Adolescents' Cognitive and Emotional Responses to Marital Hostility

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Adolescents' Cognitive and Emotional Responses to Marital Hostility Child Development, May/June 2007, Volume 78, Number 3, Pages 775 – 789 Adolescents’ Cognitive and Emotional Responses to Marital Hostility Cheryl Buehler and Garrett Lange Karen L. Franck University of North Carolina University of Tennessee Early adolescents’ (11 – 14 years) responses to marital hostility were examined in a sample of 416 families. The cognitive-contextual perspective and emotional security hypothesis guided the study and 9 adolescent re- sponses were identified. Prospective associations were examined in several structural equation models that included adolescent problems as outcomes. Self-blame and perceived threat uniquely mediated the association between Year 1 marital hostility and Year 3 adolescent externalizing problems (po.05). Self-blame, lower con- structive representations, internalization of feelings, avoidance, and emotional dysregulation uniquely medi- ated the association between Year 1 marital hostility and Year 3 internalizing problems. Specific cognitive and emotionally based responses are important to understanding how martial hostility affects youth and need to be considered within an integrated model. Marital hostility places children and adolescents marital hostility. The study examines early adoles- at risk for concurrent and prospective psychosocial cents’ responses to marital hostility in a sample of difficulties (Buehler et al., 1997; Davies, Harold, 416 families using a three-wave, prospective, multi- Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002; Doyle & Mark- method research design. iewicz, 2005; Grych & Fincham, 1990). In addition to Early adolescence is an important juncture for this disrupting parenting (Buehler, Benson, & Gerard, examination because youth are transforming their 2006) and children’s psychophysiological function- relationships with parents and peers (Steinberg, ing (Katz, 2001), recent advances in understanding 2001). Marital hostility that occurs during this the explanatory mechanisms of this risk factor have developmental transition might create additional focused on two complementary explanations: (a) the demands on the youth because they often are role of children’s potentially problematic cognitive compelled to devote psychological resources to appraisals (Grych & Fincham, 1993) and (b) the role processing parents’ disputes. This diversion of psy- of children’s emotional insecurity (Davies et al., chological resources creates potential vulnerabilities 2002). because youth in early adolescence are also experi- The present study addressed these two explana- encing changes in physical development, schools, tions by examining the conjoint and integrative roles and social networks. This is a demanding period of of cognitive appraisals and emotional insecurity development and the concurrent experience of during early adolescence. A conjoint explanation marital hostility inhibits youth from finding refuge suggests that when considered in the same model, within the family domain. Call and Mortimer (2001) problematic cognitive appraisals and emotional in- suggest that the family domain is a potentially im- security regarding marital hostility each have unique portant ‘‘arena of comfort’’ for adolescents under- associations with adolescent problem behavior. An going a multitude of change and marital hostility integrative explanation suggests that specific aspects might endanger some of the salutary benefits of of the cognitive and emotional response systems family life. Early adolescence is also an important overlap, and that once this overlap is considered, time to examine youths’ perceptions regarding cognitively and emotionally oriented responses are marital hostility because they are exploring their both needed to understand youths’ processing of own identity as a partner in close friendships and romantic pairings (Parke et al., 2001). Youths’ per- ceptions regarding parents’ interactions might shape This research was supported by a grant from The National In- their relational templates that serve as interpretive stitute of Mental Health, R01-MH59248. We thank the staff of the schemas for developing and maintaining close rela- Family Life Project for their unending contributions to this work tionships during adolescence and early adulthood and the youth, parents, teachers, and school administrators who (Linder & Collins, 2005). made this research possible. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Cheryl Buehler, Human Development and Family Studies, UNCG, Greensboro, NC 27402-6170. Electronic mail may be sent to r 2007 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. [email protected]. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2007/7803-0008 776 Buehler, Lange, and Franck Theoretical Foundation framework, the emotional security hypothesis sug- gests that youths’ responses involve both emotion Cognitive Appraisals and cognition. Davies and Cummings (1994) theorize The potential role of cognitive appraisals in that ‘‘when conflicts are appraised as destructive and shaping children’s responses to witnessing marital reflective of significant marital disharmony, chil- hostility is detailed theoretically in the cognitive- dren’s negative emotional arousal becomes elevated contextual framework (Grych & Fincham, 1990). In and they are motivated to act to decrease their their original formulations, Grych and Fincham use feelings of emotional insecurity’’ (p. 389). work by Campos, Campos, and Barrett (1989) to In their elaboration of the emotional security hy- define marital hostility as a ‘‘significant event’’ for pothesis, Davies and Cummings (1998) outline how children and youth. Defining marital hostility as a this process mechanism is consistent with a func- significant event suggests that hostility between tionalist perspective of emotion that considers feel- parents is relevant to youth, involves emotional ings, response motivations, and appraisals (Campos, communication among significant others, and is Mumme, Kermoian, & Campos, 1994). They propose arousing (Campos et al., 1989). Grych and Fincham three process components: emotional reactivity, propose that youths’ evaluation of the significance behavioral regulation, and internal representations of interparental hostility involves both emotion and (Davies & Cummings, 1994, 1998). Emotional reac- cognition. Various emotional responses shape the tivity focuses on the feeling manifestation of insecu- appraisals of threat and prompt coping behavior. rityFspecifically fear and distress. Regulation Appraisals also shape affect and coping behaviors. focuses on the behavioral manifestation of insecurity We suggest that coping, in this case, often deals with by including avoidant and involvement coping be- emotional arousal, trying to reduce the marital hos- haviors. Internal representations focus on the ap- tility itself, and/or minimizing exposure to the hos- praisal manifestation of insecurity by including tility (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). youths’ concerns of parental separation and the Cognitive responses involve appraisals of the spillover of hostility into parent – child relations. hostile interactions (e.g., how bad is it, will parents Theoretically, these three components are interde- contain it, whose fault is it) and estimations of one’s pendent, but distinct, indicators of youths’ need for ability to handle a potentially stressful situation. The emotional security within the context of interparen- cognitive-contextual framework suggests that three tal hostility. types of cognitions are particularly salient: perceived threat, self-blame, and coping efficacy. Conceptually, A Comparative Framework perceived threat focuses on specific fears and wor- ries regarding parents getting hurt, parents separat- Clearly, these two perspectives regarding how ing, and the spillover of hostility into parent – child marital hostility affects youth are complementary. relations. Self-blame focuses on attributions regard- Both view emotion and cognition as being interde- ing the youths’ cause of or responsibility for the pendent, integral elements of youths’ responses to conflict and parental distress. Coping efficacy fo- marital hostility. For example, Grych and Fincham cuses on the youths’ appraisals of their ability to stop (1990) acknowledge the role of emotion-focused re- parents’ hostile interactions or manage their own sponses by theorizing that ‘‘affect is involved in distress. Theoretically, these three appraisals are in- evaluating the significance of the conflict and in terrelated and Grych and Fincham (1990) speculate guiding subsequent behavior’’ (p. 241). Davies et al. that they shape coping responses, particularly (2002) detail some of the interdependencies when whether to get involved in the dispute. stating ‘‘the emotional security hypothesis posits that preserving a sense of security is an important goal that organizes a child’s emotional experiences (e.g., Emotional Security fear), action tendencies (e.g., withdraw, intervene), The emotional security hypothesis suggests that and appraisals of self and interpersonal relationships youths’ responses to marital hostility focus on the (e.g., perceptions of threat to the self)’’ (p. 6). Both implications of the acrimony for their own emotional begin their theorizing regarding the effects of the security (Davies & Cummings, 1994). This hypothe- negative aspects of marital conflict (e.g., hostile, sis proposes that the attainment of emotional secu- frequent, unresolved) when youth are distressed, rity is shaped, in part, by parents’ interactions with aware, and upset.
Recommended publications
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • Good Character Is What We Look for in a Friend: Character Strengths Are Positively Related to Peer Acceptance and Friendship Quality in Early Adolescents
    Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2019 Good Character Is What We Look for in a Friend: Character Strengths Are Positively Related to Peer Acceptance and Friendship Quality in Early Adolescents Wagner, Lisa Abstract: This study investigates the role of character strengths in peer relationships among early ado- lescents. A sample of students (N = 339; X฀฀฀ age = 12.84 years, 53.1% female) nominated friends in the classroom and completed assessments of character strengths, the desirability and importance of character strengths in a friend, and friendship quality. Results indicate that the character strengths of honesty, humor, kindness, and fairness were most desirable and important in a friend. Perspective, love, kindness, social intelligence, teamwork, leadership, and humor were associated with higher peer acceptance. Dyadic analyses of mutual best friends suggested that a number of character strengths were also positively related to friend-rated friendship quality. Overall, the results demonstrate the relevance of character strengths for positive peer relationships in adolescents. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431618791286 Posted at the Zurich Open Repository and Archive, University of Zurich ZORA URL: https://doi.org/10.5167/uzh-153534 Journal Article Accepted Version Originally published at: Wagner, Lisa (2019). Good Character Is What We Look for in a Friend: Character Strengths Are Positively Related to Peer Acceptance and Friendship Quality in Early Adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 39(6):864-903. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431618791286 RUNNING HEAD: CHARACTER STRENGTHS IN FRIENDSHIPS Good character is what we look for in a friend: Character strengths are positively related to peer acceptance and friendship quality in early adolescents Author Notes Lisa Wagner, Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Switzerland.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 December 19, 2019 the Psychology of Online Political Hostility
    The Psychology of Online Political Hostility: A Comprehensive, Cross-National Test of the Mismatch Hypothesis Alexander Bor* & Michael Bang Petersen Department of Political Science Aarhus University August 30, 2021 Please cite the final version of this paper published in the American Political Science Review at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055421000885. Abstract Why are online discussions about politics more hostile than offline discussions? A popular answer argues that human psychology is tailored for face-to-face interaction and people’s behavior therefore changes for the worse in impersonal online discussions. We provide a theoretical formalization and empirical test of this explanation: the mismatch hypothesis. We argue that mismatches between human psychology and novel features of online environments could (a) change people’s behavior, (b) create adverse selection effects and (c) bias people’s perceptions. Across eight studies, leveraging cross-national surveys and behavioral experiments (total N=8,434), we test the mismatch hypothesis but only find evidence for limited selection effects. Instead, hostile political discussions are the result of status-driven individuals who are drawn to politics and are equally hostile both online and offline. Finally, we offer initial evidence that online discussions feel more hostile, in part, because the behavior of such individuals is more visible than offline. Acknowledgements This research has benefitted from discussions with Vin Arceneaux, Matt Levendusky, Mark Van Vugt, John Tooby, and members of the Research on Online Political Hostility (ROHP) group, among many others. We are grateful for constructive comments to workshop attendees at the Political Behavior Section of Aarhus University, at the NYU-SMAPP Lab, at the NYU Social Justice Lab, at the Hertie School, and to conference audiences at APSA 2019, HBES 2019, and ROPH 2020.
    [Show full text]
  • Dysphoria As a Complex Emotional State and Its Role in Psychopathology
    Dysphoria as a complex emotional state and its role in psychopathology Vladan Starcevic A/Professor, University of Sydney Faculty of Medicine and Health Sydney, Australia Objectives • Review conceptualisations of dysphoria • Present dysphoria as a transdiagnostic complex emotional state and assessment of dysphoria based on this conceptualisation What is dysphoria? • The term is derived from Greek (δύσφορος) and denotes distress that is hard to bear Dysphoria: associated with externalisation? • “Mixed affect” leading to an “affect of suspicion”1,2 1 Sandberg: Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie und Psychisch-Gerichtl Medizin 1896; 52:619-654 2 Specht G: Über den pathologischen Affekt in der chronischen Paranoia. Festschrift der Erlanger Universität, 1901 • A syndrome that always includes irritability and at least two of the following: internal tension, suspiciousness, hostility and aggressive or destructive behaviour3 3 Dayer et al: Bipolar Disord 2000; 2: 316-324 Dysphoria: associated with internalisation? • Six “dysphoric symptoms”: depressed mood, anhedonia, guilt, suicide, fatigue and anxiety1 1 Cassidy et al: Psychol Med 2000; 30:403-411 Dysphoria: a nonspecific state? • Dysphoria is a “nonspecific syndrome” and has “no particular place in a categorical diagnostic system”1; it is neglected and treated like an “orphan”1 1 Musalek et al: Psychopathol 2000; 33:209-214 • Dysphoria “can refer to many ways of feeling bad”2 2 Swann: Bipolar Disord 2000; 2:325-327 Textbook definitions: dysphoria nonspecific, mainly internalising? • “Feeling
    [Show full text]
  • A Study of Security-Insecurity Feelings Among Adolescents in Relation to Sex, Family System and Ordinal Position
    International Journal of Educational Planning & Administration. ISSN 2249-3093 Volume 3, Number 1 (2013), pp. 51-60 © Research India Publications http://www.ripublication.com/ijepa.htm A Study of Security-Insecurity Feelings among Adolescents in Relation to Sex, Family System and Ordinal Position. *Shruti Raina and **Dr. Kiran Sumbali Bhan *Research Scholar, Post-Graduate Department of Education, University of Jammu, PO Box180004, Jammu & Kashmir, India **Prof. Post-Graduate Department of Education, University of Jammu, PO Box 180004, Jammu & Kashmir, India *[email protected] Abstract The aim of this study was to investigate the significant differences in the Security -Insecurity scores of adolescent boys and girls belonging to nuclear and joint families and different ordinal positions. 240 students (120 boys and 120 girls) were selected randomly from 11 private schools of Jammu city. The technique of sampling employed was stratified random sampling. The sample was stratified into two strata on the basis of sex; two strata on the basis of family systems and further into three strata on the characteristic of different ordinal positions. The instrument employed for the collection of data was Maslow’s Security- Insecurity Inventory in Indian situations (Hindi Version). Three way ANOVA (2× 2 × 3) factorial design was used to see the effect of the main factors: sex, family systems and ordinal position and also their interactional effect on the criterion score.t-ratio technique was also applied to the main factor Ordinal Position with three levels in order to find out the significant differences in the mean security insecurity scores of adolescents belonging to different ordinal positions.
    [Show full text]
  • Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Departmental Papers (ASC) Annenberg School for Communication 11-2017 Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm Emile Bruneau University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Mina Cikara Rebecca Saxe Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/asc_papers Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons, Cognitive Psychology Commons, Communication Commons, Community Psychology Commons, Experimental Analysis of Behavior Commons, Personality and Social Contexts Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Bruneau, E., Cikara, M., & Saxe, R. (2017). Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8 (8), 934-942. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617693064 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/asc_papers/554 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm Abstract Empathic failures are common in hostile intergroup contexts; repairing empathy is therefore a major focus of peacebuilding efforts. However, it is unclear which aspect of empathy is most relevant to intergroup conflict. Although trait empathic concern predicts prosociality in interpersonal settings, we hypothesized that the best predictor of meaningful intergroup attitudes and behaviors might not be the general capacity for empathy (i.e., trait empathy), but the difference in empathy felt for the in-group versus the out-group, or “parochial empathy.” Specifically, we predicted that out-group empathy would inhibit intergroup harm and promote intergroup helping, whereas in-group empathy would have the opposite effect. In three intergroup contexts—Americans regarding Arabs, Hungarians regarding refugees, Greeks regarding Germans—we found support for this hypothesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Interparental Conflict, Toddler Emotional Security, Parental
    INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT, TODDLER EMOTIONAL SECURITY, PARENTAL EMOTION SOCIALIZATION, AND TODDLER SOCIO-EMOTIONAL OUTCOMES: TESTING DYADIC DYNAMICS USING ACTOR-PARTNER INTERDEPENDENCE MODELS By Young-Eun Lee A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Human Development and Family Studies - Doctor of Philosophy 2016 ABSTRACT INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT, TODDLER EMOTIONAL SECURITY, PARENTAL EMOTION SOCIALIZATION, AND TODDLER SOCIO-EMOTIONAL OUTCOMES: TESTING DYADIC DYNAMICS USING ACTOR-PARTNER INTERDEPENDENCE MODELS By Young-Eun Lee While much literature has focused on interparental conflict, parenting, and children’s socio- emotional outcomes, less research has utilized a dyadic perspective emphasizing the interdependent nature of mother-father relationship with regard to the impacts of interparental conflict on parenting and children’s outcomes. This dissertation provides evidence from two related studies revealing the ways in which mothers’ and fathers’ experiences with interparental conflict affect their own and their partners’ emotion socialization parenting behaviors, toddlers’ emotional security in response to conflict and toddlers’ behavioral outcomes. Utilizing a family-systems approach highlighting the interdependence among family subsystems including the parental subsystem, the goal of the Study 1 was to examine the relations between mothers’ and fathers’ interparental conflict and their emotion socialization behaviors. Dyadic analyses were conducted using multilevel
    [Show full text]
  • The Parenting Relationship
    THE PARENTING RELATIONSHIP First, we reviewed what our kids need us to do for them, as their parents Trust and respect them Provide them with emotional security Provide them with physical security, including their physical health To date we have Give them our time looked at what Give them our encouragement and we do as support parents Offer them physical and verbal affection Provide discipline to instruct rather than punish Practice the power of saying No to our kids Be consistently consistent Take care of ourselves, too A parenting style is the emotional climate, or climates, Then we looked at our in which we raise our kids parenting styles, our Parenting practices are the parenting practices, and things we specifically do, the what the impact of the actions we employ as parents different styles may be on There are four accepted and our kids evidenced parenting styles: Authoritarian Permissive Authoritative Uninvolved Each of these Demandingness – the extent we parenting styles is want to control our kids behavior categorized and demand obedience according to the + weight given to two Responsiveness – the extent to fundamental which we are accepting and dimensions of sensitive to our kids unique parenting behavior emotional and developmental needs So, we have talked a lot The critical factor is the about what relationship we have with we do as our kids parents. Or, more precisely, the But the most relationship our kids are critical factor willing to have with us in parenting is not skill… “It is not the parent/child relationship that has the greatest impact on the child.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychosexual Characteristics of Vestibulodynia Couples: Partner Solicitousness and Hostility Are Associated with Pain
    418 ORIGINAL RESEARCH—PAIN Psychosexual Characteristics of Vestibulodynia Couples: Partner Solicitousness and Hostility are Associated with Pain Mylène Desrosiers, MA,* Sophie Bergeron, PhD,* Marta Meana, PhD,† Bianca Leclerc, B.Sc.,* Yitzchak M. Binik, PhD,‡ and Samir Khalifé, MD§ *Department of Sexology, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; †Department of Psychology, University of Nevada Las Vegas, Las Vegas, NV, USA; ‡Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; §Jewish General Hospital—Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Montreal, Quebec, Canada DOI: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2007.00705.x ABSTRACT Introduction. Provoked vestibulodynia is a prevalent yet misunderstood women’s sexual health issue. In particular, data concerning relationship characteristics and psychosexual functioning of partners of these women are scarce. Moreover, no research to date has examined the role of the partner in vestibulodynia. Aims. This study aimed to characterize and compare the psychosexual profiles of women with vestibulodynia and their partners, in addition to exploring whether partner-related variables correlated with women’s pain and associ- ated psychosexual functioning. Methods. Forty-three couples in which the woman suffered from vestibulodynia completed self-report question- naires focusing on their sexual functioning, dyadic adjustment, and psychological adjustment. Women were diagnosed using the cotton-swab test during a standardized gynecological examination. They also took part in a structured interview during which they were asked about their pain during intercourse and frequency of intercourse. They also completed a questionnaire about their perceptions of their partners’ responses to the pain. Main Outcome Measures. Dependent measures for both members of the couple included the Sexual History Form, the Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Scale and the Brief Symptom Inventory.
    [Show full text]
  • Generation Z's Positive and Negative Attributes and the Impact On
    UNF Digital Commons UNF Graduate Theses and Dissertations Student Scholarship 2019 Generation Z's Positive and Negative Attributes and the Impact on Empathy After a Community- Based Learning Experience Amanda Nicole Moscrip University of North Florida Suggested Citation Moscrip, Amanda Nicole, "Generation Z's Positive and Negative Attributes and the Impact on Empathy After a Community-Based Learning Experience" (2019). UNF Graduate Theses and Dissertations. 908. https://digitalcommons.unf.edu/etd/908 This Master's Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Scholarship at UNF Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in UNF Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of UNF Digital Commons. For more information, please contact Digital Projects. © 2019 All Rights Reserved GENERATION Z’S POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE ATTRIBUTES AND THE IMPACT ON EMPATHY AFTER A COMMUNITY-BASED LEARNING EXPERIENCE by Amanda Nicole Moscrip A thesis submitted to the Department of Psychology in partial fulfillment to the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Psychological Science UNIVERSITY OF NORTH FLORIDA COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES August, 2019 Unpublished work © Amanda Nicole Moscrip GEN Z’S ATTRIBUTES AND THE IMPACT ON EMPATHY AFTER A CBL EXPERIENCE ii DEDICATION This thesis is dedicated to my family and friends for all of the support throughout this journey. I would like to extend a special thank you to my mother Teresa Moscrip, my father Michael Moscrip, and my brothers Tyler and Matthew Moscrip for all of the unconditional love and encouragement. I would like to thank my friends Evan Wagoner, Michelle Boss, and Anna Lall for always supporting me.
    [Show full text]
  • Assessment and Treatment of Posttraumatic Anger and Aggression: a Review
    Volume 49, Number 5, 2012 JRRDJRRD Pages 777–788 Assessment and treatment of posttraumatic anger and aggression: A review Casey T. Taft, PhD;1–2* Suzannah K. Creech, PhD;1,3 Lorig Kachadourian, PhD1–2 1National Center for PTSD, Department of Veterans Affairs (VA) Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA; 2Boston Uni- versity School of Medicine, Boston, MA; 3Providence VA Medical Center, Providence, RI; and Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, RI Abstract—The Department of Veterans Affairs (VA) and hyperarousal and are increasingly being viewed as a Department of Defense’s (DOD) recently published and updated defining feature of patients’ clinical presentations. Department of Veterans Affairs/Department of Defense VA/ DOD Clinical Practice Guideline for Management of Post- Traumatic Stress includes irritability, severe agitation, and METHODS anger as specific symptoms that frequently co-occur with PTSD. For the first time, the guideline includes nine specific We begin by reviewing literature on the associations recommendations for the assessment and treatment of PTSD- related anger, irritability, and agitation. This article will review between PTSD and both anger and aggression. The the literature on PTSD and its association with anger and review that occurred for the update in the 2010 VA/DOD aggression. We highlight explanatory models for these associa- PTSD clinical practice guideline and additional relevant tions, factors that contribute to the occurrence of anger and literature published since that guideline review is aggression in PTSD, assessment of anger and aggression, and included. We then discuss theoretical models that have effective anger management interventions and strategies. been proposed to explain these associations.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:2011.03588V1 [Cs.CL] 6 Nov 2020
    Hostility Detection Dataset in Hindi Mohit Bhardwajy, Md Shad Akhtary, Asif Ekbalz, Amitava Das?, Tanmoy Chakrabortyy yIIIT Delhi, India. zIIT Patna, India. ?Wipro Research, India. fmohit19014,tanmoy,[email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Abstract Despite Hindi being the third most spoken language in the world, and a significant presence of Hindi content on social In this paper, we present a novel hostility detection dataset in Hindi language. We collect and manually annotate ∼ 8200 media platforms, to our surprise, we were not able to find online posts. The annotated dataset covers four hostility di- any significant dataset on fake news or hate speech detec- mensions: fake news, hate speech, offensive, and defamation tion in Hindi. A survey of the literature suggest a few works posts, along with a non-hostile label. The hostile posts are related to hostile post detection in Hindi, such as (Kar et al. also considered for multi-label tags due to a significant over- 2020; Jha et al. 2020; Safi Samghabadi et al. 2020); however, lap among the hostile classes. We release this dataset as part there are two basic issues with these works - either the num- of the CONSTRAINT-2021 shared task on hostile post detec- ber of samples in the dataset are not adequate or they cater tion. to a specific dimension of the hostility only. In this paper, we present our manually annotated dataset for hostile posts 1 Introduction detection in Hindi. We collect more than ∼ 8200 online so- The COVID-19 pandemic has changed our lives forever, cial media posts and annotate them as hostile and non-hostile both online and offline.
    [Show full text]