Neuroscience Research on Human Visual Path Integration: Empirical Overview and Strategic Considerations

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Neuroscience Research on Human Visual Path Integration: Empirical Overview and Strategic Considerations Report No. MAE0820 Neuroscience Research on Human Visual Path Integration: Empirical Overview and Strategic Considerations Jimmy Y. Zhong12 1School of Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering (MAE), Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 639798, Singapore 2 GSU/GT Center for Advanced Brain Imaging (CABI), Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA 30318, USA Email: [email protected] Report No. MAE0820 1 Foreword 2 3 This manuscript is based on modifications and extensions of copyrighted writings of the author 4 that were previously unpublished in any book or journal. It is intended as a 5 conceptual/theoretical piece of writing and should not be regarded as a research report or article 6 of any kind. All written opinions and recommendations expressed herein belong solely to the 7 author and do not belong to any other person or organization. If deemed useful, please cite and 8 reference this paper in its original form: 9 10 Zhong, J. Y. (2021, August). (Technical Report No. MAE0820). Neuroscience research on 11 human visual path integration: Empirical overview and strategic considerations. School of 12 Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore. 13 https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/h6u3b i Report No. MAE0820 14 Abstract 15 16 Over the past two decades, many neuroimaging studies have attempted to uncover the brain 17 regions and networks involved in path integration and identify the underlying neurocognitive 18 mechanisms. Although these studies made inroads into the neural basis of path integration, they 19 have yet to offer a full disclosure of the functional specialization of the brain regions supporting 20 path integration. In this paper, I reviewed notable neuroscientific studies on visual path 21 integration in humans, identified the commonalities and discrepancies in their findings, and 22 incorporated fresh insights from recent path integration studies. Specifically, this paper presented 23 neuroscientific studies performed with virtual renditions of the triangle/path completion task and 24 addressed whether or not the hippocampus is necessary for human path integration. Based on 25 studies that showed evidence supporting and negating the involvement of the hippocampal 26 formation in path integration, this paper introduces the proposal that the use of different path 27 integration strategies may determine the extent to which the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex 28 are engaged during path integration. To this end, recent studies that investigated the impact of 29 different path integration strategies on behavioral performance and functional brain activity were 30 discussed. Methodological concerns were raised with feasible recommendations for improving 31 the experimental design of future strategy-related path integration studies, which can cover 32 cognitive neuroscience research on age-related differences in the role of the hippocampal 33 formation in path integration and Bayesian modelling of the interaction between landmark and 34 self-motion cues. The practical value of investigating different path integration strategies was 35 also discussed briefly from a biomedical perspective. 36 37 Keywords: path integration, strategy, spatial navigation, hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, 38 cognitive neuroscience ii Report No. MAE0820 39 1. Introduction 40 41 In everyday contexts, whenever we have a destination in mind we want to travel to, we 42 rely on our innate adeptness for navigation, as typified by our cognitive ability to reason in 43 space, plan, and execute a path to our desired location (Gallistel, 1990). Navigation generally 44 occurs in the presence of external cues that present themselves ubiquitously in the form of 45 stationary landmarks in our everyday environments but can also proceed when only internal 46 bodily cues are available (i.e., during non-visual self-locomotion, see, e.g., Klatzky et al., 1990; 47 Loomis et al., 1993). The process of estimating distance and directional changes relative to a 48 starting position and integrating such displacements with self-motion cues to compute and update 49 a homing vector is called path integration (also known as dead reckoning) [see, e.g., Gallistel, 50 1990; Mittelstaedt & Mittelstaedt, 1980, 1982; Müller & Wehner, 1988; for reviews, see Etienne 51 & Jeffery, 2004; Loomis, Klatzky, Golledge, & Philbeck, 1999; Loomis, Klatzky, & Golledge, 52 2001; Srinivasan, 2015]. 53 Path integration was first postulated to apply to humans by Darwin (1873), who recounted 54 the remarkable feat in which the natives of North Siberia were able to chart direct courses of 55 return after meandering through icy plains without relying on any visible cues at sea or in the 56 sky, concluding that: 57 58 “We must bear in mind that neither a compass, nor the north star, nor any other such sign, suffices to 59 guide a man to a particular spot through an intricate country, or through hummocky ice, when many 60 deviations from a straight course are inevitable, unless the deviations are allowed for, or a sort of ‘dead 61 reckoning’ is kept.” 62 Darwin (1873, p. 418) 63 64 Subsequent empirical studies showed that this special “dead reckoning” ability is present 65 in a wide variety of animals, encompassing insects (e.g., honey bees, Saharan desert ants), 66 spiders, birds (e.g., pigeons, geese), and mammals (e.g., golden hamsters, gerbils, dogs, humans) 67 [Gallistel, 1990; Mittelstaedt & Mittelstaedt, 1980, 1982; see Etienne & Jeffery, 2004, 68 Srinivasan, 2015, for reviews of path integration in non-human animals; see Loomis et al., 1999, 69 2001, for reviews of path integration in humans]. In the animal kingdom, the perfect example of 70 the ”path integrator” is perhaps the Saharan desert ant, Cataglyphis fortis, which is capable of - 1 - Report No. MAE0820 71 returning in a straight-line path back to its nest after tracking a tortuous outbound path in a 72 featureless landscape to find food (see, e.g., Müller & Wehner, 1988; Wehner & Srinivasan, 73 2003; Wehner & Wehner, 1990) [Fig. 1]. Based on the ant’s foraging and homing behavior, path 74 integration was proposed to enable the navigating animal to compute its movement changes 75 continuously along each step of its outbound journey and integrate such changes with idiothetic 76 signals (derived from the vestibular and proprioceptive systems) to update a homing vector (i.e., 77 a representation of distance and directional estimates from a point of origin) (Müller & Wehner, 78 1988). 79 To date, research on path integration continues to draw the attention of many researchers 80 in spatial cognition due to the possibilities it offers for a better understanding of the basic 81 neurocognitive processes or mechanisms that are involved in spatial perception and cognitive 82 mapping (Burgess, 2014; Hafting, Fyhn, Molden, Moser, & Moser, 2005; Moser, Kropff, & 83 Moser, 2008). 84 Fig. 1 Path integration in Cataglyphis fortis. An ant’s tortuous outbound path (from N to F) and straight homeward path (from F to N, in bold) recorded in a featureless salt pan. [Source: Fig. 1.1 in Wehner & Srinivasan (2003). Reproduced with permission.] 85 86 1.1 Assessing Path Integration in Humans 87 88 The traditional behavioral paradigm used for studying path integration in humans is the 89 path- or triangle-completion task (see, e.g., Klatzky et al., 1990; Loomis et al., 1993; Fukusima, - 2 - Report No. MAE0820 90 Loomis, & Da Silva, 1997; Philbeck, Klatzky, Behrmann, Loomis, & Goodridge, 2001; Sholl, 91 1989). The path completion task, originally developed by Klatzky et al. (1990), requires a 92 participant, donning blindfolds and headphones, to walk two or more straight segments with one 93 or more turns in between under the guidance of the experimenter, and to walk back (or point 94 back) to the origin on his/her initiative at the end of the outbound journey. Triangle completion 95 (Fig. 2) is the popular derivative of this path completion paradigm and refers to participants’ 96 attempts at returning to the origin (or pointing toward it) after traversing two straight segments 97 with one turn (i.e., the whole trajectory forms a triangle) [Klatzky, Loomis, Beall, Chance, & 98 Golledge, 1998; Loomis et al., 1993; Philbeck et al., 2001; Sholl, 1989]. Importantly, the 99 donning of blindfolds and headphones throughout the task blocked out visual and auditory cues 100 that could facilitate the updating of positional estimates and obliged the participant to attend to 101 idiothetic cues from vestibular and proprioceptive systems (e.g., efferent motor commands, 102 kinesthetic feedback from the musculature, acceleratory signals from the vestibular system) for a 103 moment-to-moment updating of the perceived home or target location. 104 Fig. 2 In a trial of the conventional triangle completion task, the participant traverses an outbound path (shown by dark arrows) and then attempts to walk back to the starting position with visual and auditory cues occluded (the dotted line segment shows the ideal/perfect path of return). Apart from walking responses, pointing or heading responses toward the point of origin have also been applied. 105 106 In the first extensive study that applied the triangle completion task, Loomis et al. (1993) 107 showed that sighted participants who were blindfolded performed as well as their congenitally 108 and adventitiously blind counterparts when walking back to the starting position at the end of 109 outbound travel. No group showed particularly good performance and analyses of the distance
Recommended publications
  • Activity Strength Within Optic Flow-Sensitive Cortical Regions Is Associated with Visual Path Integration Accuracy in Aged Adults
    brain sciences Article Activity Strength within Optic Flow-Sensitive Cortical Regions Is Associated with Visual Path Integration Accuracy in Aged Adults Lauren Zajac 1,2,* and Ronald Killiany 1,2 1 Department of Anatomy & Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, 72 East Concord Street (L 1004), Boston, MA 02118, USA; [email protected] 2 Center for Biomedical Imaging, Boston University School of Medicine, 650 Albany Street, Boston, MA 02118, USA * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: Spatial navigation is a cognitive skill fundamental to successful interaction with our envi- ronment, and aging is associated with weaknesses in this skill. Identifying mechanisms underlying individual differences in navigation ability in aged adults is important to understanding these age- related weaknesses. One understudied factor involved in spatial navigation is self-motion perception. Important to self-motion perception is optic flow–the global pattern of visual motion experienced while moving through our environment. A set of optic flow-sensitive (OF-sensitive) cortical regions was defined in a group of young (n = 29) and aged (n = 22) adults. Brain activity was measured in this set of OF-sensitive regions and control regions using functional magnetic resonance imaging while participants performed visual path integration (VPI) and turn counting (TC) tasks. Aged adults had stronger activity in RMT+ during both tasks compared to young adults. Stronger activity in the OF-sensitive regions LMT+ and RpVIP during VPI, not TC, was associated with greater VPI Citation: Zajac, L.; Killiany, R. accuracy in aged adults. The activity strength in these two OF-sensitive regions measured during Activity Strength within Optic VPI explained 42% of the variance in VPI task performance in aged adults.
    [Show full text]
  • Correction for Partial-Volume Effects on Brain Perfusion SPECT in Healthy Men
    Correction for Partial-Volume Effects on Brain Perfusion SPECT in Healthy Men Hiroshi Matsuda, MD1; Takashi Ohnishi, MD1; Takashi Asada, MD2; Zhi-jie Li, MD1,3; Hidekazu Kanetaka, MD1; Etsuko Imabayashi, MD1; Fumiko Tanaka, MD1; and Seigo Nakano, MD4 1Department of Radiology, National Center Hospital for Mental, Nervous, and Muscular Disorders, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tokyo, Japan; 2Department of Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan; 3Department of Nuclear Medicine, The Second Clinical Hospital of China Medical University, Shen-Yang City, China; and 4Department of Geriatric Medicine, National Center Hospital for Mental, Nervous, and Muscular Disorders, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Tokyo, Japan The limited spatial resolution of SPECT scanners does not allow Functional changes in the brains of healthy elderly people an exact measurement of the local radiotracer concentration in and patients with neurodegenerative disorders have been brain tissue because partial-volume effects (PVEs) underesti- studied widely by SPECT. However, due to the limited mate concentration in small structures of the brain. The aim of this study was to determine which brain structures show greater spatial resolution of SPECT, the accurate measurement of influence of PVEs in SPECT studies on healthy volunteers and to tracer concentration in brain structures depends on several investigate aging effects on SPECT after the PVE correction. physical limitations—particularly, the relation between ob- Methods: Brain perfusion SPECT using 99mTc-ethylcysteinate ject size and scanner spatial resolution. This relation, known dimer was performed in 52 healthy men, 18–86 y old. The as the partial-volume effect (PVE), biases the measured regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) was noninvasively measured concentration in small structures by diminishing the true using graphical analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • Translingual Neural Stimulation with the Portable Neuromodulation
    Translingual Neural Stimulation With the Portable Neuromodulation Stimulator (PoNS®) Induces Structural Changes Leading to Functional Recovery In Patients With Mild-To-Moderate Traumatic Brain Injury Authors: Jiancheng Hou,1 Arman Kulkarni,2 Neelima Tellapragada,1 Veena Nair,1 Yuri Danilov,3 Kurt Kaczmarek,3 Beth Meyerand,2 Mitchell Tyler,2,3 *Vivek Prabhakaran1 1. Department of Radiology, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, USA 2. Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, USA 3. Department of Kinesiology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin, USA *Correspondence to [email protected] Disclosure: Dr Tyler, Dr Danilov, and Dr Kaczmarek are co-founders of Advanced Neurorehabilitation, LLC, which holds the intellectual property rights to the PoNS® technology. Dr Tyler is a board member of NeuroHabilitation Corporation, a wholly- owned subsidiary of Helius Medical Technologies, and owns stock in the corporation. The other authors have declared no conflicts of interest. Acknowledgements: Professional medical writing and editorial assistance were provided by Kelly M. Fahrbach, Ashfield Healthcare Communications, part of UDG Healthcare plc, funded by Helius Medical Technologies. Dr Tyler, Dr Kaczmarek, Dr Danilov, Dr Hou, and Dr Prabhakaran were being supported by NHC-TBI-PoNS-RT001. Dr Hou, Dr Kulkarni, Dr Nair, Dr Tellapragada, and Dr Prabhakaran were being supported by R01AI138647. Dr Hou and Dr Prabhakaran were being supported by P01AI132132, R01NS105646. Dr Kulkarni was being supported by the Clinical & Translational Science Award programme of the National Center for Research Resources, NCATS grant 1UL1RR025011. Dr Meyerand, Dr Prabhakaran, Dr Nair was being supported by U01NS093650.
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Strategies, Sensory Cues, and Brain Mechanisms
    Intro to Neuroscience: Behavioral Neuroscience Animal Navigation: Behavioral strategies, sensory cues, and brain mechanisms Nachum Ulanovsky Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science Outline of today’s lecture • Introduction: Feats of animal navigation • Navigational strategies: • Beaconing • Route following • Path integration • Map and Compass / Cognitive Map • Sensory cues for navigation: • Compass mechanisms • Map mechanisms • Brain mechanisms of Navigation (brief introduction) • Summary Outline of today’s lecture • Introduction: Feats of animal navigation • Navigational strategies: • Beaconing • Route following • Path integration • Map and Compass / Cognitive Map • Sensory cues for navigation: • Compass mechanisms • Map mechanisms • Brain mechanisms of Navigation (brief introduction) • Summary Shearwater migration across the pacific יסעור Population data from 19 birds 3 pairs of birds Recaptured at their breeding Shaffer et al. PNAS 103:12799-12802 (2006) grounds in New Zealand Some other famous examples • Wandering Albatross: finding a tiny island in the vast ocean • Salmon: returning to the river of birth after years in the ocean • Sea Turtles • Monarch Butterflies • Spiny Lobsters • … And many other examples (some of them we will see later) Mammals can also do it… Medium-scale navigation: Egyptian fruit bats navigating to an individual tree Tsoar, Nathan, Bartan, Vyssotski, Dell’Omo & Ulanovsky (PNAS, 2011) GPS movie: Bat 079 A typical example of a full night flight of an individual bat released @ cave Bat roost Foraging tree 5 Km Characteristics of the bats’ commuting flights: • Long-distance flights (often > 15 km one-way) • Very straight flights (straightness index > 0.9 for almost all bats) • Very fast (typically 30–40 km/hr, and up to 63 km/hr) • Very high (typically 100–200 meters, and up to 643 m) • Bats returned to the same individual tree night after night, for many nights tree cave Tsoar, Nathan, Bartan, With Vyssotski, Dell’Omo & Y.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Linear Navigation: Is Vision Necessary? Isabelle Israël, Aurore Capelli, Anne-Emmanuelle Priot, I
    Spatial linear navigation: Is vision necessary? Isabelle Israël, Aurore Capelli, Anne-Emmanuelle Priot, I. Giannopulu To cite this version: Isabelle Israël, Aurore Capelli, Anne-Emmanuelle Priot, I. Giannopulu. Spatial linear navigation: Is vision necessary?. Neuroscience Letters, Elsevier, 2013, 554, pp.34-38. 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.08.060. hal-02395669 HAL Id: hal-02395669 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02395669 Submitted on 5 Dec 2019 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Elsevier Editorial System(tm) for Neuroscience Letters Manuscript Draft Manuscript Number: Title: Spatial linear navigation : is vision necessary ? Article Type: Research Paper Keywords: Path integration; self-motion perception; multisensory integration Corresponding Author: Dr. Isabelle Israel, PhD Corresponding Author's Institution: CNRS First Author: Isabelle ISRAEL, PhD Order of Authors: Isabelle ISRAEL, PhD; Aurore CAPELLI, PhD; Anne-Emmanuelle PRIOT, MD, PhD; Irini GIANNOPULU, PhD, D.Sc. Abstract: In order to analyze spatial linear navigation through a task of self-controlled reproduction, healthy participants were passively transported on a mobile robot at constant velocity, and then had to reproduce the imposed distance of 2 to 8 m in two conditions: "with vision" and "without vision".
    [Show full text]
  • Dissection Technique for the Study of the Cerebral Sulci, Gyri and Ventricles
    Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2008;66(2-A):282-287 Dissection technique for the stuDy of the cerebral sulci, gyri anD ventricles João Paulo Mattos1, Marcos Juliano dos Santos2, João Flavio Daniel Zullo2, Andrei Fernandes Joaquim2, Feres Chaddad-Neto1, Evandro de Oliveira3 Abstract – Neuroanatomy in addition to neurophysiology, are the basic areas for the proper formation from health students to specialized professionals in neuroscience. A step by step guide for practical studies of neuroanatomy is required for this kind of knowledge to become more acceptable among medical students, neurosurgeons, neurologists, neuropediatricians and psychiatric physicians. Based on the well known courses of sulci, gyri and ventricles offered by Beneficência Portuguesa Hospital in São Paulo, Brazil, two times a year, since 1994, totalizing more than 20 complete courses, and answering the request of many neuroscience students and professionals whose asked for a practical guide to the neuroanatomy study, the authors suggest a protocol for the study of superficial and deep brain structures showing how to approach the more structures as possible with minimum damage to the anatomic piece and with the smaller number of brains. Key wordS: neuroanatomy, brain, dissection technique. técnica de dissecação para o estudo dos sulcos, giros e ventriculos cerebrais Resumo – Neuroanatomia e a neurofisiologia são as áreas básicas para a adequada formação desde estudantes na área da saúde a profissionais especializados em neurociências. Um guia prático, passo a passo, para o estudo
    [Show full text]
  • Mesial Frontal Epilepsy
    Epikpsia, 39(Suppl. 4):S49-S61. 1998 Lippincon-Raven Publishers, Philadelphia 0 International League Against Epilepsy Mesial Frontal Epilepsy Norman K. So Oregon Comprehensive Epilepsy Program, Legacy Portland Hospitals, Portland, Oregon, U.S.A. Summary: The mesiofrontal cortex comprises a number of occur. The task of localization of the epileptogenic zone can be distinct anatomic and functional areas. Structural lesions and challenging, whether EEG or imaging methods are used. Suc- cortical dysgenesis are recognized causes of mesial frontal epi- cessful localization can lead to a rewarding outcome after epi- lepsy, but a specific gene defect may also be important, as seen lepsy surgery, particularly in those with an imaged lesion. in some forms of familial frontal lobe epilepsy. The predomi- Key Words: Mesial frontal epilepsy-cingulate gyrus- nant seizure manifestations, which are not necessarily strictly Supplementary motor area-Absence seizure-Hypermotor correlated with a specific ictal onset zone, are absence, hyper- seizure-Postural tonic seizure-Epilepsy surgery. motor, and postural tonic seizures. Other seizure types also FUNCTIONAL ANATOMY convolution. Traditional cytoarchitectonics have further subdivided this anterior frontal region. The frontal pole The frontal lobe is the largest lobe in the brain, ac- refers to the anterior most portion of the frontal lobe, but counting for one-third to one-half of total brain volume there is little consensus on how far back this extends. and weight. On the medial surface, the most important One or more curved.sulci are seen anterior and inferior to landmark is the cingulate sulcus (Fig. 1). This runs as an the cingulate sulcus, called the superior and inferior ros- inverted “C” following the contour of the corpus callo- tral sulci.
    [Show full text]
  • Path Integration Deficits During Linear Locomotion After Human Medial Temporal Lobectomy
    Path Integration Deficits during Linear Locomotion after Human Medial Temporal Lobectomy John W.Philbeck 1,Marlene Behrmann 2,Lucien Levy 3, Samuel J.Potolicchio 3,andAnthony J.Caputy 3 Abstract & Animalnavigation studies have implicated structures inand both adecrease inthe consistency ofpath integration and a around the hippocampal formation as crucialin performing systematic underregistration oflinear displacement (and/or path integration (amethod ofdetermining one’s position by velocity) during walking.Moreover, the deficits were observable monitoring internally generated self-motion signals). Less is even when there were virtually no angular acceleration known about the role ofthese structures forhuman path vestibular signals. Theresults suggest that structures inthe integration. We tested path integration inpatients whohad medial temporal lobe participate inhuman path integration undergone left orright medial temporal lobectomy as therapy when individuals walkalong linearpaths and that thisis so to forepilepsy. Thisprocedure removed approximately 50% ofthe agreater extent inright hemisphere structures than left. anterior portion ofthe hippocampus, as wellas the amygdala Thisinformation is relevant forfuture research investigating and lateral temporal lobe. Participants attempted to walk the neural substrates ofnavigation, not only inhumans without vision to apreviously viewed target 2–6 mdistant. (e.g.,functional neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies), Patients withright, but not left,hemisphere lesions exhibited but also
    [Show full text]
  • Path Integration in Place Cells of Developing Rats PNAS PLUS
    Path integration in place cells of developing rats PNAS PLUS Tale L. Bjerknesa,b, Nenitha C. Dagslotta,b, Edvard I. Mosera,b, and May-Britt Mosera,b,1 aKavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, NO-7489 Trondheim, Norway; and bCentre for Neural Computation, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, NO-7489 Trondheim, Norway Contributed by May-Britt Moser, January 2, 2018 (sent for review November 1, 2017; reviewed by John L. Kubie and Mayank R. Mehta) Place cells in the hippocampus and grid cells in the medial entorhinal Firing locations of grid cells and place cells are not determined cortex rely on self-motion information and path integration for exclusively by path integration, however. Position information may spatially confined firing. Place cells can be observed in young rats be obtained also from distal landmarks, as suggested by the fact that as soon as they leave their nest at around 2.5 wk of postnatal life. In place fields (17) as well as grid fields (18, 19) follow the location of contrast, the regularly spaced firing of grid cells develops only after the walls of the recording environment when the environment is weaning, during the fourth week. In the present study, we sought to stretched or compressed. This observation points to local bound- determine whether place cells areabletointegrateself-motion aries as a strong determinant of firing location. On the other hand, information before maturation of the grid-cell system. Place cells other work has demonstrated that place cells fire in a predictable were recorded on a 200-cm linear track while preweaning, post- relationship to the animal’s start location on a linear track even weaning, and adult rats ran on successive trials from a start wall to a when the position of the starting point is shifted (20, 21).
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Influence on Path Integration in Darkness Indicates a Multimodal Representation of Large-Scale Space
    Visual influence on path integration in darkness indicates a multimodal representation of large-scale space Lili Tcheanga,b,1, Heinrich H. Bülthoffb,d,1, and Neil Burgessa,c,1 aUCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom; bMax Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tuebingen 72076, Germany; cUCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom; and dDepartment of Brain and Cognitive Engineering, Korea University, Seoul 136-713, Korea Edited by Charles R. Gallistel, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, and approved November 11, 2010 (received for review August 10, 2010) Our ability to return to the start of a route recently performed in representations are sufficiently abstract to support other actions darkness is thought to reflect path integration of motion-related in- aimed at the target location, such as throwing (19). formation. Here we provide evidence that motion-related interocep- To specifically examine the relationship between visual and in- tive representations (proprioceptive, vestibular, and motor efference teroceptive inputs to navigation, we investigated the effect of ma- copy) combine with visual representations to form a single multi- nipulating visual information. In the first experiment, we investigated modal representation guiding navigation. We used immersive virtual whether visual information present during the outbound trajectory reality to decouple visual input from motion-related interoception by makes an important contribution
    [Show full text]
  • Seed MNI Coordinates Lobe
    MNI Coordinates Seed Lobe (Hemisphere) Region BAa X Y Z FP1 -18 62 0 Frontal Lobe (L) Medial Frontal Gyrus 10 FPz 4 62 0 Frontal Lobe (R) Medial Frontal Gyrus 10 FP2 24 60 0 Frontal Lobe (R) Superior Frontal Gyrus 10 AF7 -38 50 0 Frontal Lobe (L) Middle Frontal Gyrus 10 AF3 -30 50 24 Frontal Lobe (L) Superior Frontal Gyrus 9 AFz 4 58 30 Frontal Lobe (R) Medial Frontal Gyrus 9 AF4 36 48 20 Frontal Lobe (R) Middle Frontal Gyrus 10 AF8 42 46 -4 Frontal Lobe (R) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 10 F7 -48 26 -4 Frontal Lobe (L) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47 F5 -48 28 18 Frontal Lobe (L) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 45 F3 -38 28 38 Frontal Lobe (L) Precentral Gyrus 9 F1 -20 30 50 Frontal Lobe (L) Superior Frontal Gyrus 8 Fz 2 32 54 Frontal Lobe (L) Superior Frontal Gyrus 8 F2 26 32 48 Frontal Lobe (R) Superior Frontal Gyrus 8 F4 42 30 34 Frontal Lobe (R) Precentral Gyrus 9 F6 50 28 14 Frontal Lobe (R) Middle Frontal Gyrus 46 F8 48 24 -8 Frontal Lobe (R) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47 FT9 -50 -6 -36 Temporal Lobe (L) Inferior Temporal Gyrus 20 FT7 -54 2 -8 Temporal Lobe (L) Superior Temporal Gyrus 22 FC5 -56 4 22 Frontal Lobe (L) Precentral Gyrus 6 FC3 -44 6 48 Frontal Lobe (L) Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 FC1 -22 6 64 Frontal Lobe (L) Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 FCz 4 6 66 Frontal Lobe (R) Medial Frontal Gyrus 6 FC2 28 8 60 Frontal Lobe (R) Sub-Gyral 6 FC4 48 8 42 Frontal Lobe (R) Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 FC6 58 6 16 Frontal Lobe (R) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 44 FT8 54 2 -12 Temporal Lobe (R) Superior Temporal Gyrus 38 FT10 50 -6 -38 Temporal Lobe (R) Inferior Temporal Gyrus 20 T7/T3
    [Show full text]
  • Possible Uses of Path Integration in Animal Navigation
    Animal Learning & Behavior 2000,28 (3),257-277 Possible uses ofpath integration in animal navigation ROBERT BIEGLER University ofEdinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland Path integration, in its simplest form, keeps track of movement from a starting point and so makes it possible to return to this point Path integration can also be used to build a metric spatial represen­ tation of the environment, if given a suitable readout mechanism that can store and recall the coordi­ nates of anyone of multiple locations, A simple averagingprocess can make this representation as ac­ curate as desired, givenenough visits to the locations stored in the representation, There are more than these two ways of using path integration in navigation. They can be classified systematically accord­ ing to the following three criteria: Is there one point at which coordinates can be reset to correct er­ rors, or several? Is there one possible goal, or several? Is there one path integrator, or several? I de­ scribe the resulting eight methods of using path integration and compare their characteristics with the availableexperimental evidence, The classification offers a theoretical framework for further research, Methods of navigation can be broadly divided into integration (arthropods: Beugnon & Campan, 1989; those that use location-based information, which specifies Hoffmann, 1984; H. Mittelstaedt, 1985; Muller & Wehner, (with varying precision) where an animal is by reference to 1988; Ronacher & Wehner, 1995; Schmidt, Collett, Dil­ specific and identifiable features ofthe environment, and lier, & Wehner, 1992; von Frisch, 1967; Wehner & Raber, those that use movement-based or location-independent 1979; Wehner & Wehner, 1990; Zeil, 1998; mammals: information.
    [Show full text]