Personal Creativity for Personal Productivity

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Personal Creativity for Personal Productivity Copyright © 2014 by McGraw-Hill Education. All rights reserved. Except as permitted under the United States Copyright Act of 1976, no part of this publication may be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means, or stored in a database or retrieval system, without the prior written permission of the publisher. ISBN: 978-0-07-182723-2 MHID: 0-07-182723-4 e-Book conversion by Cenveo® Publisher Services Version 1.0 The material in this chapter eBook also appears in the print version of Global Innovation Science Handbook: ISBN: 978-0-07-179270-7, MHID: 0-07-179270-8. McGraw-Hill Education eBooks are available at special quantity discounts to use as premiums and sales promotions, or for use in corporate training programs. To contact a representative, please visit the Contact Us page at www.mhprofessional.com. All trademarks are trademarks of their respective owners. Rather than put a trademark symbol after every occurrence of a trademarked name, we use names in an editorial fashion only, and to the benefit of the trademark owner, with no intention of infringement of the trademark. Where such designations appear in this book, they have been printed with initial caps. Information has been obtained by McGraw-Hill Education from sources believed to be reliable. However, because of the possibility of human or mechanical error by our sources, McGraw-Hill Education, or others, McGraw-Hill Education does not guarantee the accuracy, adequacy, or completeness of any information and is not responsible for any errors or omissions or the results obtained from the use of such information. TERMS OF USE This is a copyrighted work and McGraw-Hill Education and its licensors reserve all rights in and to the work. Use of this work is subject to these terms. Except as permitted under the Copyright Act of 1976 and the right to store and retrieve one copy of the work, you may not decompile, disassemble, reverse engineer, reproduce, modify, create derivative works based upon, transmit, distribute, disseminate, sell, publish or sublicense the work or any part of it without McGraw-Hill Education’s prior consent. You may use the work for your own noncommercial and personal use; any other use of the work is strictly prohibited. Your right to use the work may be terminated if you fail to comply with these terms. THE WORK IS PROVIDED “AS IS.” McGRAW-HILL EDUCATION AND ITS LICENSORS MAKE NO GUARANTEES OR WARRANTIES AS TO THE ACCURACY, ADEQUACY OR COMPLETENESS OF OR RESULTS TO BE OBTAINED FROM USING THE WORK, INCLUDING ANY INFORMATION THAT CAN BE ACCESSED THROUGH THE WORK VIA HYPERLINK OR OTHERWISE, AND EXPRESSLY DISCLAIM ANY WARRANTY, EXPRESS OR IMPLIED, INCLUDING BUT NOT LIMITED TO IMPLIED WARRANTIES OF MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. McGraw-Hill Education and its licensors do not warrant or guarantee that the functions contained in the work will meet your requirements or that its operation will be uninterrupted or error free. Neither McGraw-Hill Education nor its licensors shall be liable to you or anyone else for any inaccuracy, error or omission, regardless of cause, in the work or for any damages resulting therefrom. McGraw-Hill Education has no responsibility for the content of any information accessed through the work. Under no circumstances shall McGraw-Hill Education and/or its licensors be liable for any indirect, incidental, special, punitive, consequential or similar damages that result from the use of or inability to use the work, even if any of them has been advised of the possibility of such damages. This limitation of liability shall apply to any claim or cause whatsoever whether such claim or cause arises in contract, tort or otherwise. CHAPTER 5 Creating Creativity: Personal Creativity for Personal Productivity Arin N. Reeves Introduction Although creativity and innovation are often used as interchangeable terms or meshed together as one concept, the difference between the two is an important one that actually helps us to understand each more fully. One way to differentiate between the two is to understand creativity as the mental precursor to innovation; creativity is about imagination and ideas where innovation is about action and process. The authors of the new book Smart- Storming: The Game-Changing Process for Generating Bigger, Better Ideas, Mitchell Rigie and Keith Harmeyer describe the difference between creativity and innovation as “Creativity is most often defined as the mental ability to conceptualize (imagine) new, unusual or unique ideas, to see the new connection between seemingly random or unrelated things. Innovation on the other hand, is defined as the process that transforms those forward-looking new ideas into real-world (commercial) products, services, or processes of enhanced value. The result of such a transformation can be incremental, evolutionary, or radical in its impact on the status quo.”1 Innovation requires creativity, but creativity does not always lead to innovation. Organi- zations that seek innovative thinking need workplaces and talent development systems that foster creativity, and they also need process systems that can translate the creativity into innovation. Understanding these distinctions between creativity and innovation allows us to understand, learn, and maximize each more comprehensively. Since creativity is separate from, albeit necessary to, innovation, individuals can develop and utilize their personal creativity capabilities regardless of whether their jobs and work- places explicitly require or seek innovation. Furthermore, an IBM survey of more than 1500 Chief Executives from over 60 countries and 30 industries found that “chief executives believe that—more than rigor, management discipline, integrity, or even vision—successfully navi- gating an increasing complex world will require creativity.”2 Developing and using personal creativity in the workplace is no longer relegated to the “creative arts” or deemed as a “nice to have,” developing and using personal creativity at work—regardless of the work, work- place, or industry—is quickly becoming a competitive differentiator in talent and a core competency for leadership whether or not it is fostered in the workplace where it is valued. What Is Personal Creativity? Thought leaders from many industries have defined creativity in a myriad of ways, and there is no universal way to define creativity that makes sense for all who are trying to understand it in the context of their own lives. The definition used in this exploration of creativity is based on the standard definition of creativity explored in the Creativity Research 73 05_Gupta_ch05_p071-082.indd 73 16/12/13 9:17 AM 74 Section 2: General Concepts Chapter 5: Creating Creativity 75 Journal by researchers from The Torrance Creativity Center3 as well as the definition set forth by Robert E. Franken in Human Motivation.4 “Creativity is the ability to create original ideas, connections, alternatives, or possibilities that are effective in solving problems, communicating with others, and inspiring new and useful ideas in others.” Creativity requires both the unrestrained openness of originality and the “does it work” constraints of effectiveness. It is the ability to think of things that no one has thought of before or see connections between existing ideas that no one has seen before in a way that is inspira- tional, useful, and/or enjoyable. As the following quote suggests, creativity needs some con- straint in order to be creative; otherwise, it could just be random, muddled, or even bizarre. “Creativity is not simply originality and unlimited freedom. There is much more to it than that. Creativity also imposes restrictions. While it uses methods other than those of ordinary thinking, it must not be in disagreement with ordinary thinking—or rather, it must be something that, sooner or later, ordinary think- ing will understand, accept, and appreciate. Otherwise, the result would be bizarre, not creative.” —Sylvano Arieti, Creativity: The Magic Synthesis How Does Personal Creativity Work? In order to explore the process of personal creativity, it is important to explicitly recognize that creativity is indeed a process, and it can be honed through practice. The process may feel intuitive and organic to some, and it may feel unnatural and uncomfortable for others, but the output from the process is creative regardless of how the process is internalized. Based on the research we have conducted with dozens of individuals in a multitude of industries and professions, the process of generating original and effective ideas (see Fig. 5-1) requires 1. Diverse inputs/inclusive thinking 2. Context articulation 3. Divergent thinking 4. Convergent thinking Diverse Inputs/Inclusive Thinking While researchers have posited everything from neurobehavioral differences to cultural dif- ferences to differences in education levels and intelligence to explain the variations in indi- vidual creativity, recent research indicates that seeking and integrating diverse inputs into everyday thinking can simply and effectively increase overall personal creativity regardless of other factors.5 Diverse inputs from a multitude of cultures, ages, professions, languages, nationalities, geographic origins, educational backgrounds, interests, and so on, have the potential to enhance an individual’s ability to generate new ideas and new connections between existing ideas as long as the individual thinks inclusively about integrating the diverse inputs into his every- day thinking. These diverse
Recommended publications
  • Creativity in Nietzsche and Heidegger: the Relation of Art and Artist
    Creativity in Nietzsche and Heidegger: The Relation of Art and Artist Justin Hauver Philosophy and German Mentor: Hans Sluga, Philosophy August 22, 2011 I began my research this summer with a simple goal in mind: I wanted to out- line the ways in which the thoughts of Friedrich Nietzsche and Martin Heidegger complement one another with respect to art. I had taken a few courses on each philosopher beforehand, so I had some inclination as to how their works might be brought into agreement. However, I almost immediately ran into difficulty. It turns out that Heidegger, who lived and thought two or three generations after Nietzsche, had actually lectured on the topic of Nietzsche's philosophy of art and had placed Nietzsche firmly in a long tradition characterized by its mis- understanding of art and of the work of art. This means that Heidegger himself did not agree with me|he did not see his thoughts on art as complementary with Nietzsche's. Rather, Heidegger saw his work as an improvement over the misguided aesthetic tradition. Fortunately for me, Heidegger was simply mistaken. At least, that's my thesis. Heidegger did not see his affinity with Nietzsche because he was misled by his own misinterpretation. Nevertheless, his thoughts on art balance nicely with those of Nietzsche. To support this claim, I will make three moves today. First, I will set up Heidegger's critique, which is really a challenge to the entire tradition that begins with Plato and runs its course up to Nietzsche. Next, I will turn to Heidegger's views on art to see how he overcomes the tradition and answers his own criticism of aesthetics.
    [Show full text]
  • 4Creativity, Shared Meaning, and Relationships
    Creativity, Shared Meaning, and Relationships By Carlina Rinaldi 4The search for meaning begins from the moment of birth, from the child’s first silent “why,” and continues all through life. It is a difficult search. Young children make enormous efforts to put together often-disconnected fragments of experience to make sense of things. They persevere with their search stub- bornly, tirelessly, making mistakes, and often on their own. But while engaged in this search, children ask us to share the search with them. We as teachers are asked by children to see them as scientists or philosophers searching to understand something, to draw out a meaning, to grasp a “piece of life,” and to respect this search as a quality central to all human beings. We are asked to be the child’s traveling companion in this search for meaning. We are also asked to respect the meanings that children produce, the explanatory theories they develop, and their attempts to find and give answers. When we honor children this way, the children reveal themselves to us: We come to know how they perceive, question, and inter- pret reality, and to understand their relationships with it. Concepts for Care • 2 1 I believe that teachers must communicate behaviors that teachers exhibit toward a willingness to assist children in their the child’s intention to search, the pro- search for meaning in life. Two of the cess of searching, and the conclusions a most important questions we have to ask child reaches either support or dampen ourselves as teachers are creativity. The teacher’s job is to engage in a “relational creativity” with the child “How can we aid young children • that both revels in the child’s creativity in their search for the meaning of and stimulates the teacher’s own creativ- things, and the meaning of ity to find ways to help the child observe, life itself?” analyze, interpret, and build theories.
    [Show full text]
  • 75 Quotes About Creativity and Innovation
    75 Quotes about Creativity and Innovation 1. “There is no doubt that creativity is the most important human resource of all. Without creativity, there would be no progress, and we would be forever repeating the same patterns.” — Edward de Bono 2. “There is only one of you in all time, this expression is unique. And if you block it, it will never exist through any other medium and it will be lost.” — Martha Graham 3. “Creativity is thinking up new things. Innovation is doing new things.” — Theodore Levitt 4. “A new idea is delicate. It can be killed by a sneer or a yawn; it can be stabbed to death by a quip and worried to death by a frown on the right man’s brow.” — Charles Brower 5. “When we engage in what we are naturally suited to do, our work takes on the quality of play and it is play that stimulates creativity.” – Linda Naiman 6. “The creative is the place where no one else has ever been. You have to leave the city of your comfort and go into the wilderness of your intuition. What you’ll discover will be wonderful. What you’ll discover is yourself.” — Alan Alda 7. “It is better to have enough ideas for some of them to be wrong, than to be always right by having no ideas at all.” — Edward de Bono 8. “A painter told me that nobody could draw a tree without in some sort becoming a tree; or draw a child by studying the outlines of its form merely .
    [Show full text]
  • Postgraduate Students' Perception of Creativity in the Research Process
    c e p s Journal | Vol.1 | No1 | Year 2011 169 Postgraduate Students’ Perception of Creativity in the Research Process Mojca Juriševič1 • The purpose of the research was, with the aid of a short ques- tionnaire, to determine how postgraduate students (N = 32) perceive the opportunities for creative research in general, and how they perceive creativity in the preparation of their own re- search work in particular. Descriptive analysis shows that stu- dents (1) perceive a positive study-research climate that encour- ages creative processes (independence, motivation, intellectual challenges), (2) judge that researchers have numerous opportu- nities for creative work in the various phases of research and (3) evaluate themselves as highly creative individuals in everyday life. Students perceive themselves as being at their most creative in the definition of the research problem, which they mainly identify with the use of personal strategies (work experience) and take various lengths of time to form, typically up to one year. The most difficult problem in this regard is represented by giving meaning to the problem (breadth, depth, specificity, ap- plication). Amongst the perceived encouragement with which mentors motivate students for creative research the most fre- quent is less directive general guidance in study and research. On the basis of the presented findings, guidelines are suggested for the more effective encouragement of creative research in postgraduate students. Key words: Postgraduate study, Research, Research problem, Scientific
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroaesthetics of Art Vision: an Experimental Approach to the Sense of Beauty
    Cl n l of i ica a l T rn r u ia o l s J ISSN: 2167-0870 Journal of Clinical Trials Research Article Neuroaesthetics of Art Vision: An Experimental Approach to the Sense of Beauty Maddalena Coccagna1, PietroAvanzini2, Mariagrazia Portera4, Giovanni Vecchiato2, Maddalena Fabbri Destro2, AlessandroVittorio Sironi3,9, Fabrizio Salvi8, Andrea Gatti5, Filippo Domenicali5, Raffaella Folgieri3,6, Annalisa Banzi3, Caselli Elisabetta1, Luca Lanzoni1, Volta Antonella1, Matteo Bisi1, Silvia Cesari1, Arianna Vivarelli1, Giorgio Balboni Pier1, Giuseppe Santangelo Camillo1, Giovanni Sassu7, Sante Mazzacane1* 1Department of CIAS Interdepartmental Research Center, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy;2Department of CNR Neuroscience Institute, Parma, Italy;3Department of CESPEB Neuroaesthetics Laboratory, University Bicocca, Milan, Italy;4Department of Letters and Philosophy, University of Florence, Italy;5Department of Humanistic Studies, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy;6Department of Philosophy Piero Martinetti, University La Statale, Milan, Italy;7Department of Musei Arte Antica, Ferrara, Italy;8Department of Neurological Sciences, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna, Italy;9Department of Centre of the history of Biomedical Thought, University Bicocca, Milan, Italy ABSTRACT Objective: NEVArt research aims to study the correlation between a set of neurophysiological/emotional reactions and the level of aesthetic appreciation of around 500 experimental subjects, during the observation of 18 different paintings from the XVI-XVIII century, in a real museum context. Methods: Several bio-signals have been recorded to evaluate the participants’ reactions during the observation of paintings. Among them: (a) neurovegetative, motor and emotional biosignals were recorded using wearable tools for EEG (electroencephalogram), ECG (electrocardiogram) and EDA (electrodermal activity); (b) gaze pattern during the observation of art works, while (c) data of the participants (age, gender, education, familiarity with art, etc.) and their explicit judgments about paintings have been obtained.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts
    Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts Ready, Set, Create: What Instructing People to “Be Creative” Reveals About the Meaning and Mechanisms of Divergent Thinking Emily C. Nusbaum, Paul J. Silvia, and Roger E. Beaty Online First Publication, May 5, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036549 CITATION Nusbaum, E. C., Silvia, P. J., & Beaty, R. E. (2014, May 5). Ready, Set, Create: What Instructing People to “Be Creative” Reveals About the Meaning and Mechanisms of Divergent Thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036549 Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts © 2014 American Psychological Association 2014, Vol. 8, No. 2, 000 1931-3896/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0036549 Ready, Set, Create: What Instructing People to “Be Creative” Reveals About the Meaning and Mechanisms of Divergent Thinking Emily C. Nusbaum, Paul J. Silvia, and Roger E. Beaty University of North Carolina at Greensboro The “be creative effect”—instructing people to “be creative” before a divergent thinking task makes the responses more creative—is one of the oldest findings in creativity science. The present research suggests that this seemingly simple effect has underappreciated implications for the assessment of divergent thinking and for theories of the executive and controlled nature of creative thought. An experiment measured fluid intelligence and gave 2 divergent thinking tasks: people completed one with “be creative” instructions and the other with “be fluent” (generate as many ideas as possible) instructions. The responses were scored for creativity (using subjective scoring) and for fluency (the total number of ideas).
    [Show full text]
  • Second Nature in Kant's Theory of Artistic Creativity Adam Blazej University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee
    University of Wisconsin Milwaukee UWM Digital Commons Theses and Dissertations May 2013 Second Nature in Kant's Theory of Artistic Creativity Adam Blazej University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee Follow this and additional works at: https://dc.uwm.edu/etd Part of the Philosophy Commons Recommended Citation Blazej, Adam, "Second Nature in Kant's Theory of Artistic Creativity" (2013). Theses and Dissertations. 79. https://dc.uwm.edu/etd/79 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by UWM Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of UWM Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. SECOND NATURE IN KANT’S THEORY OF ARTISTIC CREATIVITY by Adam Blazej A Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements of the Degree of Master of Arts In Philosophy at The University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee May 2013 ABSTRACT SECOND NATURE IN KANT’S THEORY OF ARTISTIC CREATIVITY by Adam Blazej The University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, 2013 Under the Supervision of Professor William Bristow One of the central claims of John McDowell’s Mind and World is that, in reconciling an apparent opposition between the normative and the natural, philosophers should look to a notion of second nature: the idea that nature includes a species of animals (namely, human beings) who, through their socialization, transform themselves into rational beings capable of thinking about and acting in the world in response to reasons. McDowell argues that Kant lacks a notion of second nature and thereby fails to overcome the relevant problem of reconciliation.
    [Show full text]
  • Creativity and Perception
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology Psychology, Department of 1989 Creativity and Perception John H. Flowers University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Calvin P. Garbin University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychfacpub Part of the Psychiatry and Psychology Commons Flowers, John H. and Garbin, Calvin P., "Creativity and Perception" (1989). Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology. 453. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychfacpub/453 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychology, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published (as Chapter 8) in Handbook of Creativity, edited by John A. Glover, Royce R. Ronning, and Cecil R. Reynolds (New York & London: Plenum Press, 1989), pp. 147-162. Copyright © 1989 Plenum Press (Springer). Used by permission. Creativity and Perception John H. Flowers and Calvin P. Garbin Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588. Informal thought about the nature of mental oper- dealing with the topic of creativity, most of these refer- ations important to creative human behavior suggests ences refer to research related to specific theories about that perceptual processes are of considerable impor- individual differences in perceptual styles or process- tance. The ability to “see relationships among ele- ing modes, as opposed to broader contemporary is- ments” is an attribution commonly made toward au- sues of perceptual processing. Clearly, most research- thors of major scientific discoveries or of noteworthy ers in the field of perception have not touched upon artistic achievements.
    [Show full text]
  • A Mathematician's Lament
    A Mathematician’s Lament by Paul Lockhart musician wakes from a terrible nightmare. In his dream he finds himself in a society where A music education has been made mandatory. “We are helping our students become more competitive in an increasingly sound-filled world.” Educators, school systems, and the state are put in charge of this vital project. Studies are commissioned, committees are formed, and decisions are made— all without the advice or participation of a single working musician or composer. Since musicians are known to set down their ideas in the form of sheet music, these curious black dots and lines must constitute the “language of music.” It is imperative that students become fluent in this language if they are to attain any degree of musical competence; indeed, it would be ludicrous to expect a child to sing a song or play an instrument without having a thorough grounding in music notation and theory. Playing and listening to music, let alone composing an original piece, are considered very advanced topics and are generally put off until college, and more often graduate school. As for the primary and secondary schools, their mission is to train students to use this language— to jiggle symbols around according to a fixed set of rules: “Music class is where we take out our staff paper, our teacher puts some notes on the board, and we copy them or transpose them into a different key. We have to make sure to get the clefs and key signatures right, and our teacher is very picky about making sure we fill in our quarter-notes completely.
    [Show full text]
  • Concept of Mimesis in Theodor W. Adorno's and Max Horkheimer‘S Aesthetic Theory
    VYTAUTAS MAGNUS UNIVERSITY FACULTY OF POLITICAL SCIENCE AND DIPLOMACY DEPARTMENT OF PHILOSOPHY AND SOCIAL CRITIQUE Kristijonas Jakevičius CONCEPT OF MIMESIS IN THEODOR W. ADORNO'S AND MAX HORKHEIMER‘S AESTHETIC THEORY Master Thesis Practical philosophy, state code 621V53001 Humanitarian sciences study field Supervisor prof. dr. Jay Daniel Mininger Defended prof. dr. Šarūnas Liekis Kaunas, 2018 Contents 1 Summary ................................................................................................................................... 4 2 Santrauka ................................................................................................................................... 5 3 Introduction ............................................................................................................................... 6 1 Concept of mimesis ................................................................................................................... 8 1.1 Change of mimetic paradigm .............................................................................................. 9 2 The concept of Culture industry ............................................................................................... 10 2.1 Mass culture or culture industry? ...................................................................................... 12 3 Technological root of the Culture industry ............................................................................... 14 3.1 Benjamin‘s views on technology .....................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroaesthetics: a Coming of Age Story
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Neuroethics Publications Center for Neuroscience & Society 1-2011 Neuroaesthetics: A Coming of Age Story Anjan Chatterjee University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/neuroethics_pubs Part of the Medicine and Health Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Chatterjee, A. (2011). Neuroaesthetics: A Coming of Age Story. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23 (1), 53-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21457 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/neuroethics_pubs/58 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Neuroaesthetics: A Coming of Age Story Abstract Neuroaesthetics is gaining momentum. At this early juncture, it is worth taking stock of where the field is and what lies ahead. Here, I review writings that fall under the rubric of neuroaesthetics. These writings include discussions of the parallel organizational principles of the brain and the intent and practices of artists, the description of informative anecdotes, and the emergence of experimental neuroaesthetics. I then suggest a few areas within neuroaesthetics that might be pursued profitably. Finally, I raise some challenges for the field. These challenges are not unique to neuroaesthetics. As neuroaesthetics comes of age, it might take advantage of the lessons learned from more mature domains of inquiry within cognitive neuroscience. Disciplines Medicine and Health Sciences Comments This journal article is available at ScholarlyCommons: https://repository.upenn.edu/neuroethics_pubs/58 Neuroaesthetics: A Coming of Age Story Anjan Chatterjee Abstract ■ Neuroaesthetics is gaining momentum. At this early junc- gence of experimental neuroaesthetics. I then suggest a few areas ture, it is worth taking stock of where the field is and what lies within neuroaesthetics that might be pursued profitably.
    [Show full text]
  • Evaluation of Self-Perceptions of Creativity: Is It a Useful Criterion? Roni Reiter-Palmon University of Nebraska at Omaha, [email protected]
    University of Nebraska at Omaha DigitalCommons@UNO Psychology Faculty Publications Department of Psychology 6-2012 Evaluation of Self-Perceptions of Creativity: Is It a Useful Criterion? Roni Reiter-Palmon University of Nebraska at Omaha, [email protected] Erika Morral University of Nebraska at Omaha, [email protected] James C. Kaufman California State University - San Bernardino Jonathan Bruce Santo University of Nebraska at Omaha, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/psychfacpub Part of the Industrial and Organizational Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Reiter-Palmon, Roni; Morral, Erika; Kaufman, James C.; and Santo, Jonathan Bruce, "Evaluation of Self-Perceptions of Creativity: Is It a Useful Criterion?" (2012). Psychology Faculty Publications. 19. https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/psychfacpub/19 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Psychology at DigitalCommons@UNO. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@UNO. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 1 Evaluation of Self-Perceptions of Creativity: Is it a Useful Criterion? Roni Reiter-Palmon, Erika Robinson-Morral University of Nebraska at Omaha James C. Kaufman California State University at San Bernardino and Jonathan Santo University of Nebraska at Omaha Author note Roni Reiter-Palmon, Department of Psychology and Center for Collaboration Science, University of Nebraska at Omaha; Erika Robinson-Morral, Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska at Omaha; James C. Kaufman, Department of Psychology and Learning Research Institute, California State University at San Bernardino; Jonathan Santo, Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska at Omaha.
    [Show full text]