because they are heavy.” • The slightly-less obvious point: appeals to phonetic naturalness can give us novel solutions where the representational hypothesis fails to hold up. 14. This Session’s Papers • Include work on both phonetically-guided phonology and phonologically-guided phonetics. • Represent a variety of opinions on the role of phonology in phonetics and vice versa. References for this talk are posted at http://www.humnet.ucla.edu/humnet/linguistics/people/hayes/ 1 R stands for a sonorant consonant. p. 5 Hayes Arvaniti, Amalia, D. Robert Ladd, Ineke Mennen (1998) “Stability of tonal alignment: the case of Greek prenuclear accents,” Journal of Phonetics 26, 3-25. Baroni, Marco (1999) “How do languages get crazy constraints?,” ms., Department of Linguistics, UCLA. Blevins, Juliette (1999) paper given at the Berkeley Phonology Workshop, December 1999. Boersma, Paul (1998) Functional Phonology, Holland Academic Graphics, The Hague. Bruce, Gösta (1977) Swedish Word Accents in Sentence Perspective, Gleerup, Lund. Flemming, Edward (1995) Perceptual features in phonology, Ph.D. dissertation, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA. Gordon, Matthew (1999) Syllable Weight: Phonetics, Phonology, and Typology, Ph.D. dissertation, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA. Gussenhoven, Carlos (1984) On the Grammar and Semantics of Sentence Accents, Foris, Dordrecht. Hayes, Bruce (1989) “Compensatory Lengthening in Moraic Phonology,” Linguistic Inquiry 20, 253-306. Hayes, Bruce (1999) “Phonetically-Driven Phonology: The Role of Optimality Theory and Inductive Grounding,” in Michael Darnell, Edith Moravscik, Michael Noonan, Frederick Newmeyer, and Kathleen Wheatly, eds., Functionalism and Formalism in Linguistics, Volume I: General Papers, John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 243-285. Hayes, Bruce and Aditi Lahiri (1991) “Bengali Intonational Phonology,” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 9, 47-96. Hyman, Larry (1985) A Theory of Phonological Weight, Foris, Dordrecht. Jun, Sun-Ah (1996) The Phonetics and Phonology of Korean Prosody: intonational phonology and prosodic structure, Garland Publishing Inc., New York: NY Kirchner, Robert (1998) Lenition in Phonetically-Based Optimality Theory, Ph.D. dissertation,
Recommended publications
  • Conceptual Foundations of Phonology As a Laboratory Science1 Janet Pierrehumbert Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University Mary E
    Conceptual Foundations of Phonology as a Laboratory Science1 Janet Pierrehumbert Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University Mary E. Beckman Department of Linguistics, Ohio State University D. R. Ladd Department of Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, University of Edinburgh Paper to appear in N. Burton-Roberts, P. Carr, & G. Docherty (eds.) Phonological Knowledge: Its Nature and Status. Cambridge University Press. Author’s address: Janet Pierrehumbert Department of Linguistics Northwestern University 2106 Sheridan Road Evanston, IL 61208-4090 USA [email protected] 1. INTRODUCTION The term ‘laboratory phonology’ was invented more than a decade ago as the name of an interdisciplinary conference series, and all three of us have co-organized laboratory phonology conferences. Since then, the term has come into use not only for the conference series itself, but for the research activities exemplified by work presented there. In this paper, we give our own perspective on how research in laboratory phonology has shaped our understanding of phonological theory and of the relationship of phonological theory to empirical data. Research activities within laboratory phonology involve the cooperation of people who may disagree about phonological theory, but who share a concern for strengthening the scientific foundations of phonology through improved methodology, explicit modeling, and cumulation of results. These goals, we would argue, all reflect the belief that phonology is one of the natural sciences, and that all of language, including language-specific characteristics and sociolinguistic variation, is part of the natural world. In what follows, we explore the ramifications of this position for the relationship of data and methods to phonological theory; for the denotations of entities in that theory; and for our understanding of Universal Grammar and linguistic competence.
    [Show full text]
  • The Interpretation of Phonological Patterns in First Language Acquisition
    The interpretation of phonological patterns in first language acquisition Yvan Rose Memorial University of Newfoundland Sharon Inkelas University of California, Berkeley Acknowledgements: Several aspects of our work have benefited from discussions with other scholars, especially Heather Goad, François Pellegrino, Sophie Kern and Marilyn Vihman as well as from useful comments from two anonymous reviewers, to whom we are indebted. We also want to thank the Companion’s editors for their invitation to contribute as well as for their feedback and support at all stages in the making of this chapter. Of course, all remaining errors or omissions are our own. 1. Introduction Since the seminal work of Jakobson (1941/1968), phonological patterns observed in child language have been documented and analyzed from a number of perspectives. Throughout the relevant literature, an interesting paradox often manifests itself: while child language is generally characterized as a ‘simpler’ version of the target language, many types of phonological patterns observed in acquisition data create challenges for theories developed to account for more ‘complex’ adult systems. Phonologists within the generative framework have reacted to this problem in a number of ways, from tacitly or conspicuously ignoring acquisition data (e.g. Chomsky & Halle 1968: 331) to elaborating phonological models that account for phenomena observed in child phonology (e.g. Smith 1973, Bernhardt & Stemberger 1998) or, in some extreme cases, rejecting the validity of evidence from child production data for theoretical investigations of phonology (e.g. Hale & Reiss 1998). The disconnect between findings from child phonology and generalizations arising out of generative theories of grammar based on adult phonology has also been recruited in support of alternative, functionalist approaches to child phonology, including Stampe (1969) within Natural Phonology and, more recently, Vihman & Croft (2007) within Construction Grammar.
    [Show full text]
  • OUP Bibliography: Phonology Draft Reference List This Document
    OUP Bibliography: Phonology Draft Reference List This document includes an unannotated list of the references I had in the draft version of this entry. There were a number of restrictions on the bibliographic entry: • a maximum of 8 references per section and 150 references in the entire entry • Section introductions could be maximally one paragraph long • Work that was forthcoming or in press could not be cited Consequently, there were a number of things that I wanted to cite but could not. I have included these excised works below. *Typological Database System (TDS)[http://languagelink.let.uu.nl/tds/main.html]* Anderson, Stephen. 1985. Phonology in the 20th century. The University of Chicago Press. An excellent historical overview of theoretical developments in phonology up to the early 1980s. Anderson, John & Charles Jones 1974. Three theses concerning phonological representations. Journal of Linguistics 10: 1-26. Anderson, Stephen R. 1992. A-morphous morphology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Archangeli, Diana and Douglas Pulleyblank. 1994. Grounded Phonology. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press. Archibald, John. 1998. Second Language Phonology. John Benjamins Publishing Company. Bagemihl, Bruce. 1995. Language games and related areas. In John A. Goldsmith (ed.) The handbook of phonological theory. Oxford, Blackwell, pp.697-712. Baković, Eric (ed). 2004 onwards. *Phonoloblog: All things phonology Re: OBO article: Ethnoarchaeology revision requests[http://quote.ucsd.edu/blogs/phonoloblog/]*. Beckman, Jill. 1998. *Positional Faithfulness. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts Amherst[http://roa.rutgers.edu/view.php3?roa=234]* Beckman, Mary and John Kingston (series editors). 1990-2010. Laboratory Phonology book series. Various publishers. Benua, Laura. 2000. Phonological relations between words.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Psycholinguistic Data in Phonology
    Andries W. Coetzee, University of Michigan. January 2008. Preprint version of paper that will appear in Language, 84. GRAMMATICALITY AND UNGRAMMATICALITY IN PHONOLOGY * ABSTRACT In this paper, I make two theoretical claims: (i) For some form to be grammatical in language L, it is not necessary that the form satisfy all constraints that are active in L, i.e. even grammatical forms can violate constraints. (ii) There are degrees of ungrammaticality, i.e. not all ungrammatical forms are equally ungrammatical. I first show that these claims follow straightforwardly from the basic architecture of an Optimality Theoretic grammar. I then show that the surface sound patterns used most widely in formal phonology cannot be used to test the truth of these two claims, but argue that results from speech processing experiments can. Finally, I discuss three experiments on the processing of non-words of the form [stVt], [skVk] and [spVp] in English that were designed to test these claims, and show that both claims are confirmed by the results of the experiments. * This paper benefited greatly from discussion with and feedback from many people, and I express my appreciation to all of them: John McCarthy, John Kingston, Joe Pater, Pam Beddor, Sally Thomason, Jaye Padgett, the audiences at GLOW 27, LabPhon 9, the University of Maryland, Cornell University, Stanford University, Indiana University, Harvard University, the University of Massachusetts, and the University of Michigan. I also need to express my thanks to the anonymous reviewers, the associate editor in charge of this paper, and the editor of the journal. Their insistence on high standards, and their suggestions for improvements, resulted in this paper becoming much better.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonotactic and Morphological Effects in the Acceptability of Pseudowords Jeremy M. Needle, Northwestern University Janet B
    Phonotactic and Morphological Effects in the Acceptability of Pseudowords Jeremy M. Needle, Northwestern University Janet B. Pierrehumbert, University of Oxford Jennifer B. Hay, University of Canterbury Author Note Jeremy M. Needle, Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University. Janet B. Pierrehumbert, Department of Engineering Science, University of Oxford; Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University; New Zealand Institute of Language Brain and Behaviour, University of Canterbury. Jennifer B. Hay, Department of Linguistics, University of Canterbury; New Zealand Institute of Language Brain and Behaviour, University of Canterbury. This paper was presented at the 2017 Workshop on Morphological Typology and Linguistic Cognition, held at the Linguistic Institute in Lexington, KY. It has been accepted for the volume Morphological Typology and Linguistic Cognition, edited by Andrea Sims & Adam Ussishkin. This project was made possible through a grant from the John Templeton Foundation. The opinions expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the John Templeton Foundation. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jeremy M. Needle, Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208. Contact: [email protected] Page 2 of 40 Abstract We develop a large set of pseudowords that systematically varies length and phonotactic probability, and obtain acceptability ratings using an online interface. We find that phonotactic likelihood and the presence of an apparent morphological parse both significantly predict acceptability; pseudowords containing known morphemes are more acceptable than otherwise comparable pseudowords that do not. We find support for the conjecture that novel words with apparent morphology are advantaged as additions to the lexicon. The resulting lexicon, as observed, is one in which long words are not a random sampling of phonotactically acceptable wordforms, but instead tend to be completely or partially decomposable into morphemes.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Dialogue Representations for Concept-To-Speech Generation
    Using Dialogue Representations for Concept-to-Speech Generation Christine H. Nakatani Jennifer Chu-Carroll Bell Laboratories, Lucent Technologies 600 Mountain Avenue Murray Hill, NJ 07974 USA {chn Ij encc}©research, bell-labs, com Abstract Grosz and Sidner (1986) computational model of dis- We present an implemented concept-to-speech course interpretation and Pierrehumbert's prosodic (CTS) syst@n'~J tl~at offers original proposals for grammar for American English (1980). certain couplings-oir dialogue computation with In the present work, certain aspects of the orig- prosodic computation. Specifically, the semantic in- inal theories are modified and adapted to the ar- terpretation, task modeling and dialogue strategy chitecture of the dialogue system in which the CTS modules in a working spoken dialogue system are component is embedded. Below, we present the im- used to generate prosodic features to better convey portant fundamental definitions and principles of in- the meaning of system replies. The new CTS system tonation underlying our CTS system. embodies and extends theoretical work on intona- 2.1 Intonational System tional meaning in a more general, robust and rigor- In our CTS system, the prosodic elements that are ous way than earlier approaches, by reflecting com- computed are based on the intonational system of positional aspects of both dialogue and intonation Pierrehumbert (1980), who defined a formal lan- interepretation in an original computational frame- guage for describing American English intonation work for prosodic generation. using the following regular grammar: 1 Introduction Inton Phrase ---~ (Interm Phrase) + Bndry Tone Conversational systems that use speech as the input Interm Phrase ~ (Pitch Acc)+ Phrase Ace and output modality are often realized by architec- Major phrases, or inlonational phrases, are made tures that decouple speech processing components up of one or more minor phrases, or inlermediale from language processing components.
    [Show full text]
  • Conceptual Foundations of Phonology As a Laboratory Science Janet
    Conceptual Foundations of Phonology as a Laboratory Science Janet Pierrehumbert Department of Linguistics, Northwestern University Mary E. Beckman Department of Linguistics, Ohio State University D. R. Ladd Department of Linguistics and English Language, University of Edinburgh [UN-NUMBERED FOOTNOTE TO GO AT THE BOTTOM OF THE FIRST PAGE] This chapter is a reprinting of a paper that originally appeared in Phonological knowledge: Conceptual and empirical issues (Burton-Roberts et al. 2000). That paper was a substantially reworked version of a position paper on laboratory phonology that was first published in Current Trends in Phonology I (Durand and Laks 1996). For the current reprinting, the authors have gone through the text to remove typos and to provide updated bibliographic information for citations that were “forthcoming” or “in press” at the time of the original printing. The following acknowledgments from the original paper still hold: For comments on previous drafts of this paper, we are grateful to Ann Bradlow, John Coleman, Jacques Durand, Jan Edwards, Stefan Frisch, Jen Hay, Patricia Keating, Chris Kennedy, John Kingston, and Moira Yip. Although none of them are likely to agree with everything we have said here, we have benefited greatly from their suggestions about both substance and exposition. We are particularly grateful to David Hull, for fruitful discussion of the philosophy of science, and to the readers of Current Trends in Phonology I and the audience at Current Trends in Phonology II, for their responses to the earlier version of this paper. Work on the paper was supported by NSF Grant No. BNS-9022484 to Northwestern University; and by an Ohio State University Distinguished Scholar award and NIH Grant No.
    [Show full text]
  • Bibliography on Variation and Gradience in Phonology
    BIBLIOGRAPHY ON VARIATION AND GRADIENCE IN PHONOLOGY Arto Anttila Warning: This bibliography originated as a source of references for my course ‘Variation in Optimality Theory’ taught at the Phonology Fest at Indiana University, June 19-22, 2006 and it has been lightly edited for a course with the same title taught at the LSA Linguistic Institute at Stanford in July 2007. It is heavily biased towards generative phonology, in particular Optimality Theory. I have not tried to systematically cover the vast phonetic, psycholinguistic, and sociolinguistic literature on variation, gradience, and lexical frequency effects. I have included occasional references to variation in syntax and semantics, learnability, and phonological variation in specific languages, in particular English and Finnish. Corrections and additions are welcome. Albright, Adam. 2002. ‘Islands of reliability for regular morphology: Evidence from Italian’, Language 78(4), 684-709. Albright, Adam. 2006. ‘Gradient phonotactic effects: lexical? grammatical? both? neither?’ 80th Annual LSA Meeting, Albuquerque, NM. Anttila, Arto. 1997. ‘Deriving variation from grammar’, in Frans Hinskens, Roeland van Hout and Leo Wetzels, (eds.), Variation, Change and Phonological Theory, John Benjamins, Amsterdam / Philadelphia, pp. 35-68. Also on http://roa.rutgers.edu/. Anttila, Arto. 2002a. ‘Morphologically Conditioned Phonological Alternations’, Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20, 1-42. Also on http://roa.rutgers.edu/. Anttila, Arto. 2002b. ‘Variation and phonological theory’, in Jack Chambers, Peter Trudgill, and Natalie Schilling-Estes, (eds.), Handbook of Language Variation and Change, Blackwell, Malden, Massachusetts, and Oxford, U.K., pp. 206-243. Anttila, Arto. 2003. ‘Finnish Assibilation’, in Makoto Kadowaki and Shigeto Kawahara (eds.), Proceedings of NELS 33, GLSA, Amherst, Massachusetts, pp.13-24.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Language, Culture, Consciousness References Aissen, Judith. 1999. Markedness and Subject Choice in Optimality Theory. Natur
    Language, Culture, Consciousness References Aissen, Judith. 1999. Markedness and subject choice in Optimality Theory. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 17, 673-711. Anderson, Stephen. 1977 . Comments on the paper by Wasow. In P. Culicover, T. Wasow, and A. Akmajian (eds.), Formal Syntax, 361-377. New York: Academic Press. Baars, Bernard J. 1988. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press. Baars, Bernard J. 1997. Understanding subjectivity: Global Workspace Theory and the resurrection of the self. In Shear 1997, 241-248. Baars, Bernard J. 2003. Working Memory requires conscious processes, not vice versa: A Global Workspace account. In Osaka 2003, 11-26. Badler, Norman I., Rama Bindiganavale, Jan Allbeck, William Schuledr, Liwei Zhao, Seung-Joo Lee, Hogeun Shin, and Martha Palmer. 1999. Parameterized Action Representation and natural instructions for dynamic behavior modification of embodied agents. American Association for Artificial Intelligence. Baillargeon, Renée (1986) Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition 23, 21-41. Baker, C. L., and John J. McCarthy (eds.) (1981) The logical problem of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Baker, Mark. 1988. Incorporation: A Theory of Grammatical Function Changing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Bangerter, Adrian, and Herbert H. Clark. 2003. Navigation joint projects with dialogue. Cognitive Science 27, 195-225. Beckman, Mary, and Janet Pierrehumbert (1986) Intonational structure in English and Japanese. Phonology 3, 255-309. Beckman, Mary, and Janet Pierrehumbert. 1986. Intonational structure in Japanese and English. Phonology Yearbook 3, 15-70. Belletti, Adriana, and Luigi Rizzi. 1988. Psych-verbs and 2-theory.
    [Show full text]
  • This Is a Repository Copy of Phonology
    This is a repository copy of Phonology. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/81053/ Version: Accepted Version Book Section: Hellmuth, Sam orcid.org/0000-0002-0062-904X (2013) Phonology. In: Owens, Jonathan, (ed.) The Oxford Handbook of Arabic Linguistics. Oxford University Press , pp. 45-70. Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ 2 Phonology 2.1 Introduction Phonology is the study of systematic patterning in the distribution and realisation of speech sounds, within, and across, language varieties. The phonology of Arabic features heavily in the work of the Arab grammarians, most notably in the Kitab of Al-Sībawayh (Harun 1983). Sībawayh provides phonetic descriptions of the articulation of individual speech sounds (see chapter 1), which are accompanied by an analysis of the patterning of sounds in Arabic which is indisputably phonological in nature. Sībawayh was thus among the first in a long line of phonologists to work on the phonology of Arabic, and in this chapter we set out some of the key strands in that research, highlighting the contribution made by the study of Arabic to our understanding of phonology itself.
    [Show full text]