Linguistic Typology 2021; 25(2): 389–400

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Linguistic Typology 2021; 25(2): 389–400 Linguistic Typology 2021; 25(2): 389–400 Book Review Easterday, Shelece. 2019. Highly complex syllable structure: A typological and diachronic study (Studies in Laboratory Phonology 9), 389 p. Berlin: Language Science Press. ISBN: 978-3-96110-194-8, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo. 3268721. https://refubium.fu-berlin.de/handle/fub188/25953 Reviewed by Natalia Kuznetsova [nɑˈtaliə kʊznʲəˈʦovə], Faculty of Linguistic Sciences and Foreign Literatures, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Largo Agostino Gemelli, 1, 20123 Milano, MI, Italy; and Institute for Linguistic Studies, Russian Academy of Sciences, Tuchkov per. 9, 199053 St. Petersburg, Russia, E-mail: [email protected] https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2020-2066 Although this book was published in the “Studies in Laboratory Phonology” series, you will find hardly any laboratory phonologyinit.Instead,youwilldiscoverplentyof excellent phonological typology intertwined with historical phonology — probably amuchrarerfind. Until very recently, phonological typology was only marginally present within mainstream linguistic typology, being barely recognised as a subfield in its own right. The last few years have seen a gradual change: two synthesising volumes, both entitled Phonological Typology, have appeared (Gordon 2016; Hyman and Plank 2018) and the first workshop on this topic was held at ALT 2019 (Grossman and Moran 2019). At that meeting, the very first Greenberg Award (a prize of the Association for Linguistic Typology for outstanding typological dissertations) was given out in phonology — anditwenttothethesisonwhichthepresentbookisbased. The task of reviewing the resulting volume is both pleasant and challenging: this open-access volume appears as a new landmark in typological studies on syllable structure and a sine qua non for future research in the field. The main part (326 pages) of the work is arranged into eight chapters followed by two appendices with a full typological database which formed the basis for the study (220 pages). The primary object of study is Highly Complex Syllable Structure (HCSS): syllabic margins containing ≥3 obstruents or ≥4 of any consonants, to which the book presents a holistic rather than a partial typological approach (viz. Himmelmann 2000). HCSS is analysed in connection with other aspects of language structure: sound inventories, word-prosodic units, vowel reduction processes, the complexity of inflection. Moreover, a significant part of the book is devoted to a detailed multidimensional comparison of the four types of syllabic structure distinguished by the author, where the HCSS represents just one type along with the simple, moderately complex, and complex syllable structure. Chapter 1 “Syllables and syllable structure” sets the theoretical and typolog- ical scene with a thorough overview of theoretical models of the syllable and cross- Open Access. © 2020 Natalia Kuznetsova [nɑˈtaliə kʊznʲəˈʦovə], published by De Gruyter. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. 390 Book Review linguistic trends in its composition. The notion of the syllable has had a long life in linguistics, but the principal scheme of its internal structure has remained nearly unchanged since the 1940s (e.g. Kuryłovicz 1948). However, as the author points out, the syllable eludes precise definition and has no cross-linguistically consis- tent phonetic, phonological, or physiological correlates. Several points stand out in the choice of HCSS as the main object of the study. One reason is a sociolinguistic one: the endangered status of many languages with HCSS. This is in line with one of the latest trends in phonetic and phonological studies: a search for new theo- retical inputs in understudied languages (Tucker and Wright 2020; Whalen and McDonough 2019). Another reason answers to a current trend in typology: making it more geographically and historically grounded (the “what’s where why?” of Bickel 2007, 2015). The book offers an insight into the paths of emergence of HCSS and reflects on its stability over time. The third and fourth reasons are especially remarkable: the cross-linguistic rarity of highly complex syllables and their theoretical marginalisation due to various issues they create for analysis within existing frameworks. Rare features can challenge theories in a particularly efficient way, and a specific interest to- wards them arose in typology from the beginning of the 2000s (Golovko et al. 2015; Plank 2000; Simon and Wiese 2011; Wohlgemuth and Cysouw 2010a, 2010b). Two types of rarities have been distinguished: absolute rarities and those features which are rare worldwide but frequent in certain genetic groupings. HCSS belongs to the first type, although some genealogical and areal biases in its distribution can be observed (e.g. towards the Caucasus and the Pacific Northwest and away from the equator, p. 29). Rarities can be ignored by a general theory, re-analysed as regular, or incor- porated by changing the theory (Simon and Wiese 2011: 9–14). While previous accounts of HCSS chose one of the first two methods, the volume in question chooses rather the latter approach. Originally, rarities were seen as something unnatural with respect to the “natural” laws of language. This book is a shining example of a recent tendency towards their de-exoticisation by expanding the theory (other examples in phonology include Anderson 2016; Blevins 2018; Kuznetsova 2018). The trend could be linked, first, to the exponential rise of research on the evolutionary aspect of both universals and rarities, attempts to explain them through the typical paths of their emergence or disappearance (Blevins 2004, 2015; Round 2019). Second, rarities, for various reasons, are often concentrated in lesser-studied languages (e.g. Mithun 2007; Mansfield and Stan- ford 2017; Jäger, forthcoming), which are progressively coming under investigation and changing our understanding of what is rare or typical, possible or impossible. The third factor is a spread of parametric methods in typology and a shift from languages as the minimal units of analysis to the features of languages, and further Book Review 391 to the parameters of these features. Multidimensional approaches include Canonical Typology (e.g. Corbett 2007; Hyman 2014; Round and Corbett 2020) and Multivariate (or Distributional) Typology (e.g. Bickel 2015; Tallman 2020). Pre- senting marginal cases as possible but rare combinations of universal parameters rather than logically impossible cases also favours their de-exoticisation. Chapter 2 “Language sample” thoroughly describes the principles and the composition of the typological sample of the study, given in Appendix A “Language sample” and B “Data”. The sample is intended to be big enough for meaningful quantitative analysis but small enough to enable in-depth qualitative study, and as balanced as possible in terms of genealogical and geographic biases and the proportions of each of the four distinguished syllable complexity groups. The author has generally succeeded in these tasks, although a perfect balance was apparently unviable given the rarity not only of HCSS but also of simple syllable structure. For example, the simple category in her sample lacks languages from Eurasia altogether and includes fewer languages from North America while more from Southeast Asia as compared to the highly complex category of the sample. Easterday also addresses the third important bias which could not be entirely avoided: the literary or sociolinguistic one, whereby big literary languages with a long tradition of study are compared to small rural under-described languages. Complex syllables structures are concentrated in the latter group, which, according to the author, is also one of the reasons for their theoretical marginalisation. The whole sample comprises 100 languages, which is less than is usual in typological databases but more than is usual in samples on rarities, cf. e.g. 55 examples of non-modal vowels in Gordon (1998) or 13 language groupings on minimal vowel systems in Anderson (2016). Small databases are also used when the author is not interested in cross-linguistic frequencies (Himmelmann 2000), rather focusing on a very detailed, often holistic study, cf. 15 languages in Corbett (2003) or 40 in Nedjalkov (2007). The medium-sized database of the present vol- ume seeks to mediate between the qualitative and the quantitative demands of typological research. Chapters 3–7 contain core quantitative studies on the sample. Each of them starts with a list of hypotheses, proceeds with testing of these hypotheses, and discusses the results. Chapter 3 examines the syllable structures themselves, while the following three chapters contribute to the holistic nature of this study by investigating other phonological properties possibly correlating with the syllabic complexity: phonemic inventories, lexical prosody and vowel reduction. Chapter 7 explores the dynamic phonetic processes which could have led to the emergence of these associations. The most important findings of these chapters are outlined below. 392 Book Review Chapter 3 “Syllable structure patterns in sample” discusses the phonotactic, distributional, and phonetic properties of the attested syllable structures. An in-depth analysis focuses only on those properties which may correlate with other linguistic features or shed light on the development of HCSS. Some features were studied across the whole sample in a confirmatory manner. The following hypotheses were tested
Recommended publications
  • Northwest Journal of Linguistics Instructions for Referees Review Criteria
    Northwest Journal of Linguistics Instructions for Referees Review Criteria Topic Papers should deal with indigenous languages of Northwestern North America, including British Columbia, Alaska, the Yukon, the Northwest Territories, Washington, Oregon, Idaho, and Northwestern Montana. This definition is to be liberally interpreted. For example, papers dealing with contact languages may be appropriate even if they developed after European contact, as may be papers dealing with languages whose focus is elsewhere if there is some particular connection to the Northwest, e.g. a paper about Michif as used in British Columbia. Papers about languages spoken wholly outside of the region will generally not be appropriate but may be considered on the basis of some other connection, e.g. if the language is genetically related to a language of the region. Papers on all aspects of linguistics are acceptable: phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, pragmatics, diachronic, writing systems, sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics, language acquisition, etc. Papers may be descriptive, theoretical, or philological. Although we expect most papers to contain recent original research, we are interested in making available older unpublished material. Papers that clearly fall outside the scope of the journal will be rejected by the editors without refereeing, but referees may be called upon to give an opinion in marginal cases. Referees should feel free to indicate that a paper would be more appropriate for another journal. Quality The criteria for evaluating
    [Show full text]
  • Journal of Phonetics (1984) 12: 345-354
    In: Journal of Phonetics (1984) 12: 345-354. On the nature of labial velar shift Raymond Hickey Bonn University Abstract Labial velar shift is a common diachronic occurrence in various languages which in recent works on phonology has been captured by the reintroduction of the Jakobsonian feature [grave]. The type of shift involved, the form and direction it takes is a matter which has received insufficient attention. The present study is an attempt to account for this shift by viewing manifestations of it in Romance, Celtic, Germanic, Slavic and Uralic. The essential difference between lenition and labial velar shift is emphasized and the notion of favouring conditions for the shift (the phonotactic environment of the segments involved) is introduced. In all cases the acoustic (and hence autditory) similarity of the segments which undergo shifting is seen to be the triggering factor. It is by now commonplace to maintain that a phonological framework must take cognizance of, and provide notational means for describing, the interrelatedness of labial and velar segments. Evidence abounds in a variety of languages (see below) that labials and velars relate in a manner which say labials and dentals do not. In early distinctive feature theory (Jakobson and Halle, 1956, p. 43) this fact could be captured by the use of the feature [grave]. It was also quickly recognized by linguists after the publication of Chomsky and Halle (1968) that the abandoning of the feature [grave] constituted a loss in generalization (Ladefoged, 1972, p.44; Hyman, 1973; Lass and Anderson, 1975, p.187). However, in those works where the necessity for the feature [grave] is insisted upon (Davidsen-Nielsen and Ørum, 1978, p.201; Sommerstein, 1977, p.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonetics-Phonology Interface in Romance Languages José Ignacio Hualde, Ioana Chitoran
    Surface sound and underlying structure : The phonetics-phonology interface in Romance languages José Ignacio Hualde, Ioana Chitoran To cite this version: José Ignacio Hualde, Ioana Chitoran. Surface sound and underlying structure : The phonetics- phonology interface in Romance languages. S. Fischer and C. Gabriel. Manual of grammatical interfaces in Romance, 10, Mouton de Gruyter, pp.23-40, 2016, Manuals of Romance Linguistics, 978-3-11-031186-0. hal-01226122 HAL Id: hal-01226122 https://hal-univ-paris.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01226122 Submitted on 24 Dec 2016 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Manual of Grammatical Interfaces in Romance MRL 10 Brought to you by | Université de Paris Mathematiques-Recherche Authenticated | [email protected] Download Date | 11/1/16 3:56 PM Manuals of Romance Linguistics Manuels de linguistique romane Manuali di linguistica romanza Manuales de lingüística románica Edited by Günter Holtus and Fernando Sánchez Miret Volume 10 Brought to you by | Université de Paris Mathematiques-Recherche Authenticated | [email protected] Download Date | 11/1/16 3:56 PM Manual of Grammatical Interfaces in Romance Edited by Susann Fischer and Christoph Gabriel Brought to you by | Université de Paris Mathematiques-Recherche Authenticated | [email protected] Download Date | 11/1/16 3:56 PM ISBN 978-3-11-031178-5 e-ISBN (PDF) 978-3-11-031186-0 e-ISBN (EPUB) 978-3-11-039483-2 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data A CIP catalog record for this book has been applied for at the Library of Congress.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Processes
    Phonological Processes Phonological processes are patterns of articulation that are developmentally appropriate in children learning to speak up until the ages listed below. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESS DESCRIPTION AGE ACQUIRED Initial Consonant Deletion Omitting first consonant (hat → at) Consonant Cluster Deletion Omitting both consonants of a consonant cluster (stop → op) 2 yrs. Reduplication Repeating syllables (water → wawa) Final Consonant Deletion Omitting a singleton consonant at the end of a word (nose → no) Unstressed Syllable Deletion Omitting a weak syllable (banana → nana) 3 yrs. Affrication Substituting an affricate for a nonaffricate (sheep → cheep) Stopping /f/ Substituting a stop for /f/ (fish → tish) Assimilation Changing a phoneme so it takes on a characteristic of another sound (bed → beb, yellow → lellow) 3 - 4 yrs. Velar Fronting Substituting a front sound for a back sound (cat → tat, gum → dum) Backing Substituting a back sound for a front sound (tap → cap) 4 - 5 yrs. Deaffrication Substituting an affricate with a continuant or stop (chip → sip) 4 yrs. Consonant Cluster Reduction (without /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (grape → gape) Depalatalization of Final Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the end of a word (dish → dit) 4 - 6 yrs. Stopping of /s/ Substituting a stop sound for /s/ (sap → tap) 3 ½ - 5 yrs. Depalatalization of Initial Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the beginning of a word (shy → ty) Consonant Cluster Reduction (with /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (step → tep) Alveolarization Substituting an alveolar for a nonalveolar sound (chew → too) 5 yrs.
    [Show full text]
  • At the Juncture of Prosody, Phonology, and Phonetics – the Interaction of Phrasal and Syllable Structure in Shaping the Timing of Consonant Gestures∗
    At the juncture of prosody, phonology, and phonetics – the interaction of phrasal and syllable structure in shaping the timing of consonant gestures∗ Dani Byrd and Susie Choi The Laboratory Phonology research paradigm has established that abstract phonological structure has direct reflections in the spatiotemporal details of speech production. Past exploration of the complex relationship between lin- guistic structural properties and articulation has resulted in a softening of a severance between phonetics and phonology, which in recent years has begun to likewise encompass the phonetics-prosody interface. The abstract organiza- tion not only of words, but also of phrases, has a complex and rich effect on the articulatory details of speech production. This paper considers how phono- logical and prosodic structure interact in shaping the temporal realization of consonant sequences. Specifically, we examine the influence on articulatory timing of syllable structure effects in onset, coda, and heterosyllabic positions, and prosodic effects in the vicinity of a variety of phrase boundary conditions that grade in strength. Simultaneously, this work seeks to test and refine the π- gesture theoretical model of prosodic representation in speech production (Byrd & Saltzman 2003). 1. Introduction Three of the founders of Laboratory Phonology – Beckman, Ladd, and Pierre- humbert – describe its research paradigm as “involving the cooperation of peo- ple...whoshareaconcernforstrengtheningfoundations of phonology through improved methodology, explicit modeling, and cumulation of results” (Pier- rehumbert, Beckman, and Ladd 2001). Over the preceding nine Laboratory Phonology conferences, leading research in linguistic speech experimentation has clearly established that many aspects of abstract phonological structure are directly reflected in the spatiotemporal details of speech production.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 5 Sound Change
    An articulatory theory of sound change An articulatory theory of sound change Hypothesis: Most common initial motivation for sound change is the automation of production. Tokens reduced online, are perceived as reduced and represented in the exemplar cluster as reduced. Therefore we expect sound changes to reflect a decrease in gestural magnitude and an increase in gestural overlap. What are some ways to test the articulatory model? The theory makes predictions about what is a possible sound change. These predictions could be tested on a cross-linguistic database. Sound changes that take place in the languages of the world are very similar (Blevins 2004, Bateman 2000, Hajek 1997, Greenberg et al. 1978). We should consider both common and rare changes and try to explain both. Common and rare changes might have different characteristics. Among the properties we could look for are types of phonetic motivation, types of lexical diffusion, gradualness, conditioning environment and resulting segments. Common vs. rare sound change? We need a database that allows us to test hypotheses concerning what types of changes are common and what types are not. A database of sound changes? Most sound changes have occurred in undocumented periods so that we have no record of them. Even in cases with written records, the phonetic interpretation may be unclear. Only a small number of languages have historic records. So any sample of known sound changes would be biased towards those languages. A database of sound changes? Sound changes are known only for some languages of the world: Languages with written histories. Sound changes can be reconstructed by comparing related languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Instruction on L2 Pronunciation Development: a Synthesis of 15 Quasiexperimental Intervention Studies
    RESEARCH ISSUES TESOL Quarterly publishes brief commentaries on aspects of qualitative and quantitative research. Edited by CONSTANT LEUNG King’s College London Effects of Instruction on L2 Pronunciation Development: A Synthesis of 15 Quasi- Experimental Intervention Studies KAZUYA SAITO Waseda University Tokyo, Japan doi: 10.1002/tesq.67 & Over the past 25 years second language (L2) acquisition research has paid considerable attention to the effectiveness of instruction on L2 morphosyntax development, and the findings of relevant empirical studies have been extensively summarized using narrative review meth- ods (e.g., Ellis, 2002) as well as meta-analytic review methods (e.g., Spada & Tomita, 2010). These researchers have reached a consensus that (a) integrating language focus into meaning-oriented classrooms is more effective than a purely naturalistic approach, and (b) contextu- alized grammar teaching methods (e.g., focus-on-form instruction, form-focused instruction) is more effective than decontexualized gram- mar teaching methods (e.g., focus-on-formS instruction, grammar- translation method). What is surprising in this vein of L2 acquisition studies, however, is the lack of research in the area of L2 pronuncia- tion development. Pronunciation teaching has been notorious for its overdependence on decontextualized practice such as mechanical drills and repetition, reminiscent of the audiolingual teaching meth- ods of several decades ago (for discussion, see Celce-Murcia, Brinton, Goodwin, & Griner, 2010). Furthermore, very few language teachers actually receive adequate training in the specific area of pronunciation teaching (Foote, Holtby, & Derwing, 2011). 842 TESOL QUARTERLY Vol. 46, No. 4, December 2012 © 2012 TESOL International Association In recent years, several researchers have made strong calls for research on teaching for intelligible (rather than native-like) pronunciation.
    [Show full text]
  • A Sociolinguistics Study on the Use of the Javanese Language in the Learning Process in Primary Schools in Surakarta, Central Java, Indonesia
    International Education Studies; Vol. 7, No. 6; 2014 ISSN 1913-9020 E-ISSN 1913-9039 Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education A Sociolinguistics Study on the Use of the Javanese Language in the Learning Process in Primary Schools in Surakarta, Central Java, Indonesia Kundharu Saddhono1 & Muhammad Rohmadi1 1 Sebelas Maret University, Indonesia Correspondence: Kundharu Saddhono, Sebelas Maret University, Jl. Ir. Sutami 36 Surakarta, Indonesia. E-mail: [email protected] Received: February 24, 2014 Accepted: April 8, 2014 Online Published: May 20, 2014 doi:10.5539/ies.v7n6p25 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ies.v7n6p25 Abstract This study aims at describing the use of language at primary schools grade 1, 2, and 3 in Surakarta. The study belongs to descriptive qualitative research. It emphasizes in a note which depict real situation to support data presentation. Content analysis is used as research methodology. It analyzes the research result of the observed speech event. The data are collected from three sources: informant, events, and documents. Results of the study demonstrate that the use of Javanese language is still dominant in the learning process at primary schools in Surakarta. Many factors affect the use of Javanese language as mother tongue in classroom teaching-learning process. They are (1) balancing the learning process, so that learners are able to better understand the material presented by the teacher (2) teacher’s habit to speak Javanese language, and (3) drawing student’s attention. The factors underlying this phenomenon are explained by teacher and student’s lack of Indonesian language vocabulary. In addition, there is an element unnoticed by teachers.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Is Pronunciation So Difficult to Learn?
    www.ccsenet.org/elt English Language Teaching Vol. 4, No. 3; September 2011 Why is Pronunciation So Difficult to Learn? Abbas Pourhossein Gilakjani (Corresponding author) School of Educational Studies, Universiti Sains Malaysia, Malaysia 3A-05-06 N-Park Jalan Batu Uban 11700 Gelugor Pulau Pinang Malaysia Tel: 60-174-181-660 E-mail: [email protected] Mohammad Reza Ahmadi School of Educational Studies, Universiti Sains Malaysia, Malaysia 3A-05-06 N-Park Alan Batu Uban 1700 Gelugor Pulau Pinang Malaysia Tel: 60-175-271-870 E-mail: [email protected] Received: January 8, 2011 Accepted: March 21, 2011 doi:10.5539/elt.v4n3p74 Abstract In many English language classrooms, teaching pronunciation is granted the least attention. When ESL teachers defend the poor pronunciation skills of their students, their arguments could either be described as a cop-out with respect to their inability to teach their students proper pronunciation or they could be regarded as taking a stand against linguistic influence. If we learn a second language in childhood, we learn to speak it fluently and without a ‘foreign accent’; if we learn in adulthood, it is very unlikely that we will attain a native accent. In this study, the researchers first review misconceptions about pronunciation, factors affecting the learning of pronunciation. Then, the needs of learners and suggestions for teaching pronunciation will be reviewed. Pronunciation has a positive effect on learning a second language and learners can gain the skills they need for effective communication in English. Keywords: Pronunciation, Learning, Teaching, Misconceptions, Factors, Needs, Suggestions 1. Introduction General observation suggests that it is those who start to learn English after their school years are most likely to have serious difficulties in acquiring intelligible pronunciation, with the degree of difficulty increasing markedly with age.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Typology, Rhythm Types and the Phonetics-Phonology Interface
    Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2012 Phonological typology, rhythm types and the phonetics-phonology interface. A methodological overview and three case studies on Italo-Romance dialects Schmid, Stephan Posted at the Zurich Open Repository and Archive, University of Zurich ZORA URL: https://doi.org/10.5167/uzh-73782 Book Section Published Version Originally published at: Schmid, Stephan (2012). Phonological typology, rhythm types and the phonetics-phonology interface. A methodological overview and three case studies on Italo-Romance dialects. In: Ender, Andrea; Leemann, Adrian; Wälchli, Bernhard. Methods in contemporary linguistics. Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton, 45-68. Phonological typology, rhythm types and the phonetics-phonology interface. A methodological overview and three case studies on Italo- Romance dialects Stephan Schmid 1. Introduction Phonological typology has mainly concentrated on phoneme inventories and on implicational universals, whereas the notion of ‘language type’ ap- pears to be less appealing from a phonological perspective. An interesting candidate for establishing language types on the grounds of phonological or phonetic criteria would have come from the dichotomy of ‘stress-timing’ vs. ‘syllable-timing’, if instrumental research carried out by a number of phoneticians had not invalidated the fundamental claim of the so-called ‘isochrony hypothesis’. Nevertheless, the idea of classifying languages according to their rhythmic properties has continued to inspire linguists and phoneticians, giving rise to two diverging methodological perspectives. The focus of the first framework mainly lies on how phonological processes relate to prosodic domains, in particular to the syllable and to the phonolog- ical word.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Treatment of Consonant Cluster Reduction
    ON THE TREATMENT OF CONSONANT CLUSTER REDUCTION Young-Seok Kim In this paper we will consider a theory of Consonant Cluster Reduction (CCR) without deletion rules. Through its analysis of CCR and closely related phenome,1a in the phonology of Korean, this paper explains why CCR should be dealt with by a general convention which we shall refer to as Stray Erasure Principle. It will be shown that, though primarily based on the Korean language, this theory makes correct predic­ tions about similar phenomena in other typologically unrelated languages such as Menomini. 1. The Syllable Structure of Korean The phonemic inventory of Standard Korean comprises 9 (or 10) vowels and 21 consonants including glides. 1 At the phonetic level, only a limited number of con­ sonants (i.e. [p=], [t=], [k=], [1], [m], [n] and [IJ] are allowed in the syllable-final position because of Obstruent Neutralization. However, all the consonants can in principle occur both morpheme-initially2 and morpheme-finally provided we admit that there are some accidental gaps for some reason or other. True consonants can also occur before an on-glide y or w. The canonical syllable structure of Korean would then look like this: (1) ~ (C) (G) V (C) Since we do not find evidence supporting the syllable-internal hierarchical struc­ ture, we will take no account of the possibility of assigning G either to the onset (cf. B.-G. Lee 1982) or to the nucleus, which in turn is dominated by an upper node, namely the rime. Instead, we will adopt the basic idea of Clements and Keyser (1983), which is minimally different from the theory developed by Kahn (1976) in that it recognizes an intermediate CV tier between the syllable (0) and the phonemic melody 1 It has been taken for granted by many grammarians that vowel length is distinctive in the Seoul dialect (H.-B.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter X Positional Factors in Lenition and Fortition
    Chapter X Positional factors in Lenition and Fortition 1. Introduction This chapter sets out to identify the bearing that the linear position of a segment may have on its lenition or fortition.1 We take for granted the defi- nition of lenition that has been provided in chapter XXX (Szigetvári): Put- ting aside stress-related lenition (see chapter XXX de Lacy-Bye), the refer- ence to the position of a segment in the linear string is another way of identifying syllabic causality. Something is a lenition iff the effect ob- served is triggered by the specific syllabic status of the segment at hand. The melodic environment thus is irrelevant, and no melodic prime is trans- mitted between segments. Lenition thereby contrasts with the other family of processes that is found in phonology, i.e. adjacency effects. Adjacency may be defined physically (e.g. palatalisation of a consonant by a following vowel) or in more abstract terms (e.g. vowel harmony): in any event, as- similations will transmit a melodic prime from one segment to another, and only a melodically defined subset of items will qualify as a trigger. Posi- tional factors, on the other hand, are unheard of in assimilatory processes: there is no palatalisation that demands, say, "palatalise velars before front vowels, but only in word-initial position". Based on an empirical record that we have tried to make as cross- linguistically relevant as possible, the purpose of this chapter is to establish appropriate empirical generalisations. These are then designed to serve as the input to theories of lenition: here are the challenges, this is what any theory needs to be able to explain.
    [Show full text]