Interpersonal Society : Essays on Shared Beliefs, Trust, Mnemonic

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Interpersonal Society : Essays on Shared Beliefs, Trust, Mnemonic Interpersonal Society i ii Interpersonal Society: Essays on Shared Beliefs, Trust, Mnemonic Oppression, Distributive Fairness, and Value Creation Per Henrik Hedberg iii Dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy, Ph.D., in Business Administration Stockholm School of Economics 2012 Keywords: social construction, social memory, shared reality, discrimination, prejudice, distributive fairness, negotiation, individual differences, organization Interpersonal Society: Essays on Shared Beliefs, Trust, Mnemonic Oppres- sion, Distributive Fairness, and Value Creation © SSE and the author, 2012 ISBN 978-91-7258-867-7 Cover by: John Mellkvist Photo by: Claes Henrik Hedberg Printed in Sweden by: Ineko, Göteborg 2012 Distributed by: The Research Secretariat Stockholm School of Economics Box 6501, SE-113 83 Stockholm, Sweden www.hhs.se iv Preface This report is a result of a research project carried out at the Department of Marketing and Strategy at the Stockholm School of Economics (SSE). This volume is submitted as a doctor’s thesis at SSE. The author has been entirely free to conduct and present his research in his own ways as an expression of his own ideas. SSE is grateful for the financial support provided by a grant from Jan Wallanders och Tom Hedelius Stiftelse which contributed toward the completion of the project. Göran Lindqvist Richard Wahlund Director of Research Professor and Head of the Stockholm School of Economics Department of Marketing and Strategy Stockholm School of Economics v vi Acknowledgements Writing the present dissertation has been a lonelier affair than when I did it the first time, as my research topics this time were quite different from what the people around me are working on. Nonetheless, looking back, there are a remarkable number of colleagues and friends that have helped me in various ways. My advisor Richard Wahlund has been a steady support. For several years he has been available with encouragement and coaching. Even though my research questions have been quite different from his, Richard has main- tained a loyal, open and interested attitude, and engaged me in useful discus- sions and offered helpful advice at several stages of the research process. I have been fortunate to work in the vicinity of two people in academia who take their leadership responsibility seriously. Richard is one; Anders Liljenberg is another. Anders has been an inspiration, both as a leader and as a careful mentor in leadership. Also, Anders explicitly encouraged me to finish my dissertation project and offered support by even taking on part of my re- sponsibilities when it was needed. Balancing administration, teaching, and re- search was a distraction, so I am very grateful for the extended hand, Anders. Indeed, as many know who have experienced the Russian adventure at SSE, it can be challenging to handle at times, and I am grateful to the many individuals, friends, in our St. Petersburg office, and Moscow, who make me and everyone else feel welcome. Olga, Tatiana, Igor, Anna, Irina Z, Alexey, Pavel, Seva K, Seva S, Evgeny, Katja, Elena L, Liza, Jane, Irina A, Irina B, Vika, Alexander, Julia, Grigory, Lyubov, Olga Le, Julia E, Irina A, Tatiana S, Elena A, Yana… (plus the stellar Stockholm and Småland group of Ciara, Mats, Katerina, Lars, Jörgen, Alena and Sergey). I am looking forward toward being able to spend more time in St. Petersburg again now that the intense end-game of the dissertation project is coming to an end. My journey has been facilitated also by the knowledgeable and helpful people on my dissertation committee. Magnus Johannesson has been a steady force to rely on for feedback with the speed of light. His reassuring and pre- cise observations from an economics viewpoint have been very helpful. Mag- nus and Richard have been paralleled by Linnda Caporael as the third member of the committee and the invisible Keyser Soze in this lineup. Although we had never met, Linnda agreed to serve on my committee after we had ex- changed a few emails. When we have been in touch during the last couple of years, she has provided much needed expertise in not only evolutionary pro- vii cesses, which I eventually pursued less than I had thought I would, but also in relativistic ontologies, social memories, real social cognition, and other things that I wish I had had more time to pursue in the dissertation but which in any case benefitted from Linnda’s wise advice (which she provided at the end even in the face of her own book project’s “drop dead” deadlines looming). Looking forward to seeing you one day, Linnda! Because this is a dissertation in business administration, I benefitted greatly from my interactions with my business mentor: Henrik Glimstedt. Surely there is still much (all?) in the dissertation that he will disapprove of, but Henrik generously and confidently guided me on authoritative journeys to various business destinations. Thank you for your tough love and advice in business, as well for your careful attention to detail in feedback on excerpts of my text. (Henrik convinced me that I needed a single theme as an entry into the introductory chapter, so the reader may be grateful to him too.) In this context I would also like to thank my long-time colleague, corri- dor neighbor, friend, and brother in arms, Patric Andersson, who is always available with well-intended, careful, and reliable advice, valuable feedback, and fun comments. May we have many more years together, Patric! Many other helpful people in the corridor should be mentioned. To Emre Yildiz for friendly and contagiously enthusiastic and knowledgeable ad- vice, assistance, and guidance in most matters including organization theory, but also for thoughtful feedback and fun discussions about anything including religion. Thanks to Göran Lindqvist for advice on a number of practical issues the value of which increased dramatically as they deadline drew closer. To Patric Regnér for quick and knowledgeable tips in the corridor. To Udo Zan- der for contacting Madan Pillutla, my dissertation discussant. For advice about book layout, I owe thanks to additional people in the corridor (Erik, Mattias and Emelie), as well as my esthetically inclined buddy Kristoffer and relatives (Maria, Claes Henrik, Carl Fredrik, and Charlotte). Among other people at SSE, I am grateful to Jan Eklöf for helpful talks about statistical analyses. Outside of SSE, I have benefitted from Richard Swedberg’s introduction to economic sociology and general encouragement. Jan Tullberg has shared his wide-ranging perspectives on matters including fairness, morality and ide- ology. Additional friends in various disciplines inside and outside of academia have contributed in many and various ways. Markus Ejenäs provided early orientations in organization theory during epic taxi rides on Eastern adven- tures. Anna Dreber Almenberg has provided precise feedback and engaged viii me in fun and stimulating lunch discussions. Torun Lindholm gracefully agreed to read a very rough version of the introductory chapter. Andreas Ols- son and I have had many stimulating lunches on wide-ranging topics, and he has also provided precise feedback in relation to learning and ecology. To Kristoffer Strandqvist for his perennial readiness to provide reality checks and advice aimed at saving me from ill-fated mistakes. To Ludvig Beckman for his quick and knowledgeable guides to political science references, and patience with me despite my misguided pre-occupation with empirical questions. You need people the most when you know the least about what you are doing. My cousin, John Mellkvist, has been a lighthouse of strength in book- cover esthetics. Although the process took place at the same time that he es- tablished a new business (SWE Design), he designed the cover of the book and endured a high number of my ignorant and doubtlessly annoying “correc- tions” of earlier drafts. My father, too, has been very helpful toward the end of the thesis pro- ject. Besides his constant willingness to handle emergencies (and only occa- sionally create them), he is nowadays weighing in not only on layout and photography but has even helped me to proofread reference lists. Mom is a constant support of me and my research, and has saved me on a number of occasions toward the end by offering last minute care of her grand-children. Thanks also to Torborg and Torbjörn for your enthusiastic caretaking. Sonia and Malkolm, my shockingly grownup darlings, you have had to endure many evenings lately that I have not been at home; now that will change, and I can’t wait to enjoy the summer with you. My companion in this, as in all things, is my good and beloved Maria. I am very grateful that you provided room for me to focus on the dissertation when I needed it the most. Although it is not much in the way of compensa- tion, the work is dedicated to you and Sonia and Malkolm. ix x Table of Contents 1. Overview .................................................................................................................................... 3 2. Business Contexts ...................................................................................................................... 4 2.1 Business and Business studies............................................................................................. 6 3. Theoretical Problems ................................................................................................................ 10 3.1 Evolutionary perspectives ................................................................................................
Recommended publications
  • Ms. Scott's A.P. Psychology Summer Assignment Due on the First Day Of
    Ms. Scott’s A.P. Psychology Summer Assignment due on the first day of class, August 12th. If you have any questions, please email me at [email protected]. Summer Assignment 2019-2020 “You Are Not So Smart” by David McRaney ***Note: you can order this book through amazon.com and many other resources online, but you can also ask your local libraries to get it for you. If you have trouble attaining this book, email me asap because I can assist you with this. Humans like to think that we are rational and logical beings, however decades of research in cognitive psychology has proven otherwise. The book, “You are Not so Smart” by David McRaney explores the failings of memory, how we do things without having a clue why we are doing them, and the narratives we construct and tell ourselves to make us feel better about our choices and decisions. This book is a fabulous summer read for many reasons. First of all, we will be learning about every single concept presented in the book at some point this year. Second of all, each chapter is very short and engaging. Best of all, as you read, your mind will be flooded with memories and personal experiences that connect with the concepts and stories presented in the book. After reading the entire book, please complete these two assignments: 1. There are many very important AP Psychology concepts presented in the book. Complete the attached chart where you define each concept using your own words and provide a specific example from the book or in your own life.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Psychology
    COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY PSYCH 126 Acknowledgements College of the Canyons would like to extend appreciation to the following people and organizations for allowing this textbook to be created: California Community Colleges Chancellor’s Office Chancellor Diane Van Hook Santa Clarita Community College District College of the Canyons Distance Learning Office In providing content for this textbook, the following professionals were invaluable: Mehgan Andrade, who was the major contributor and compiler of this work and Neil Walker, without whose help the book could not have been completed. Special Thank You to Trudi Radtke for editing, formatting, readability, and aesthetics. The contents of this textbook were developed under the Title V grant from the Department of Education (Award #P031S140092). However, those contents do not necessarily represent the policy of the Department of Education, and you should not assume endorsement by the Federal Government. Unless otherwise noted, the content in this textbook is licensed under CC BY 4.0 Table of Contents Psychology .................................................................................................................................................... 1 126 ................................................................................................................................................................ 1 Chapter 1 - History of Cognitive Psychology ............................................................................................. 7 Definition of Cognitive Psychology
    [Show full text]
  • Infographic I.10
    The Digital Health Revolution: Leaving No One Behind The global AI in healthcare market is growing fast, with an expected increase from $4.9 billion in 2020 to $45.2 billion by 2026. There are new solutions introduced every day that address all areas: from clinical care and diagnosis, to remote patient monitoring to EHR support, and beyond. But, AI is still relatively new to the industry, and it can be difficult to determine which solutions can actually make a difference in care delivery and business operations. 59 Jan 2021 % of Americans believe returning Jan-June 2019 to pre-coronavirus life poses a risk to health and well being. 11 41 % % ...expect it will take at least 6 The pandemic has greatly increased the 65 months before things get number of US adults reporting depression % back to normal (updated April and/or anxiety.5 2021).4 Up to of consumers now interested in telehealth going forward. $250B 76 57% of providers view telehealth more of current US healthcare spend % favorably than they did before COVID-19.7 could potentially be virtualized.6 The dramatic increase in of Medicare primary care visits the conducted through 90% $3.5T telehealth has shown longevity, with rates in annual U.S. health expenditures are for people with chronic and mental health conditions. since April 2020 0.1 43.5 leveling off % % Most of these can be prevented by simple around 30%.8 lifestyle changes and regular health screenings9 Feb. 2020 Apr. 2020 OCCAM’S RAZOR • CONJUNCTION FALLACY • DELMORE EFFECT • LAW OF TRIVIALITY • COGNITIVE FLUENCY • BELIEF BIAS • INFORMATION BIAS Digital health ecosystems are transforming• AMBIGUITY BIAS • STATUS medicineQUO BIAS • SOCIAL COMPARISONfrom BIASa rea• DECOYctive EFFECT • REACTANCEdiscipline, • REVERSE PSYCHOLOGY • SYSTEM JUSTIFICATION • BACKFIRE EFFECT • ENDOWMENT EFFECT • PROCESSING DIFFICULTY EFFECT • PSEUDOCERTAINTY EFFECT • DISPOSITION becoming precise, preventive,EFFECT • ZERO-RISK personalized, BIAS • UNIT BIAS • IKEA EFFECT and • LOSS AVERSION participatory.
    [Show full text]
  • Overview and History of Memory Research
    M01_RADV4825_02_SE_CH01.QXD 10/16/10 10:41 AM Page 1 CHAPTER ONE OVERVIEW AND HISTORY OF MEMORY RESEARCH Memory is perhaps the most central aspect of human thought. Any question about human behavior, cognition, development, and nature requires an understanding of mem- ory. Our memory makes us who we are, and it is one of the most intimate parts of our- selves. This may be why when we get close to someone, when we want them to know who we are and we want to know who they are, there is a sharing of memories. Many feel that the study of human memory is the closest one can get to a systematic study of the human soul. The aim of this book is to provide you, the student, with a survey and guide to what is known about human memory. As with most courses, there are a number of facts and ideas to learn. However, as any good professor will tell you, the slow accu- mulation of facts is not the main point of course work. The primary aim is to provide you with a deeper understanding and appreciation of some aspect of the world—and, hopefully, yourself. I trust that the ideas presented in this book will be useful in your life after this course is completed. A SMATTERING OF DEFINITIONS Before diving into the subject matter, we need to establish some points. Specifically, we need to define how the terms memory and learning are used. The primary subject of this book is, of course, memory. So what is memory? Well, the problem, and the beauty, of this term is that it has many meanings.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Embrace Your Cognitive Bias
    1 Embrace Your Cognitive Bias http://blog.beaufortes.com/2007/06/embrace-your-co.html Cognitive Biases are distortions in the way humans see things in comparison to the purely logical way that mathematics, economics, and yes even project management would have us look at things. The problem is not that we have them… most of them are wired deep into our brains following millions of years of evolution. The problem is that we don’t know about them, and consequently don’t take them into account when we have to make important decisions. (This area is so important that Daniel Kahneman won a Nobel Prize in 2002 for work tying non-rational decision making, and cognitive bias, to mainstream economics) People don’t behave rationally, they have emotions, they can be inspired, they have cognitive bias! Tying that into how we run projects (project leadership as a compliment to project management) can produce results you wouldn’t believe. You have to know about them to guard against them, or use them (but that’s another article)... So let’s get more specific. After the jump, let me show you a great list of cognitive biases. I’ll bet that there are at least a few that you haven’t heard of before! Decision making and behavioral biases Bandwagon effect — the tendency to do (or believe) things because many other people do (or believe) the same. Related to groupthink, herd behaviour, and manias. Bias blind spot — the tendency not to compensate for one’s own cognitive biases. Choice-supportive bias — the tendency to remember one’s choices as better than they actually were.
    [Show full text]
  • An Early History of Hindsight Research
    Social Cognition, Vol. 25, No. 1, 2007, pp. 10-13 FISCHHOFFEARLY HISTORY OF HINDSIGHT RESEARCH AN EARLY HISTORY OF HINDSIGHT RESEARCH Baruch Fischhoff Carnegie Mellon University The first studies of hindsight bias reflected the confluence of two desires. One arose from being part of the heady early days of Amos Tversky and Danny Kahneman’s heuristics–and–biases research program. As the para- digm evolved, the challenge for the participating graduate students was to find a heuristic to call one’s own or to find a way to elaborate one of the three “classics” (availability, representativeness, anchoring, and adjustment). Maya Bar Hillel and Ruth Beyth–Marom chose the latter route. However, I was still struggling to reconcile the political motives that had brought me to Israel, planning to live in a kibbutz for the rest of my life, with academic life—and its more realistic view of the pace of change in human affairs. For one meeting of the seminar, we read Paul Meehl’s (1973) “Why I Do Not Attend Case Conferences.” One of his many insights concerned clini- cians’ exaggerated feeling of having known all along how cases were going to turn out. To me, this sounded a lot like the exaggerated claims of under- standing political processes that permeated the political discussions to which I had long subjected myself. Those discussions often left me wonder- ing, “If we’re so prescient, why aren’t we running the world?” Psychological research provided an opportunity (and an obligation) to discipline such ob- servations with systematically collected evidence, interpreted in the context of evolving theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory for Memory
    Memory & Cognition 2001, 29 (6), 789-797 Memory for memory SUSAN JOSLYN, ELIZABETH LOFTUS, AMANDA MCNOUGHTON, and JAYME POWERS University of Washington, Seattle, Washington Participantsread short passages and 1 day later they answered questions via telephone about the pas- sages (text facts) and about the experimental session (event facts). They were telephoned again 6 weeks later and answered the same questions about text and event facts. They also answered new questions about whether they remembered the answers they had givenin the initial telephone interview (recall for prior memory performance). Although participants accurately remembered the majority of past memory successes, they were poor at remembering past memory failures. After being provided with the correct answer and tested again, the participants’ performance improved somewhat, espe- cially for memory failures. This suggests that some errors in recalling past forgetting might have been due to correctly remembering the answer previously given, but failing to realizethat it had been wrong. These findings have implications for a variety of situations in which people are queried about past memory performance. How well do people remember whether they remem- Self-evaluation of general memory abilities is presum- bered something in the past? If someone claims to have ably based on memory for past memory performance. forgotten something until recently, such as childhood Some evidence suggests that self-reported memory abil- sexual abuse (CSA), how accurate is that claim? It is im- ities are not related to scores on subsequent laboratory portant to note that this is a question not about the accu- administered memory tests. This is especially true of racy of the claim of abuse but rather about the accuracy people with memory impairments (e.g., Alzheimer’s), of the claim of forgetting.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Expertise on the Hindsight Bias
    The Effects of Expertise on the Hindsight Bias A Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Melissa A.Z. Marks Knoll, B.A., M.A. Graduate Program in Psychology * * * * * The Ohio State University 2009 Dissertation Committee: Dr. Hal R. Arkes, Advisor Dr. Thomas E. Nygren Dr. Michael C. Edwards ABSTRACT I present data from three experiments in which I explored the effects of expertise on the hindsight bias. In Experiment 1 participants read an essay about baseball or about owning a dog and then answered a 20-question true/false quiz about the baseball essay to the best of their ability (do-your-best group), as if they had not read the essay (discount group), or to the best of their ability even though they read about owning a dog (dogs group). Participants also completed a quiz about baseball rules (measure of expertise). Results demonstrated that as participants’ baseball expertise increased, their inability to act as if they had never read the essay also increased; expertise exacerbated hindsight bias. In Experiment 2, varsity baseball players and baseball non-experts answered a 20- question quiz about baseball current events. Foresight participants answered the questions, while hindsight participants were given the questions and the answers and had to give the probability that they would have known the answers had the answers not been provided. The baseball players displayed no hindsight bias, while non-experts demonstrated the bias. To test of the effects of subjective expertise on hindsight bias, participants in Experiment 3 ranked five topics in order of expertise and gave feeling-of- knowing (FOK) ratings for 100 questions from these topics.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: HINDSIGHT BIAS and DEPRESSION 1 in Press, Clinical
    Running head: HINDSIGHT BIAS AND DEPRESSION 1 In press, Clinical Psychological Science Hindsight Bias in Depression Julia Groß Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf Hartmut Blank University of Portsmouth Ute J. Bayen Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf Author Note Julia Groß, Institute for Experimental Psychology, Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany; Hartmut Blank, Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, U.K.; Ute Johanna Bayen, Institute for Experimental Psychology, Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany. We thank Siegmund Switala, Albert-Georg Lang, and Lennart Friese for technical assistance, and Anna-Christina Breuer, Doris Dahlke, and Laura Richter for help with data collection. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Julia Groß, Heinrich- Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, Institute for Experimental Psychology, Building 23.02., Universitätsstr. 1, D-40225 Germany, E-Mail: [email protected]. HINDSIGHT BIAS AND DEPRESSION 2 Abstract People tend to be biased by outcome knowledge when looking back on events. This phenomenon is known as hindsight bias. Clinical intuition and theoretical accounts of affect- regulatory functions of hindsight bias suggest a link between hindsight bias and depression, but empirical evidence is scarce. In two experiments, participants with varying levels of depressive symptoms imagined themselves in everyday scenarios that ended positively or negatively, and completed hindsight and affect measures. Participants with higher levels of depression judged negative outcomes, but not positive outcomes, as more foreseeable and more inevitable in hindsight. They also recalled prior expectations about outcomes as more negative for negative, but not for positive outcomes. This memory hindsight bias was accompanied by disappointment, suggesting a relation to affect-regulatory malfunction.
    [Show full text]
  • Hindsight Bias: a By-Product of Knowledge Updating? Ulrich Hoffrage, Ralph Hertwig, and Gerd Gigerenzer Max Planck Institute for Human Development
    Joemal of ~ntal Psychology: Copyright 2000 by the American PsychologicalAssociation, Inc. Learning, Memory,and Cognition 0278-7393/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.3.566 2000, Vol. 26, No. 3, 566-581 Hindsight Bias: A By-Product of Knowledge Updating? Ulrich Hoffrage, Ralph Hertwig, and Gerd Gigerenzer Max Planck Institute for Human Development With the benefit of feedback about the outcome of an event, people's recalled judgments are typically closer to the outcome of the event than their original judgments were. It has been suggested that this hindsight bias may be due to a reconstruction process of the prior judgment. A model of such a process is proposed that assumes that knowledge is updated after feedback and that reconstruction is based on the updated knowledge. Consistent with the model's predictions, the results of 2 studies show that knowledge after feedback is systematically shifted toward feedback, and that assisting retrieval of the knowledge prior to feedback reduces hindsight bias. In addition, the model accounts for about 75% of cases in which either hindsight bias or reversed hindsight bias occurred. The authors conclude that hindsight bias can be understood as a by-product of an adaptive process, namely the updating of knowledge after feedback. In attempting to understand the past, historians have to outcome of an event, should differ systematically from deal with a number of methodological problems. The foresight judgments, that is, judgments made without knowl- problem that concerns us here stems from what Leo Tolstoy edge of the outcome. Indeed, this is what Fischhoff (1975) (186911982) described in War and Peace as the "law of found.
    [Show full text]
  • Hindsight Bias in Judgements of Dating Couples
    Hindsight Bias in Judgements of Dating Couples Kai Nguyen Dr. April Bleske-Rechek, Department of Psychology Abstract Hindsight bias, also known as the “I knew it all along” effect, exaggerates our ability to understand our initial perceptions and predictions after something has already occurred. Although hindsight bias is a universal cognitive phenomenon, there are cultural differences in how it is exhibited. Hindsight bias has been studied in various contexts, such as clinical diagnoses, rape, and eyewitness testimonies, but has yet to be assessed in a romantic context. When considering romantic situations, individual differences, such as gender and sex, can influence how the relationship is perceived. Considering these individual differences, hindsight bias in relationships should be assessed from an outside perspective to minimize the effect of potential distortions. Introduction Hindsight bias is a phenomenon that is often discussed in the field of psychology. Hindsight bias, also known as the “I knew it all along” effect, tends to exaggerate our ability to understand our initial perceptions and predictions after something has already occurred. This phenomenon occurs in our everyday lives in various contexts, ranging from clinical diagnoses to relationship dissolutions. For instance, upon dissolution of a romantic relationship, we are able to identify red flags and we then blame ourselves for not seeing these signs earlier because they seem so obvious now that the relationship is over. As obvious as these signs may seem after the relationship is over, in reality it is actually quite difficult for us to predict how the relationship will turn out. We encounter innumerable ambiguous signals as we move through our lives, but once something happens, we are able to sort out these signals and find ourselves believing that we knew it all along or that we saw it coming.
    [Show full text]
  • Unspringing the Witness Memory and Demeanor Trap: What Every Judge and Juror Needs to Know About Cognitive Psychology and Witness Credibility
    Unspringing The Witness Memory and Demeanor Trap: What Every Judge And Juror Needs to Know About Cognitive Psychology And Witness Credibility By: Mark W. Bennett Unspringing The Witness Memory and Demeanor Trap: What Every Judge And Juror Needs to Know About Cognitive Psychology And Witness Credibility 1 Mark W. Bennett The soul of America’s civil and criminal justice systems is the ability of jurors and judges to accurately determine the facts of a dispute. This invariably implicates the credibility of witnesses. In making credibility determinations, jurors and judges necessarily decide the accuracy of witnesses’ memories and the effect of the witnesses’ demeanor on their credibility. Almost all jurisdictions’ pattern jury instructions about witness credibility explain nothing about how a witness’s memories for events and conversations work—and how startlingly fallible memories actually are. They simply instruct the jurors to consider the witness’s “memory”—with no additional guidance. Similarly, the same pattern jury instructions on demeanor seldom do more than ask jurors to speculate about a witness’s demeanor by instructing them to merely observe “the manner of the witness” while testifying. Yet, thousands of cognitive psychological studies have provided major insights into witness memory and demeanor. The resulting cognitive psychological principles that are now widely accepted as the gold standard about witness memory and demeanor are often contrary to what jurors intuitively, but wrongly, believe. Most jurors believe that memory works like a video camera that can perfectly recall the details of past events. Rather, memory is more like a Wikipedia page where you can go in and change it, but so can others.
    [Show full text]