The History of Neuroscience in Autobiography Volume 11
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Rhythms of the Brain
Rhythms of the Brain György Buzsáki OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Rhythms of the Brain This page intentionally left blank Rhythms of the Brain György Buzsáki 1 2006 3 Oxford University Press, Inc., publishes works that further Oxford University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education. Oxford New York Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto With offices in Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam Copyright © 2006 by Oxford University Press, Inc. Published by Oxford University Press, Inc. 198 Madison Avenue, New York, New York 10016 www.oup.com Oxford is a registered trademark of Oxford University Press All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Oxford University Press. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Buzsáki, G. Rhythms of the brain / György Buzsáki. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN-13 978-0-19-530106-9 ISBN 0-19-530106-4 1. Brain—Physiology. 2. Oscillations. 3. Biological rhythms. [DNLM: 1. Brain—physiology. 2. Cortical Synchronization. 3. Periodicity. WL 300 B992r 2006] I. Title. QP376.B88 2006 612.8'2—dc22 2006003082 987654321 Printed in the United States of America on acid-free paper To my loved ones. This page intentionally left blank Prelude If the brain were simple enough for us to understand it, we would be too sim- ple to understand it. -
Physical Health Worksheet
WORKSHEET # 16 Promoting Health, Vibrancy, & Flourishing Lives Contributed by Elaine O’Brien, PhD, MAPP, CPT [email protected] | www.ElaineOBrienPhD.com o you want to feel more healthy and energetic? LIFESTYLE MEDICINE Don’t we all? Physical activity can help: In Spring 2018, at the inaugural American College D• Engaging in physical activity helps build of Lifestyle Medicine Summit, leaders in health, positive resources and promotes health, vibrancy, medicine, fitness, and well-being, joined forces. They and flourishing lives (Mutrie & Faulkner, 2004). sough to define the empirical, fast-growing science of Lifestyle Medicine. As defined, Lifestyle Medicine • Physical activity, movement, and play are directly encourages: essential to our physical, social, emotional, cognitive well-being and for our development at • Healthful eating of whole plant based foods every age. • Developing strategies to manage stress • Epidemiological data and considerable research • Forming and maintaining positive relationships indicate that physical activity is a major factor in reducing the risk of disease and disability, and for • Improving your sleep improving our well-being. • Cessation of smoking • Increasing physical activity. CALL TO ACTION The rationale is that Lifestyle Medicine not only has A “critical call to action” was made at the United the power to prevent, treat, and reverse disease, but Nations High-Level Meeting on Physical Activity it may also contribute to real health care reform. and Non-Communicable Diseases that I attended in 2011. This summit identified physical activity as “a fast-growing public health problem contributing to a variety of chronic diseases and health complications, including obesity, heart disease, diabetes, hypertension, cancer, depression and anxiety, arthritis, and osteoporosis.” Three urgent, guiding principles were articulated: 1. -
The Anatomy, Physiology and Functions of the Perirhinal Cortex Wendy a Suzuki
179 The anatomy, physiology and functions of the perirhinal cortex Wendy A Suzuki The perirhinal cortex is a polymodal association area that discuss findings from neuroanatomical studies examining contributes importantly to normal recognition memory. the boundaries and connectivity of the perirhinal cortex. A convergence of recent findings from lesion and I will then consider evidence from behavioral and electrophysiological studies has provided new evidence that electrophysiological studies examining the contribution of this area participates in an even broader range of memory this area to a variety of different functions, including functions than previously thought, including associative sensory/perceptual functions, recognition memory, associa- memory and emotional memory, as well as consolidation tive memory, emotional memory and consolidation. functions. These results are consistent with neuroanatomical research showing that this area has strong and reciprocal Neuroanatomy of the perirhinal cortex connections with widespread cortical sensory areas and with Recent neuroanatomical studies in the macaque monkey other memory-related structures, including the hippocampal have revealed that the perirhinal cortex is characterized formation and amygdala. by strong interconnections with diverse unimodal and polymodal cortical association areas, as well as with the hippocampal formation and the amygdala [3,4**,5**]. Less Address information is available concerning the neuroanatomical Laboratory of Neuropsychology, Building 49, Room 1 B80, -
The Creation of Neuroscience
The Creation of Neuroscience The Society for Neuroscience and the Quest for Disciplinary Unity 1969-1995 Introduction rom the molecular biology of a single neuron to the breathtakingly complex circuitry of the entire human nervous system, our understanding of the brain and how it works has undergone radical F changes over the past century. These advances have brought us tantalizingly closer to genu- inely mechanistic and scientifically rigorous explanations of how the brain’s roughly 100 billion neurons, interacting through trillions of synaptic connections, function both as single units and as larger ensem- bles. The professional field of neuroscience, in keeping pace with these important scientific develop- ments, has dramatically reshaped the organization of biological sciences across the globe over the last 50 years. Much like physics during its dominant era in the 1950s and 1960s, neuroscience has become the leading scientific discipline with regard to funding, numbers of scientists, and numbers of trainees. Furthermore, neuroscience as fact, explanation, and myth has just as dramatically redrawn our cultural landscape and redefined how Western popular culture understands who we are as individuals. In the 1950s, especially in the United States, Freud and his successors stood at the center of all cultural expla- nations for psychological suffering. In the new millennium, we perceive such suffering as erupting no longer from a repressed unconscious but, instead, from a pathophysiology rooted in and caused by brain abnormalities and dysfunctions. Indeed, the normal as well as the pathological have become thoroughly neurobiological in the last several decades. In the process, entirely new vistas have opened up in fields ranging from neuroeconomics and neurophilosophy to consumer products, as exemplified by an entire line of soft drinks advertised as offering “neuro” benefits. -
May 9-12 Rotterdam Netherlands
2018 ANNUAL MEETING MAY 9-12 ROTTERDAM NETHERLANDS PROGRAM BOOK www.autism-insar.org INSAR 2018 Sponsors We thank the following organizations for their generous support of the INSAR Annual Meeting. Platinum Sponsor Level Gold Sponsor Level Silver Sponsor Level Autism Science Foundation Hilibrand Foundation Nancy Lurie Marks Family Foundation TABLE OF CONTENTS Sponsorship .................................Inside Front Cover TABLE OF CONTENTS Special Interest Groups Schedule .......................... 6 Speaker Ready Room ............................................ 6 De Doelen Floor Plans ........................................ 7-9 Meeting Information Schedule-At-A-Glance .................................... 10-12 In-Conjunction Events .................................... 13-14 Keynote Speakers .............................................. 15 Awardees ..................................................... 16-19 INSAR MISSION Acknowledgments .......................................... 20-21 STATEMENT To promote the highest quality INSAR Summer Institute .................................... 22 research in order to improve the Abstract Author Index ...................................... 134 lives of people affected by autism. General Information .......................................... 208 Exhibitors ....................................................... 210 Strategic Initiatives Setting the Bar: Increase the quality, AM diversity and relevance of research promoted through annual meetings, journal, Keynote Address ............................................... -
Are Systems Neuroscience Explanations Mechanistic?
Are Systems Neuroscience Explanations Mechanistic? Carlos Zednik [email protected] Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Osnabrück 49069 Osnabrück, Germany Paper to be presented at: Philosophy of Science Association 24th Biennial Meeting (Chicago, IL), November 2014 Abstract Whereas most branches of neuroscience are thought to provide mechanistic explanations, systems neuroscience is not. Two reasons are traditionally cited in support of this conclusion. First, systems neuroscientists rarely, if ever, rely on the dual strategies of decomposition and localization. Second, they typically emphasize organizational properties over the properties of individual components. In this paper, I argue that neither reason is conclusive: researchers might rely on alternative strategies for mechanism discovery, and focusing on organization is often appropriate and consistent with the norms of mechanistic explanation. Thus, many explanations in systems neuroscience can also be viewed as mechanistic explanations. 1 1. Introduction There is a widespread consensus in philosophy of science that neuroscientists provide mechanistic explanations. That is, they seek the discovery and description of the mechanisms responsible for the behavioral and neurological phenomena being explained. This consensus is supported by a growing philosophical literature on past and present examples from various branches of neuroscience, including molecular (Craver 2007; Machamer, Darden, and Craver 2000), cognitive (Bechtel 2008; Kaplan and Craver 2011), and computational -
Entorhinal Cortex Lesions Disrupt the Relational Organization of Memory in Monkeys
The Journal of Neuroscience, November 3, 2004 • 24(44):9811–9825 • 9811 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Entorhinal Cortex Lesions Disrupt the Relational Organization of Memory in Monkeys Cindy A. Buckmaster,1,3 Howard Eichenbaum,4 David G. Amaral,5 Wendy A. Suzuki,6 and Peter R. Rapp1,2 1Fishberg Department of Neuroscience and 2Kastor Neurobiology of Aging Laboratories, Department of Geriatrics and Adult Development, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, New York 10029-6574, 3Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, State University of New York at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, New York 11794, 4Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts 02215, 5The Medical Investigation of Neurodevelopmental Disorders Institute, University of California, Davis, California 95817, and 6Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003 Recent accounts suggest that the hippocampal system critically supports two central characteristics of episodic memory: the ability to establish and maintain representations for the salient relationships between experienced events (relational representation) and the capacity to flexibly manipulate memory (flexible memory expression). To test this proposal in monkeys, intact controls and subjects with bilateral aspiration lesions of the entorhinal cortex were trained postoperatively on two standard memory tasks, delayed nonmatching- to-sample (DNMS) and two-choice object discrimination (OD) learning, and three procedures intended to emphasize relational repre- sentation and flexible memory expression: a paired associate (PA) task, a transitive inference (TI) test of learning and memory for hierarchical stimulus relationships, and a spatial delayed recognition span (SDRS) procedure. The latter assessments each included critical “probe” tests that asked monkeys to evaluate the relationships among previously learned stimuli presented in novel combina- tions. -
How Big Is Human Memory, Or on Being Just Useful Enough
Downloaded from learnmem.cshlp.org on September 29, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press REVIEW Yadin Dudai How Big Is Human Memory, Department of Neur0bi010gy or On Being Just Useful Enough The Weizmann Institute of Science Reh0v0t 76100 Israel We are, in many respects, what we remember. But how much do we do? So far, science has provided only a very partial answer to this riddle. The magical number seven, plus or minus two, seems to constrain the capacity of our immediate memory (Miller 1956). But surely its constraints dissipate when memories settle in long-term stores. Yet how big are these stores? If we combine all of our factual knowledge and personal reminiscence, childhood scenes and memories of the past day, intimate experiences and professional expertisemhow many items are there, that, combined together, mold us into unique individuals? The answer is not simple, and neither is the question. For example, what is an item in long-term memory? And how can we measure it, being sure that we unveil memory capacity and not merely the occasional ability to tap it? Such theoretical and practical difficulties, no doubt, have contributed to the fact that the capacity of human memory is still an enigma. Yet, despite the inherent and undeniable complexities, the issue deserves to be retrieved, once in a while, from the oblivions of the collective memory of the scientific community. (For a selection of earlier discussions of the size of human long-term memory, see Galton 1879; Landauer 1986; Crovitz et al. 1991.) Folk Psychology and When confronted with the issue, many tend to provide an intuitive Early Views estimate of the size of their memory, based either on belief or introspection or both. -
Systems Neuroscience of Mathematical Cognition and Learning
CHAPTER 15 Systems Neuroscience of Mathematical Cognition and Learning: Basic Organization and Neural Sources of Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development Teresa Iuculano, Aarthi Padmanabhan, Vinod Menon Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States OUTLINE Introduction 288 Ventral and Dorsal Visual Streams: Neural Building Blocks of Mathematical Cognition 292 Basic Organization 292 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 295 Parietal-Frontal Systems: Short-Term and Working Memory 296 Basic Organization 296 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 299 Lateral Frontotemporal Cortices: Language-Mediated Systems 302 Basic Organization 302 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 302 Heterogeneity of Function in Numerical Cognition 287 https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-811529-9.00015-7 © 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 288 15. SYSTEMS NEUROSCIENCE OF MATHEMATICAL COGNITION AND LEARNING The Medial Temporal Lobe: Declarative Memory 306 Basic Organization 306 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 306 The Circuit View: Attention and Control Processes and Dynamic Circuits Orchestrating Mathematical Learning 310 Basic Organization 310 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 312 Plasticity in Multiple Brain Systems: Relation to Learning 314 Basic Organization 314 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 315 Conclusions and Future Directions 320 References 324 INTRODUCTION Mathematical skill acquisition is hierarchical in nature, and each iteration of increased proficiency builds on knowledge of a lower-level primitive. For example, learning to solve arithmetical operations such as “3 + 4” requires first an understanding of what numbers mean and rep- resent (e.g., the symbol “3” refers to the quantity of three items, which derives from the ability to attend to discrete items in the environment). -
A Meta-Analysis of Functional MRI Results
RESEARCH Cooperating yet distinct brain networks engaged during naturalistic paradigms: A meta-analysis of functional MRI results 1 1 1 2 Katherine L. Bottenhorn , Jessica S. Flannery , Emily R. Boeving , Michael C. Riedel , 3,4 1 2 Simon B. Eickhoff , Matthew T. Sutherland , and Angela R. Laird 1Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA 2Department of Physics, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA 3Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/netn/article-pdf/3/1/27/1092290/netn_a_00050.pdf by guest on 01 October 2021 4Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Brain & Behaviour (INM-7), Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany an open access journal Keywords: Neuroimaging meta-analysis, Naturalistic paradigms, Clustering analysis, Neuro- informatics ABSTRACT Cognitive processes do not occur by pure insertion and instead depend on the full complement of co-occurring mental processes, including perceptual and motor functions. As such, there is limited ecological validity to human neuroimaging experiments that use Citation: Bottenhorn, K. L., Flannery, highly controlled tasks to isolate mental processes of interest. However, a growing literature J. S., Boeving, E. R., Riedel, M. C., Eickhoff, S. B., Sutherland, M. T., & shows how dynamic, interactive tasks have allowed researchers to study cognition as it more Laird, A. R. (2019). Cooperating yet naturally occurs. Collective analysis across such neuroimaging experiments may answer distinct brain networks engaged during naturalistic paradigms: broader questions regarding how naturalistic cognition is biologically distributed throughout A meta-analysis of functional MRI k results. Network Neuroscience, the brain. We applied an unbiased, data-driven, meta-analytic approach that uses -means 3(1), 27–48. -
Systems Processes and Pathologies: Creating an Integrated Framework for Systems Science
IS13-SysProc&Path-Troncale.docx Page 1 of 24 Systems Processes and Pathologies: Creating An Integrated Framework for Systems Science Dr. Len Troncale Professor Emeritus and Past Chair, Biological Sciences Director, Institute for Advanced Systems Science California State Polytechnic University, Pomona, California, 91711 [email protected] Copyright © 2013 by Len Troncale. Published and used by INCOSE with permission Abstract. Among the several official projects of the INCOSE Systems Science Working Group, one focuses on integrating the plethora of systems theories, sources, approaches, and tools developed over the past half-century with the purpose of enabling a new and unified “science” of systems as a fundamental basis for SE. Another seeks to develop a much more SE-usable Systems Pathology also grounded in a “science” of systems. This paper introduces the wider SE community to the current status of this unique knowledge base produced over the past three years by an INCOSE-ISSS alliance summarizing the current output of 7 Workshops, 12 Papers, >24 Presentations or Webinars, and 5 Reports. It describes the need for integration of systems knowledge by demonstrating the extensive fragmentation of numerous contributing fields. It presents the current 12-step “protocol” used by the current group to guide its efforts at synthesis across systems domains, disciplines, tools, and scales asking for feedback to improve the approach. It introduces 15 Working Assumptions or Hypotheses that form the foundation for this attempt at unification citing why these could be used as working principles but why it may be undesirable to call them “principles” as others often do. The paper presents working frameworks for integration and criteria used to judge whether results are a “science” of systems or not with reminders that these early guidelines are being subjected to constant testing and revision. -
An Enlightening Conversation on Autism « Dana Foundation
An Enlightening Conversation on Autism « Dana Foundation https://danablog.org/2016/02/10/an-enlightening-conversation-o... An Enlightening Conversation on Autism FEBRUARY 10, 2016 An hour before a program titled “The Spectrum” began, featured participant David Amaral, Ph.D., complained that in the weeks leading up to the event, he had had trouble getting his hands on Snow Cake, a 2007 film in which Sigourney Weaver plays a high-functioning autistic woman. “I tried Netflix, Amazon Prime, and Hulu, and couldn’t find it anywhere,” said Amaral. “But I was eventually able to find the DVD on Amazon—and liked it very much.” That’s high praise coming from one of the nation’s foremost researchers on autism. Amaral, UC Davis Distinguished Professor of Psychiatry and Neuroscience and the Beneto Foundation chair, and founding research director of the M.I.N.D. Institute, and Weaver, an acclaimed actress, film producer, and noted environmentalist, discussed autism to a sold-out audience at the Rubin Museum of Art in Manhattan. Lyn Hughes Photography, courtesy of the Rubin Museum of Art. The program—the second in an annual Brainwave series that runs through April—opened with a clip from Snow Cake [here’s a short clip via YouTube]. Despite a review in the New York Times that called Weaver’s performance “convincingly precise,” few in the audience had seen or heard of the film. Brain- centric films, even when they are lauded for extraordinary performances (Concussion with Will Smith and Still Alice with Julianne Moore come to mind), rarely live up to box office expectations.