Effects of Interference Competition on the Foraging Activity of Tropical Roseate Terns ’

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Effects of Interference Competition on the Foraging Activity of Tropical Roseate Terns ’ The Condor95~322-329 0 The Cooper Omthologml Society 1993 EFFECTS OF INTERFERENCE COMPETITION ON THE FORAGING ACTIVITY OF TROPICAL ROSEATE TERNS ’ DAVID A. SHEALERAND JOANNA BURGER Department of BiologicalSciences, Rutgers University,Piscataway, NJ 08855 Abstract. Although difficult to detect, interference competition among foraging birds may affect the fitness of an inferior competitor by lowering foraging success.We studied foraging flocks of Roseate Terns (Sterna dougallii) at Culebra, Puerto Rico, in 1990 to examine the effectsof small and large monospecificand mixed-speciesflocks on the foraging successand behavior of this threatenedspecies. Roseate Terns mostly foragedin small flocks with Brown Noddies (Anousstolidus). The frequency of foraging attempts in Roseate Terns decreasedwith increasingsize of mixed-speciesflocks, primarily due to passiveinterference by Brown Noddies, which foragedjust over the water surfaceand limited the area available to Roseate Terns. The size of monospecific Roseate Tern flocks had no effect on foraging attempt frequency. Roseate Terns also aborted more feeding dives and foraged more by dipping when in flocks with noddies, relative to flocks of only conspecifics.Proportionally fewer Roseate Terns than expected foraged in large, mixed-species flocks (>8 birds), sug- gestingthat they preferentially avoid large flocks that result in lower foraging success.Al- though Brown Noddies caused a reduction in Roseate Tern foraging attempt frequencies and an increasein aborted dives, we were unable to detect a negative effect on adult survival or reproductive successof this population. Key words: Competition:interference; foraging success;Roseate Tern; Sterna dougallii; Brown Noddy; Anous stolidus. INTRODUCTION Fish schools tend to be monotypic (Breder 1959, Interference competition among foraging birds Shaw 1970) suggesting that all birds in a flock may affect the fitness of an inferior competitor are vying for the same resource. Further, seabirds by lowering foraging success, particularly when use different prey-capture techniques (Porter and food availability is limited. Interference com- Sealy 1982, Hulsman 1989) creating the poten- petition occurs when one species impedes the tial for asymmetrical prey acquisition and pas- access of another species to a resource either sive interference between species. Competition through active aggression or territoriality (Schoe- probably does not lead to competitive exclusion ner 1983) or by passive means, such as creating (sensu Gause 1934) among breeding seabird spe- a barrier to the resource (Chamov et al. 1976). cies because the guild is only congregated during Maurer (1984) developed a model that predicts part of the year. Ashmole (1963) has argued that the ecological setting in which interference com- selection (in terms of mortality) in birds is stron- petition may occur. Despite Maurer’s model, only gest during the nonbreeding season when re- a few examples of passive interference compe- sources are more limited. Seasonal fluctuation in tition exist (Goss-Custard 1980, Waite 1984, food abundance may be a primary determining Poysa 1985), possibly due to the difficulty in de- factor in clutch size (Ricklefs 1980) and breeding tecting it (Maurer 1984) or in differentiating it success(Uttley et al. 1989). But competition may from exploitative competition (Anholt 1990). restrict breeding population size and reproduc- Aggregations of foraging seabirds provide an tive success as well, particularly when prey is opportunity to examine the effects of interference scarce (Ashmole 197 1). competition for prey resources. Seabirds often In 1990 we studied a guild of seabirds at Cu- congregate in mixed-species flocks over prey fish lebra, Puerto Rico, with particular emphasis on schools that are patchily-distributed and ephem- the foraging associations, behavior and success eral (Lack 1968, Safinaet al. 1988, Safina 1990). of the Roseate Tern (Sternadougallii). Roseate Terns historically nested on several cays in the Culebra Archipelago, but the breeding popula- ’ Received 15 October 1992. Accepted 15 January tion has remained consistently small (< 225 pairs; 1993. Fumiss 1983; Burger and Gochfeld, unpubl. data). I3221 INTERFERENCE COMPETITION IN TERNS 323 Nearby RoseateTern colonies at St. Thomas and scanning the flock with 7 x 35 binoculars until along the southwestcoast of Puerto Rico number the first Roseate Tern was encountered. We then up to 1,000 breeding pairs. The low numbers of timed the length of the bird’s foraging bout with Roseate Terns at Culebra may indicate that this a stopwatch and counted the number of com- site is suboptimal for breeding, possibly because pleted dives and aborted dives the bird made. of low food availability or becauseRoseate Terns Roseate Terns feed primarily by plunge-diving are inferior competitors to other breeding seabird into the water from variable heights, making speciesin the area. feeding attempts easy to quantify. We scored an Our objectives in this study were to quantify aborted dive when a tern began a dive but pulled the composition of foraging flocksat Culebra that up before contacting the water. Terns aborted included RoseateTerns, and to compare foraging dives both when other birds moved between them behavior and successof Roseate Terns between and the fish school and when their path from the large and small monospecific and mixed-species air to the water was clear. We were not certain flocks. A secondary goal of this study was to what causedterns to abort dives in the latter case, provide comparative data on the foraging be- but presumably it was related to the target fish havior of Roseate Terns in the Caribbean to the becoming inaccessible.Thus, for convention, we well-studied population in the northeastern hereafter refer to the two types of aborted dives United States. as being caused by birds and fish. We excluded all observations in which we could not determine STUDY AREA AND METHODS which situation caused an aborted dive. We studied foraging flocks of Roseate Terns in In this study we use the frequency of prey cap- the Culebra Archipelago, Puerto Rico. The study ture attempts as a relative measure of foraging site lies 37 km east of Puerto Rico proper and success.We were unable to determine capture 25 km west of the U.S. Virgin Islands (see Wet- successbecause we often observed flocks from a more 19 17, Kepler and Kepler 1978, Fumiss considerable distance and becauseprey fish were 1983, for detailed site descriptions). Roseate quite small (~20 mm standard length; Shealer, Terns nested on two small islands in the archi- unpubl. data). In 199 1, we observed R.oseate pelago (see Burger and Gochfeld 1988 for nest- Terns foraging in a shallow tidal lagoon in south- site characteristics).The total breeding popula- western Puerto Rico and found that capture suc- tion at Culebra in 1990 was 102 pairs. Other cessfrequency was highly correlated with capture breeding seabirds in the area included Brown attempt frequency (Fig. 1). Our 199 1 study only Noddies (Anous stolid@, Laughing Gulls (Larus validated this relationship up to four capture at- atricilla), Bridled and Sooty Terns (Sterna an- tempts per minute, whereas capture attempt fre- aethetus and S. fiscata), tropicbirds (Phaethon quenciesin the present study were always higher. aethereus and P. lepturus) and boobies (&da A possiblereason for this difference is that in the leucogaster,S. sula and S. dactylatra). 199 1 study, terns were foraging on larger fish On 14 days from 26 May to 1 July 1990, we (> 30 mm standard length) and in the absenceof observed foraging flocks of seabirds which in- predatory fish. Thus, search and handling times cluded Roseate Terns in the Culebra Archipel- were almost certainly greater,resulting in a lower ago. Sites were chosen opportunistically, wher- capture attempt frequency. Search and handling ever and whenever groups of birds were foraging times in the present study were presumably neg- over schools of prey fish, but all observations ligible since the surface thrashing of predatory were made from land. Data were collected only fish indicated the presenceof prey and the small- when predatory fish were visible at the surface, er fish were easier to swallow. Salt and Willard indicating that prey fish were readily available (1971) found that Forster’s Terns (Sterna fir- to foraging birds, and only when birds were ac- steri) swallowed small fish “almost immediately, tively foraging. sometimes while the bird was ascendingto hunt- When the above criteria were met, feeding ob- ing altitude.” Ulenaers et al. (1992) found that servationswere conducted on individual Roseate handling times of Great Crested Grebes (Podi- Terns until the feeding flock dispersed.A feeding cepscristatus) feeding on small fish did not limit observation consisted of first counting the total the number of prey eaten per unit time. The few number and speciescomposition of seabirds in times we observed Roseate Terns catching fish the flock. A test bird was randomly selectedby in flocks over predatory fish, we also noted that 324 DAVID A. SHEALER AND JOANNA BURGER 0 1 2 3 4 Attemptdmin FIGURE 1. Relationship between frequency of plunge dives and capturesin individual Roseate Terns. Study was conducted at a Roseate Tern colony in southwestern Puerto Rico in 1991. Points are individual birds followed for r4 min each. R2 = 0.911, df = 23, P < 0.01. fish were swallowed immediately upon the bird’s FORAGING BEHAVIOR AND SUCCESS emergencefrom the water. For the
Recommended publications
  • Foraging Modes of Carnivorous Plants Aaron M
    Israel Journal of Ecology & Evolution, 2020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22244662-20191066 Foraging modes of carnivorous plants Aaron M. Ellison* Harvard Forest, Harvard University, 324 North Main Street, Petersham, Massachusetts, 01366, USA Abstract Carnivorous plants are pure sit-and-wait predators: they remain rooted to a single location and depend on the abundance and movement of their prey to obtain nutrients required for growth and reproduction. Yet carnivorous plants exhibit phenotypically plastic responses to prey availability that parallel those of non-carnivorous plants to changes in light levels or soil-nutrient concentrations. The latter have been considered to be foraging behaviors, but the former have not. Here, I review aspects of foraging theory that can be profitably applied to carnivorous plants considered as sit-and-wait predators. A discussion of different strategies by which carnivorous plants attract, capture, kill, and digest prey, and subsequently acquire nutrients from them suggests that optimal foraging theory can be applied to carnivorous plants as easily as it has been applied to animals. Carnivorous plants can vary their production, placement, and types of traps; switch between capturing nutrients from leaf-derived traps and roots; temporarily activate traps in response to external cues; or cease trap production altogether. Future research on foraging strategies by carnivorous plants will yield new insights into the physiology and ecology of what Darwin called “the most wonderful plants in the world”. At the same time, inclusion of carnivorous plants into models of animal foraging behavior could lead to the development of a more general and taxonomically inclusive foraging theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Foraging : an Ecology Model of Consumer Behaviour?
    This is a repository copy of Foraging : an ecology model of consumer behaviour?. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/122432/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Wells, VK orcid.org/0000-0003-1253-7297 (2012) Foraging : an ecology model of consumer behaviour? Marketing Theory. pp. 117-136. ISSN 1741-301X https://doi.org/10.1177/1470593112441562 Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Foraging: an ecology model of consumer behavior? Victoria.K.Wells* Durham Business School, UK First Submission June 2010 Revision One December 2010 Revision Two August 2011 Accepted for Publication: September 2011 * Address for correspondence: Dr Victoria Wells (née James), Durham Business School, Durham University, Mill Hill Lane, Durham, DH1 3LB & Queen’s Campus, University Boulevard, Thornaby, Stockton-on-Tees, TS17 6BH, Telephone: +44 (0)191 334 0472, E-mail: [email protected] I would like to thank Tony Ellson for his guidance and Gordon Foxall for his helpful comments during the development and writing of this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Water Stress Inhibits Plant Photosynthesis by Decreasing
    letters to nature data on a variety of animals. The original foraging data on bees Acknowledgements (Fig. 3a) were collected by recording the landing sites of individual We thank V. Afanasyev, N. Dokholyan, I. P. Fittipaldi, P.Ch. Ivanov, U. Laino, L. S. Lucena, bees15. We find that, when the nectar concentration is low, the flight- E. G. Murphy, P. A. Prince, M. F. Shlesinger, B. D. Stosic and P.Trunfio for discussions, and length distribution decays as in equation (1), with m < 2 (Fig. 3b). CNPq, NSF and NIH for financial support. (The exponent m is not affected by short flights.) We also find the Correspondence should be addressed to G.M.V. (e-mail: gandhi@fis.ufal.br). value m < 2 for the foraging-time distribution of the wandering albatross6 (Fig. 3b, inset) and deer (Fig. 3c, d) in both wild and fenced areas16 (foraging times and lengths are assumed to be ................................................................. proportional). The value 2 # m # 2:5 found for amoebas4 is also consistent with the predicted Le´vy-flight motion. Water stress inhibits plant The above theoretical arguments and numerical simulations suggest that m < 2 is the optimal value for a search in any dimen- photosynthesis by decreasing sion. This is analogous to the behaviour of random walks whose mean-square displacement is proportional to the number of steps in coupling factor and ATP any dimension17. Furthermore, equations (4) and (5) describe the W. Tezara*, V. J. Mitchell†, S. D. Driscoll† & D. W. Lawlor† correct scaling properties even in the presence of short-range correlations in the directions and lengths of the flights.
    [Show full text]
  • Predation Risk and Feeding Site Preferences in Winter Foraging Birds
    Eastern Illinois University The Keep Masters Theses Student Theses & Publications 1992 Predation Risk and Feeding Site Preferences in Winter Foraging Birds Yen-min Kuo This research is a product of the graduate program in Zoology at Eastern Illinois University. Find out more about the program. Recommended Citation Kuo, Yen-min, "Predation Risk and Feeding Site Preferences in Winter Foraging Birds" (1992). Masters Theses. 2199. https://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/2199 This is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses & Publications at The Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of The Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THESIS REPRODUCTION CERTIFICATE TO: Graduate Degree Car1dichi.tes who have written formal theses. SUBJECT: Permission to reproduce theses. The University Library is receiving a number of requests from other institutions asking permission to reproduce dissertations for inclusion in their library holdings. Although no copyright laws are involved~ we feel that professional courtesy demands that permission be obtained from the author before we ~llow theses to be copied. Please sign one of the following statements: Booth Library o! Eastern Illinois University has my permission to lend my thesis tq a reputable college or university for the purpose of copying it for inclul!lion in that institution's library or research holdings. Date I respectfully request Booth Library of Eastern Illinois University not allow my thesis be reproduced because ~~~~~~-~~~~~~~ Date Author m Predaton risk and feeding site preferences in winter foraging oirds (Tllll) BY Yen-min Kuo THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF Master of Science in Zoology IN TH£ GRADUATE SCHOOL, EASTERN ILLINOIS UNIVfRSITY CHARLESTON, ILLINOIS 1992 YEAR I HEREBY R£COMMEND THIS THESIS BE ACCEPTED AS FULFILLING THIS PART or THE GRADUATE DEGREE CITED ABOVE 2f vov.
    [Show full text]
  • Photosynthetic Pigments Estimate Diet Quality in Forage and Feces of Elk (Cervus Elaphus) D
    51 ARTICLE Photosynthetic pigments estimate diet quality in forage and feces of elk (Cervus elaphus) D. Christianson and S. Creel Abstract: Understanding the nutritional dynamics of herbivores living in highly seasonal landscapes remains a central chal- lenge in foraging ecology with few tools available for describing variation in selection for dormant versus growing vegetation. Here, we tested whether the concentrations of photosynthetic pigments (chlorophylls and carotenoids) in forage and feces of elk (Cervus elaphus L., 1785) were correlated with other commonly used indices of forage quality (digestibility, energy content, neutral detergent fiber (NDF), and nitrogen content) and diet quality (fecal nitrogen, fecal NDF, and botanical composition of the diet). Photosynthetic pigment concentrations were strongly correlated with nitrogen content, gross energy, digestibility, and NDF of elk forages, particularly in spring. Winter and spring variation in fecal pigments and fecal nitrogen was explained with nearly identical linear models estimating the effects of season, sex, and day-of-spring, although models of fecal pigments were 2 consistently a better fit (r adjusted = 0.379–0.904) and estimated effect sizes more precisely than models of fecal nitrogen 2 (r adjusted = 0.247–0.773). A positive correlation with forage digestibility, nutrient concentration, and (or) botanical composition of the diet implies fecal photosynthetic pigments may be a sensitive and informative descriptor of diet selection in free-ranging herbivores. Key words: carotenoid, chlorophyll, diet selection, digestibility, energy, foraging behavior, nitrogen, phenology, photosynthesis, primary productivity. Résumé : La compréhension de la dynamique nutritive des herbivores vivant dans des paysages très saisonniers demeure un des défis centraux de l’écologie de l’alimentation, peu d’outils étant disponibles pour décrire les variations du choix de plantes dormantes ou en croissances.
    [Show full text]
  • Interpreting Temporal Variation in Omnivore Foraging Ecology Via
    Functional Ecology 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2009.01553.x InterpretingBlackwell Publishing Ltd temporal variation in omnivore foraging ecology via stable isotope modelling Carolyn M. Kurle*† Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Department, University of California, 100 Shaffer Road, Santa Cruz, CA 95060, USA Summary 1. The use of stable carbon (C) and nitrogen (N) isotopes (δ15N and δ13C, respectively) to delineate trophic patterns in wild animals is common in ecology. Their utility as a tool for interpreting temporal change in diet due to seasonality, migration, climate change or species invasion depends upon an understanding of the rates at which stable isotopes incorporate from diet into animal tissues. To best determine the foraging habits of invasive rats on island ecosystems and to illuminate the interpretation of wild omnivore diets in general, I investigated isotope incorporation rates of C and N in fur, liver, kidney, muscle, serum and red blood cells (RBC) from captive rats raised on a diet with low δ15N and δ13C values and switched to a diet with higher δ15N and δ13C values. 2. I used the reaction progress variable method (RPVM), a linear fitting procedure, to estimate whether a single or multiple compartment model best described isotope turnover in each tissue. Small sample Akaike Information criterion (AICc) model comparison analysis indicated that 1 compartment nonlinear models best described isotope incorporation rates for liver, RBC, muscle, and fur, whereas 2 compartment nonlinear models were best for serum and kidney. 3. I compared isotope incorporation rates using the RPVM versus nonlinear models. There were no differences in estimated isotope retention times between the model types for serum and kidney (except for N turnover in kidney from females).
    [Show full text]
  • Foraging Ecology of a Winter Bird Community in Southeastern Georgia
    Georgia Southern University Digital Commons@Georgia Southern Electronic Theses and Dissertations Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of Fall 2018 Foraging Ecology of a Winter Bird Community in Southeastern Georgia Rachel E. Mowbray Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.georgiasouthern.edu/etd Part of the Behavior and Ethology Commons, and the Population Biology Commons Recommended Citation Mowbray, Rachel E., "Foraging Ecology of a Winter Bird Community in Southeastern Georgia" (2018). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. 1861. https://digitalcommons.georgiasouthern.edu/etd/1861 This thesis (open access) is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate Studies, Jack N. Averitt College of at Digital Commons@Georgia Southern. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons@Georgia Southern. For more information, please contact [email protected]. FORAGING ECOLOGY OF A WINTER BIRD COMMUNITY IN SOUTHEASTERN GEORGIA by RACHEL MOWBRAY (Under the Direction of C. Ray Chandler) ABSTRACT Classical views on community structure emphasized deterministic processes and the importance of competition in shaping communities. However, the processes responsible for shaping avian communities remain controversial. Attempts to understand distributions and abundances of species are complicated by the fact that birds are highly mobile. Many species migrate biannually between summer breeding grounds and wintering grounds. The goal of this study was to test four hypotheses that attempt to explain how migratory species integrate into resident assemblages of birds (Empty-Niche Hypothesis, Competitive-Exclusion Hypothesis, Niche-Partitioning Hypothesis, and Generalist-Migrant Hypothesis). I collected data on birds foraging during the winter of 2017-2018 in Magnolia Springs State Park, Jenkins County, Georgia, U.S.A.
    [Show full text]
  • Nocturnal and Diurnal Foraging Behaviour of Brown Bears (Ursus Arctos) on a Salmon Stream in Coastal British Columbia
    Color profile: Disabled Composite Default screen 1317 Nocturnal and diurnal foraging behaviour of brown bears (Ursus arctos) on a salmon stream in coastal British Columbia D.R. Klinka and T.E. Reimchen Abstract: Brown bears (Ursus arctos) have been reported to be primarily diurnal throughout their range in North America. Recent studies of black bears during salmon migration indicate high levels of nocturnal foraging with high capture efficiencies during darkness. We investigated the extent of nocturnal foraging by brown bears during a salmon spawning migration at Knight Inlet in coastal British Columbia, using night-vision goggles. Adult brown bears were observed foraging equally during daylight and darkness, while adult females with cubs, as well as subadults, were most prevalent during daylight and twilight but uncommon during darkness. We observed a marginal trend of increased cap- ture efficiency with reduced light levels (day, 20%; night, 36%) that was probably due to the reduced evasive behaviour of the salmon. Capture rates averaged 3.9 fish/h and differed among photic regimes (daylight, 2.1 fish/h; twilight, 4.3 fish/h; darkness, 8.3 fish/h). These results indicate that brown bears are highly successful during nocturnal foraging and exploit this period during spawning migration to maximize their consumption rates of an ephemeral resource. Résumé : Les ours bruns (Ursus arctos) sont généralement reconnus comme des animaux à alimentation surtout diurne dans toute leur aire de répartition. Les résultats d’études récentes sur les ours noirs durant la migration des saumons indiquent qu’ils font une quête de nourriture intense pendant la nuit et que l’efficacité de leurs captures est élevée à l’obscurité.
    [Show full text]
  • Seabird Foraging Ecology
    2636 SEABIRD FORAGING ECOLOGY Ballance L.T., D.G. Ainley, and G.L. Hunt, Jr. 2001. Seabird Foraging Ecology. Pages 2636-2644 in: J.H. Steele, S.A. Thorpe and K.K. Turekian (eds.) Encyclopedia of Ocean Sciences, vol. 5. Academic Press, London. SEABIRD FORAGING ECOLOGY L. T. Balance, NOAA-NMFS, La Jolla, CA, USA Introduction D. G. Ainley, H.T. Harvey & Associates, San Jose, CA, USA Though bound to the land for reproduction, most G. L. Hunt, Jr., University of California, Irvine, seabirds spend 90% of their life at sea where they CA, USA forage over hundreds to thousands of kilometers in a matter of days, or dive to depths from the surface ^ Copyright 2001 Academic Press to several hundred meters. Although many details of doi:10.1006/rwos.2001.0233 seabird reproductive biology have been successfully SEABIRD FORAGING ECOLOGY 2637 elucidated, much of their life at sea remains a mesoscales (100}1000km, e.g. associations with mystery owing to logistical constraints placed on warm- or cold-core rings within current systems). research at sea. Even so, we now know a consider- The question of why species associate with different able amount about seabird foraging ecology in water types has not been adequately resolved. At terms of foraging habitat, behavior, and strategy, as issue are questions of whether a seabird responds well as the ways in which seabirds associate with or directly to habitat features that differ with water partition prey resources. mass (and may affect, for instance, thermoregula- tion), or directly to prey, assumed to change with Foraging Habitat water mass or current system.
    [Show full text]
  • A Practical Technique for Measuring the Behavior of Foraging Animals
    g k sTHow A PractIcal Technique for )D Do Measuring the Behavior of It _ . It Foraging hnlmalS RosemaryJ. Smith Joel S. Brown Animal behavior is well suited for havioral questions that can be readily ness (survivaland reproduction).If so, illustrating ecological and evolution- addressed gives students the opportu- an animal should leave a patch when ary principles. For ecology projects or nity to suggest their own hypotheses its benefits of continued feeding in the labs, animal behavior has a number of and experiments.The projectsempha- patch no longer exceed its cost of feed- advantages:It is inherently interesting size the methodology of a scientist ing in the patch. The formal frame- Downloaded from http://online.ucpress.edu/abt/article-pdf/53/4/236/45243/4449276.pdf by guest on 01 October 2021 to most students, it can be observed, it and, we hope, the excitement that work behind the assertion in the pre- is flexible and it can change quickly. accompanies creative thinking and vious sentence is called Optimal Unfortunately, meaningful observa- novel results. Foraging Theory (see Stephens & tions of behavior tend to be time- Krebs 1986). consuming and difficultto analyze. To We begin by elaborating a rule for overcome this handicap, we introduce Rationale when an animal should leave a patch an indirect procedure that uses the and then discuss how this rule can be foraging behavior of animals at exper- "Talking" to animals requires a common language, and feeding be- used to reveal aspects of that animal's imental food patches to address ques- ecology. An example from our own tions in animal behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • Fifty Years of Food and Foraging in Moose: Lessons in Ecology from a Model Herbivore
    ALCES VOL. 46, 2010 SHIPLEY - MOOSE AS A MODEL HERBIVORE FIFTY YEARS OF FOOD AND FORAGING IN MOOSE: LESSONS IN ECOLOGY FROM A MODEL HERBIVORE Lisa A. Shipley Department of Natural Resource Sciences, Washington State University, Pullman, Washington 99164-6410, USA ABSTRACT: For more than half a century, biologists have intensively studied food habits and forag- ing behavior of moose (Alces alces) across their circumpolar range. This focus stems, in part, from the economic, recreational, and ecosystem values of moose, and because they are relatively easy to observe. As a result of this research effort and the relatively simple and intact ecosystems in which they often reside, moose have emerged as a model herbivore through which many key ecological ques- tions have been examined. First, dietary specialization has traditionally been defined solely based on a narrow, realized diet (e.g., obtaining >60% of its diet from 1 plant genus). This definition has not been particularly useful in understanding herbivore adaptations because >99% of mammalian herbi- vores are thus classified as generalists. Although moose consume a variety of browses across their range, many populations consume 50-99% of their diets from 1 genus (e.g., Salix). Like obligatory herbivores, moose have demonstrated adaptations to the chemistry and morphology of their nearly monospecific diets, which precludes them from eating large amounts of grass and many forbs. New classifications for dietary niche suggest that moose fit on the continuum between facultative special- ists and facultative generalists. Second, moose have been the subject of early and influential models predicting foraging behavior based on the tradeoffs between quality and quantity in plants.
    [Show full text]
  • Rangeland Herbivores Learn to Forage in a World Where the Only Constant Is Change Larry D
    ARIZONA COOPERATIVE E TENSION AZ1518 July 2010 Rangeland Herbivores Learn to Forage in a World Where the Only Constant is Change Larry D. Howery, Frederick D. Provenza, Beth Burritt Introduction When we go to the grocery store it is a fairly easy task to select and purchase nutritious meals. A readily available, predictable food supply is conveniently organized and displayed in the aisles. The nutritional composition of most foods is clearly labeled so you can immediately know what nutrients (and perhaps, toxins) you will be consuming. In contrast, rangeland animals live in a world where nutrients and toxins are constantly changing across space and time. For example, there may be 10’s to 100’s of plant species ROVENZA growing on a single acre, and each plant can differ widely F. D. P F. BY in the kinds and amounts of nutrients and toxins it offers to free-ranging herbivores. Even at the level of the individual HOTO plant, plant parts vary in their concentration of nutrients and P toxins. Leaves, stems, and flowers, all differ in the kinds Photo 1. Mother can have a profound influence on her offspring’s dietary and amounts of nutrients and toxins they contain. Nutrient preferences and toxin content of the same plant species can also vary depending on where it grows (in the sun vs. shade, on a wet Exposure to foods with mother at an early age vs. dry site, on a fertile vs. infertile site, etc.). Mother Nature has long-term effects on diet selection can also drastically alter foraging environments as a result of We have all heard the old adage “mother knows best.” natural disasters like floods, fires, or droughts.
    [Show full text]