Long-Term Repeatability and Reproducibility of Phosphene Characteristics in Chronically Implanted Argus II Retinal Prosthesis Subjects

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Long-Term Repeatability and Reproducibility of Phosphene Characteristics in Chronically Implanted Argus II Retinal Prosthesis Subjects Long-term Repeatability and Reproducibility of Phosphene Characteristics in Chronically Implanted Argus II Retinal Prosthesis Subjects YVONNE H-L. LUO, JOE JIANGJIAN ZHONG, MONICA CLEMO, AND LYNDON DA CRUZ PURPOSE: Previously published literatures of acute (Second Sight Medical Products Inc., Sylmar, CA, USA) studies on few subjects have shown contradictory evi- retinal prosthesis system has been implanted in an dence on the reproducibility and characteristics of the increasing number of patients.1 However, despite this elicited phosphenes, despite using the same stimulating growing clinical use, data describing the features of artifi- parameters with epiretinal electrode arrays. In this study, cial vision perceived by the users remain scarce in the we set out to investigate the long-term repeatilibity and published literature.2–5 The idea of developing useful reproducibility of phosphenes in subjects chronically vision by epiretinal electrical stimulation hinges on the implanted with the Argus II retinal prosthesis (Second premise that stimulation with a single electrode gives rise Sight Medical Products, Inc., Sylmar, CA, USA). to a discrete focal percept in a retinotopic manner. DESIGN: Retrospective interventional case series and Simultaneous stimulation with multiple electrodes reliability study. therefore theoretically leads to perception of a pattern in METHODS: Six Argus II subjects of >5 years implanta- concordance with the pattern defined by the stimulating tion from a single site participated. The 4-electrode clus- electrodes.3 ter (‘‘quad’’) closest to fovea was stimulated in each In earlier studies, Rizzo and associates have called into subject with a fixed biphasic current. Perceived phos- question the consistency and reproducibility of phosphenes phenes were depicted relative to subjective visual field elicited by patterned epiretinal microelectrode stimula- center. The stimulus was applied at reducing time inter- tion.4 In a study involving 5 end-stage RP patients and 1 vals from 20 minutes to 1 second. Two sets of stimula- patient with normal retina, only 48% of the single- tions were performed on the same day and 2 further sets electrode stimulations and 32% of the multielectrode stim- repeated on a separate visit >1 week apart. ulations elicited visual percepts that matched the electrical RESULTS: Each subject depicted phosphenes of consis- stimulation patterns. Of the single-electrode stimulations, tent shapes and sizes, and reported seeing the same colors 3 subjects reported ‘‘a line’’ on some occasions, while ‘‘clus- with the fixed stimulating parameters, irrespective of the ters of 2 or 3 images’’ were seen on other occasions. In interstimuli intervals. However, there is a wide intersubject particular, the authors reported that only 66% (out of 99 variation in the phosphene characteristics. Four subjects stimulations) of the elicited visual percepts were reproduc- drew phosphenes in the same visual field quadrant, as ible in 3 RP patients on 2 separate trials, despite using the predicted by the quad-fovea location. Two subjects depicted same stimulating parameters to activate the same elec- phosphenes in the same hemifield as the expected locations. trodes. Such inconsistencies in the form and reproduc- CONCLUSION: Phosphenes for each subject were ibility of phosphenes would seriously undermine the consistently reproducible in all our chronically implanted formation of pixelated vision. subjects. This has important implications in the develop- In this study, we set out to investigate the consistency ment of long-term pixelated prosthetic vision for future and reproducibility of phosphenes elicited in a cohort of devices. (Am J Ophthalmol 2016;170:100–109. Ó subjects chronically implanted with the Argus II system 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.) at a single site. All of the subjects described have had the device implanted and functioning for more than 5 years. INCE ENTERING THE COMMERCIAL MARKET AS A retinal prosthetic device for the treatment for end- Ò S stage retinitis pigmentosa (RP), the Argus II METHODS Accepted for publication Jul 20, 2016. SUBJECT INCLUSION/EXCLUSION CRITERIA: This is a From the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Ophthalmology (Y.H- L.L., J.J.Z., M.C., L.d.C.) and Vitreoretinal Service (Y.H-L.L., L.d.C.), single-center prospective study. All but 1 subject from Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, London, United our center implanted with the Argus II retinal pros- Kingdom. thesis system as part of the phase I/II clinical trial Inquiries to Yvonne H-L. Luo, NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Ophthalmology, Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, 162 (clinicaltrials.gov identifier: NCT00407602) took part City Road, London EC1V 2PD, UK; e-mail: [email protected] in the study (n ¼ 6). One subject was excluded, as his 100 © 2016 ELSEVIER INC.ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. 0002-9394/$36.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ajo.2016.07.021 TABLE 1. Demographics and Operation Dates of the Participating Chronically Implanted Argus II Retinal Prosthesis Subjects Year of Age at Time of Subject ID Diagnosis Operation Operation (y) 001 Retinitis pigmentosa 2008 70 003 Retinitis pigmentosa 2008 72 005 Retinitis pigmentosa 2009 55 006 Choroideremia 2009 66 007 Retinitis pigmentosa 2009 63 009 Retinitis pigmentosa 2009 45 device ceased to function after he developed retinal detachment and thick macular pucker as a result of a FIGURE 1. Red-free fundus photograph of the microelectrode fall. The participating subjects’ demographics and oper- array of a chronically implanted Argus II retinal prosthesis sub- ation dates are shown in Table 1.Thestudywas ject (ID: 007). The designated quad for stimulation consisted of electrodes E07E08F07F08 (enclosed in white square). The approved by the institutional review boards and ethics fovea location is estimated to be 15.5 degrees temporal and committee, and adhered to the tenets of the Declara- 1.5 degrees inferior to the center of the optic disc. The quad- tion of Helsinki. fovea relation is calculated from the estimated fovea location, to the center of the stimulated quad. SELECTION OF STIMULATING ELECTRODES AND PA- RAMETERS: For each subject, a cluster of 4 electrodes (hereinafter referred to as a ‘‘quad’’) closest to the fovea, which were functioning with thresholds within the safety was performed, to assess the contact between the stimu- charge density limit, was selected for stimulation, and the lating electrodes and the retinal surface. The selected elicited phosphenes characterized for the purpose of this quad, quad-retina relation, quad threshold, and stimulating study (Figure 1). current for each subject were as shown in Table 2. An estimated location of the fovea was made on the fundus photograph (taken at the outset of the study) and PHOSPHENES DEPICTION: To record the phosphenes was used for each subject as a reference point, measuring perceived by each subject, we constructed a wall covered 15.5 6 1.1 degrees from the center of the optic disc hori- with smooth-surface black mats. The subjects were first zontally and À1.5 6 0.9 degrees vertically.6 The foveal po- asked to stand up and stretch out both arms fully to touch sition was estimated, as there were no remaining features of the black wall, so that their shoulders were square, facing the fovea on color photographs, fluorescein angiograms, or the wall. The standing position of each subject was then optical coherence tomography (OCT) scans owing to se- adjusted so that the distance between the front of their vere end-stage RP. eyes and the wall equaled 30 cm. Next, the subjects were Once the designated quad was chosen, the stimulating asked to point with the index finger of both hands simulta- current for each subject was arbitrarily set to be 100 mA neously on the black wall, to where they believed the cen- above the threshold (measured within the last 6 months) ter of their visual field was, while keeping their head and initially, and then adjusted according to the strength of eyes pointing straight ahead. A stack of white A4-size response and comfort level reported by the subject. We papers (in landscape layout) was then placed underneath aimed to elicit a clear, definite visual percept without the index fingers of each subject and pinned to the wall, causing any discomfort or physical ‘‘tingling’’ sensation so that the index fingers were pointing at the center of for each subject. Default settings for the Argus II retinal the top sheet of paper (ie, the center of the paper was prosthesis system Clinical Fitting System (CFS) employed approximating the proclaimed center of each subject’s for device fitting and standard testing were likewise used for visual field). A drawing pin with a protruding cylindrical this study, which generate cathodic-first, charge-balanced head was then inserted at the point where their index fin- biphasic square waves to avoid tissue damage from charge gers contacted with the wall, so as to mark the location of build-up. These default waveform parameters were as fol- the proclaimed visual field center. lows: phase width of 0.46 ms, interphase duration of 0 sec- During the experiment, the subjects were asked to posi- onds, and total stimulation duration of 250 ms at the tion themselves according to the setup above, hold onto frequency of 20 Hz.7 Swept-source OCT (DRI OCT-1 the preplaced drawing pin head, and adjust their head Atlantis; TOPCON, Topcon Medical Systems, Tokyo, and eye position until they felt the center of their visual Japan) imaging through the
Recommended publications
  • Subliminal Afterimages Via Ocular Delayed Luminescence: Transsaccade Stability of the Visual Perception and Color Illusion
    ACTIVITAS NERVOSA SUPERIOR Activitas Nervosa Superior 2012, 54, No. 1-2 REVIEW ARTICLE SUBLIMINAL AFTERIMAGES VIA OCULAR DELAYED LUMINESCENCE: TRANSSACCADE STABILITY OF THE VISUAL PERCEPTION AND COLOR ILLUSION István Bókkon1,2 & Ram L.P. Vimal2 1Doctoral School of Pharmaceutical and Pharmacological Sciences, Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary 2Vision Research Institute, Lowell, MA, USA Abstract Here, we suggest the existence and possible roles of evanescent nonconscious afterimages in visual saccades and color illusions during normal vision. These suggested functions of subliminal afterimages are based on our previous papers (i) (Bókkon, Vimal et al. 2011, J. Photochem. Photobiol. B) related to visible light induced ocular delayed bioluminescence as a possible origin of negative afterimage and (ii) Wang, Bókkon et al. (Brain Res. 2011)’s experiments that proved the existence of spontaneous and visible light induced delayed ultraweak photon emission from in vitro freshly isolated rat’s whole eye, lens, vitreous humor and retina. We also argue about the existence of rich detailed, subliminal visual short-term memory across saccades in early retinotopic areas. We conclude that if we want to understand the complex visual processes, mere electrical processes are hardly enough for explanations; for that we have to consider the natural photobiophysical processes as elaborated in this article. Key words: Saccades Nonconscious afterimages Ocular delayed bioluminescence Color illusion 1. INTRODUCTION Previously, we presented a common photobiophysical basis for various visual related phenomena such as discrete retinal noise, retinal phosphenes, as well as negative afterimages. These new concepts have been supported by experiments (Wang, Bókkon et al., 2011). They performed the first experimental proof of spontaneous ultraweak biophoton emission and visible light induced delayed ultraweak photon emission from in vitro freshly isolated rat’s whole eye, lens, vitreous humor, and retina.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Perception in Migraine: a Narrative Review
    vision Review Visual Perception in Migraine: A Narrative Review Nouchine Hadjikhani 1,2,* and Maurice Vincent 3 1 Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02129, USA 2 Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, 41119 Gothenburg, Sweden 3 Eli Lilly and Company, Indianapolis, IN 46285, USA; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-617-724-5625 Abstract: Migraine, the most frequent neurological ailment, affects visual processing during and between attacks. Most visual disturbances associated with migraine can be explained by increased neural hyperexcitability, as suggested by clinical, physiological and neuroimaging evidence. Here, we review how simple (e.g., patterns, color) visual functions can be affected in patients with migraine, describe the different complex manifestations of the so-called Alice in Wonderland Syndrome, and discuss how visual stimuli can trigger migraine attacks. We also reinforce the importance of a thorough, proactive examination of visual function in people with migraine. Keywords: migraine aura; vision; Alice in Wonderland Syndrome 1. Introduction Vision consumes a substantial portion of brain processing in humans. Migraine, the most frequent neurological ailment, affects vision more than any other cerebral function, both during and between attacks. Visual experiences in patients with migraine vary vastly in nature, extent and intensity, suggesting that migraine affects the central nervous system (CNS) anatomically and functionally in many different ways, thereby disrupting Citation: Hadjikhani, N.; Vincent, M. several components of visual processing. Migraine visual symptoms are simple (positive or Visual Perception in Migraine: A Narrative Review. Vision 2021, 5, 20. negative), or complex, which involve larger and more elaborate vision disturbances, such https://doi.org/10.3390/vision5020020 as the perception of fortification spectra and other illusions [1].
    [Show full text]
  • Phosphene Perception Is Due to the Ultra-Weak Photon Emission Produced in Various Parts of the Visual System: Glutamate in the Focus
    Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2016 Phosphene perception is due to the ultra-weak photon emission produced in various parts of the visual system: glutamate in the focus Császár, Noémi ; Scholkmann, Felix ; Salari, Vahid ; Szőke, Henrik ; Bókkon, István Abstract: Phosphenes are experienced sensations of light, when there is no light causing them. The physiological processes underlying this phenomenon are still not well understood. Previously, we proposed a novel biopsychophysical approach concerning the cause of phosphenes based on the assumption that cellular endogenous ultra-weak photon emission (UPE) is the biophysical cause leading to the sensation of phosphenes. Briefly summarized, the visual sensation of light (phosphenes) is likely to be duetothe inherent perception of UPE of cells in the visual system. If the intensity of spontaneous or induced photon emission of cells in the visual system exceeds a distinct threshold, it is hypothesized that it can become a conscious light sensation. Discussing several new and previous experiments, we point out that the UPE theory of phosphenes should be really considered as a scientifically appropriate and provable mechanism to explain the physiological basis of phosphenes. In the present paper, we also present our idea that some experiments may support that the cortical phosphene lights are due to the glutamate-related excess UPE in the occipital cortex. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2015-0039 Posted at the Zurich Open Repository and Archive, University of Zurich ZORA URL: https://doi.org/10.5167/uzh-126012 Journal Article Published Version Originally published at: Császár, Noémi; Scholkmann, Felix; Salari, Vahid; Szőke, Henrik; Bókkon, István (2016).
    [Show full text]
  • Taming Hallucinations: an Investigation of the Phenomenology and Precipitating Conditions of Visual
    Taming hallucinations: An investigation of the phenomenology and precipitating conditions of visual hallucinations in the general population Sebastian Rogers A thesis in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy School of Psychology Faculty of Science March 2019 Thesis/Dissertation Sheet Surname/Family Name : Rogers Given Name/s : Sebastian Liam Abbreviation for degree as give in the University calendar : PhD Faculty : Science School : Psychology Taming hallucinations: An investigation of the phenomenology and Thesis Title : precipitating conditions of visual hallucinations in the general population Abstract 350 words maximum: (PLEASE TYPE) Historically, the investigation of visual hallucinations has been hindered by the complexity and unpredictability of their occurrence and content. However, presenting individuals with luminance flicker confined to an annulus reliably induces visual hallucinations comprised of numerous shadowy, colourless, circular shapes rotating around the annular path, thereby facilitating objective mechanistic investigation of hallucinations by overcoming their unpredictability, heterogeneity, and complexity. The work presented in this thesis utilises these hallucinations in combination with behavioural and neuroscientific experimentation to explore processes underlying hallucination phenomenology, and the factors that precipitate individual hallucination episodes. The experiments reported in Chapter 2 probed the low- level visual features of induced hallucinations. Using a novel method for objectively estimating hallucination sensory strength, we present evidence that hallucination strength varies with the frequency of the inducing flicker. We also analysed the temporal dynamics of hallucinatory motion to test the claim that the hallucinations are bistable, and to unveil clues about their underlying neural processing. The experiments in Chapter 3 targeted the neural mechanisms that determine hallucination sensory strength.
    [Show full text]
  • What Do Blind People “See” with Retinal Prostheses? Observations And
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.03.932905; this version posted February 4, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. 1 What do blind people “see” with retinal prostheses? Observations and qualitative reports of epiretinal 2 implant users 3 4 5 6 Cordelia Erickson-Davis1¶* and Helma Korzybska2¶* 7 8 9 10 11 1 Stanford School of Medicine and Stanford Anthropology Department, Stanford University, Palo Alto, 12 California, United States of America 13 14 2 Laboratory of Ethnology and Comparative Sociology (LESC), Paris Nanterre University, Nanterre, France 15 16 17 18 19 * Corresponding author. 20 E-mail: [email protected], [email protected] 21 22 23 ¶ The authors contributed equally to this work. 24 25 26 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.03.932905; this version posted February 4, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. 27 Abstract 28 29 Introduction: Retinal implants have now been approved and commercially available for certain 30 clinical populations for over 5 years, with hundreds of individuals implanted, scores of them closely 31 followed in research trials. Despite these numbers, however, few data are available that would help 32 us answer basic questions regarding the nature and outcomes of artificial vision: what do 33 participants see when the device is turned on for the first time, and how does that change over time? 34 35 Methods: Semi-structured interviews and observations were undertaken at two sites in France and 36 the UK with 16 participants who had received either the Argus II or IRIS II devices.
    [Show full text]
  • From Prehistoric Shamanism to Early Civilizations: Eye Floater Structures in Ancient Egypt*
    May, 2012 Volume 12, No. 2 From Prehistoric Shamanism to Early Civilizations: Eye Floater Structures in Ancient Egypt* By Floco Tausin Abstract This article is based on the assumption that prehistoric shamanic rituals include the perception, interpretation and depiction of what we today know as eye floaters (muscae volitantes). It is suggested that, together with other shamanic symbols, floaters continue to be experienced and depicted not only in later shamanic societies up to the present day, but also entered the visual arts of early civilizations. The present article supports this thesis from the example of ancient Egypt. A closer look at Egyptian visual arts reveals geometric structures and characteristics that are typical of eye floaters. It is speculated that two central mythological concepts, the sun and the world, are directly or indirectly inspired by the perception of floaters. Key words: eye floaters, entoptic phenomena, phosphenes, visual arts, ancient Egypt, shamanism What are eye floaters? Many people experience mobile and scattered semi- transparent dots and strands in the visual field, best perceived in bright light conditions (See Figure 1). They float according to eye movements, which makes them hard to focus on. People often consult their eye doctors because they are worried by these dots and strands. Usually, the doctors check the patients’ eyes, find nothing to worry about and reassure the patients that these dots and strands are called eye floaters or vitreous floaters, also known as muscae volitantes (Latin: “flying Figure 1: Example of semi- flies”). They are explained as opacities in the gel transparent, mobile dots and strings between the lens and the retina (vitreous humor) due to in the visual field.
    [Show full text]
  • "Visual Prostheses"
    530 VISUAL PROSTHESES VISUAL PROSTHESES JEAN DELBEKE CLAUDE VERAART Catholic University of Louvain Brussels, Belgium INTRODUCTION Minute electrical stimuli delivered to the retina, the optic nerve, or the occipital cortex can induce light perceptions called phosphenes. The visual prosthesis aims at exploiting these phosphenes to restore a form of vision in some cases of blindness. Very schematically, a camera or a picture capturing device transforms images into electrical signals that are then adapted and passed on to some still functional part of the visual pathways, thus bridging the defective structures. The system has at least some parts implanted, including electrodes and their stimulator circuits. A photo- sensitive array in the eye could provide the necessary image input, but most approaches use an external minia- ture camera. Typically, the visual data handling requires an external processor and the power supply as well as the data are provided to the implant by a transcutaneous transmission system. Despite a first pioneering attempt by Brindley and Lewin as early as 1968 (1) only very few experimental visual prostheses have been implanted in humans so far. The limited accessibility of the involved anatomical struc- tures, the poorly understood neural encoding, and the huge amount of information handled by the visual nervous system have clearly hampered a development that can not yet be compared with the far more advanced evolution of cochlear implants (see article on Cochlear Implants in this encyclopedia). The visual prosthesis is still at a very Encyclopedia of Medical Devices and Instrumentation, Second Edition, edited by John G. Webster Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • Migraine Aura Without Headache Donald M
    B rief Reports Migraine Aura Without Headache Donald M. Pedersen, PA-C, PhD, William M. Wilson, PhD, George L. White, Jr, PA-C PhD Richard T. Murdock, MS/HSA, and Kathleen B. Digre, MD Salt Lake City, Utah Migraine is described as a familial disorder characterized to the disturbance described. The patient also denies by recurrent headaches that are variable in intensity, antecedent trauma or emotional stress. The episodes arc frequency, and duration.1 Attacks are usually unilateral reportedly always similar in nature, with an expanding but can also be bilateral and accompanied by throbbing scintillating scotoma and without subsequent headache, pain, photophobia, phonophobia, nausea, and vomiting. The patient’s first episode, his m ost recent episode, and Some migraines are preceded by, or are associated with, “a few” of the others have occurred after a 60-minutc neurological and mood disturbances. All of the above exercise period, which he performs consistently as a mat­ characteristics, however, are not necessarily present in ter of his daily routine. There is a history of myopia, for each attack, nor in each patient.2 correction o f which soft contact lenses are used, and It has been suggested that die prevalence of mi­ numerous vitreous floaters have been reported. The pa­ graine is probably markedly underestimated. Estimates tient takes no medication, has no history of illicit drug range from 10% to 34% of the general population, with use, and is otherwise healthy except for a history of mild some authors reporting both age-related and sex-related seasonal allergic rhinitis. There is a family history of differences.
    [Show full text]
  • Subjective Visual Experiences of Colour and Form Induced by Temporally Modulated Light
    Subjective visual experiences of colour and form induced by temporally modulated light Cordula Becker M¨unchen2005 Subjective visual experiences of colour and form induced by temporally modulated light Cordula Becker Inaugural-Dissertation zur Erlangung des Doktorgrades der Philosophie an der Ludwig–Maximilians–Universit¨at M¨unchen vorgelegt von Cordula Becker aus Magdeburg M¨unchen, den 21.03.2005 Erstgutachter: Prof. Hermann J. M¨uller Zweitgutachter: Prof. Heiner Deubel Tag der m¨undlichen Pr¨ufung:29.06.2005 Ich hatte die Gabe, wenn ich die Augen schloß und mit niedergesenktem Haupte mir in die Mitte des Sehorgans eine Blume dachte, so verharrte sie nicht einen Augenblick in ihrer ersten Gestalt, sondern sie legte sich auseinander, und aus ihrem Inneren entfalteten sich wieder neue Blumen aus farbigen, auch wohl gr¨unen Bl¨attern;es waren keine nat¨urlichenBlumen, sondern phantastische, jedoch regel- m¨aßigwie die Rosetten der Bildhauer. Es war unm¨oglich,die hervorsprossende Sch¨opfungzu fixieren, hingegen dauerte sie so lange, als mir beliebte, ermattete nicht und verst¨arktesich nicht. Dasselbe konnte ich hervorbringen, wenn ich mir den Zierrat einer buntgemalten Scheibe dachte, welche dann ebenfalls aus der Mitte gegen die Peripherie hin sich immerfort ver¨anderte,v¨ollig wie die in unseren Tagen erst erfundenen Kaleidoskope. Johann Wolfgang von Goethe Studien zur Morphologie I wish to express my thanks to everyone who helped me during the various phases of writing this dissertation. I am thankful to Hermann M¨uller for the opportunity to do research and write a thesis in his group in Munich. I also wish to thank him and Heiner Deubel for their willingness to act as reviewers of this thesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Distinct Oscillatory Frequencies Underlie Excitability of Human Occipital and Parietal Cortex
    2824 • The Journal of Neuroscience, March 15, 2017 • 37(11):2824–2833 Behavioral/Cognitive Distinct Oscillatory Frequencies Underlie Excitability of Human Occipital and Parietal Cortex X Jason Samaha,1 Olivia Gosseries,2,3 and Bradley R. Postle1,2 1Department of Psychology and 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin 53704, and 3Coma Science Group, GIGA consciousness research, University and University Hospital of Lie`ge, 4000 Lie`ge-Belgium Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of human occipital and posterior parietal cortex can give rise to visual sensations called phosphenes. We used near-threshold TMS with concurrent EEG recordings to measure how oscillatory brain dynamics covary, on single trials,withtheperceptionofphosphenesafteroccipitalandparietalTMS.Prestimuluspowerandphase,predominantlyinthealphaband (8–13 Hz), predicted occipital TMS phosphenes, whereas higher-frequency beta-band (13–20 Hz) power (but not phase) predicted parietal TMS phosphenes. TMS-evoked responses related to phosphene perception were similar across stimulation sites and were characterized by an early (200 ms) posterior negativity and a later (Ͼ300 ms) parietal positivity in the time domain and an increase in low-frequency (ϳ5–7 Hz) power followed by a broadband decrease in alpha/beta power in the time–frequency domain. These correlates of phosphene perception closely resemble known electrophysiological correlates of conscious perception of near-threshold visual stim- uli. The regionally differential pattern of prestimulus
    [Show full text]
  • Retina Or Visual Cortex? the Site of Phosphene Induction by Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation
    GENERAL COMMENTARY published: 18 May 2009 INTEGRATIVE NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.006.2009 Retina or visual cortex? The site of phosphene induction by transcranial alternating current stimulation Caspar M. Schwiedrzik* Department of Neurophysiology, Max Planck Institute for Brain Research, Frankfurt am Main, Germany *Correspondence: [email protected] A commentary on If this were the case, tACS would poten- needed to elicit phosphenes increased with tially be an interesting tool to inquire the distance from the eye, Rohracher concluded Frequency-dependent electrical stimula- causal role of oscillatory synchrony in cor- that the most likely site of stimulation was tion of the visual cortex. tical processing. However, previously pub- the retina (although he also considered an Kanai, R., Chaieb, L., Antal, A., Walsh, V., lished evidence renders the interpretation effect of AC stimulation on the cortex). and Paulus, W. (2008). Curr. Biol. 18, that tACS interacts directly with cortical A few years later, Schwarz (1947) showed 1839–1843. activity questionable: It has been suggested that the intensity needed to elicit phos- that peripheral phosphenes as observed by phenes by AC stimulation depends on the Since the fi rst descriptions of oscillatory Kanai et al. (2008) are elicited on the level of adaptational state of the eye. The initial synchrony in the cortex, a plethora of stud- the retina, and not in cortical structures. adjustment of retinal sensitivity to back- ies have provided empirical evidence for The fi rst successful attempts to stimulate ground illumination only takes fractions of correlations between cortical oscillations the visual system by alternating current (AC) a second (Meister and Berry, 1999), thus 5 s and perception, attention, and other cog- were already introduced in the fi rst half of of eyes closed as in Kanai et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Reduced Fading of Visual Afterimages After Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Over Early Visual Cortex
    Reduced Fading of Visual Afterimages after Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation over Early Visual Cortex Tahnée Engelen1,2*, Rosanne L. Rademaker3,4*, and Alexander T. Sack1 Abstract ■ In the complete absence of small transients in visual inputs stimulation (TMS) was applied over EVC. In a first experiment, (e.g., by experimentally stabilizing an image on the retina or in triple-pulse TMS was able to make a faded hand appear less faded everyday life during intent staring), information perceived by the after the pulses were applied, compared with placebo pulses. A eyes will fade from the perceptual experience. Although the second experiment demonstrated that this was because triple- mechanisms of visual fading remain poorly understood, one pos- pulse TMS slowed down fading of the removed hand that other- sibility is that higher level brain regions actively suppress the sta- wise occurs naturally over time. Interestingly, TMS similarly ble visual signals via targeted feedback onto early visual cortex affected the left and right hands, despite being applied only over (EVC). Here, we used positive afterimages and multisensory con- the right EVC. Together, our results suggest that TMS over EVC flict to induce gestalt-like fading of participants’ own hands. In attenuates the effects of visual fading in positive afterimages, and two separate experiments, participants rated the perceived qual- it might do so by crossing transcollosal connections or via multi- ity of their hands both before and after transcranial magnetic modal integration sites in which both hands are represented. ■ INTRODUCTION flash, a grayscale visual afterimage develops of everything Our perception of the world is the result of smooth inte- that was perceived during the flash.
    [Show full text]