Effects of Attention on Visual Experience During Monocular Rivalry ⇑ Eric A

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Effects of Attention on Visual Experience During Monocular Rivalry ⇑ Eric A Vision Research 83 (2013) 76–81 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Effects of attention on visual experience during monocular rivalry ⇑ Eric A. Reavis a, , Peter J. Kohler a, Gideon P. Caplovitz b, Thalia P. Wheatley a, Peter U. Tse a a Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, 6207 Moore Hall, Hanover, NH 03755, United States b Department of Psychology, University of Nevada at Reno, 1664 North Virginia Street, Psychology Department Mailstop 0296, Reno, NV 89557-0296, United States article info abstract Article history: There is a long-running debate over the extent to which volitional attention can modulate the appearance Received 20 November 2012 of visual stimuli. Here we use monocular rivalry between afterimages to explore the effects of attention Received in revised form 4 February 2013 on the contents of visual experience. In three experiments, we demonstrate that attended afterimages are Available online 13 March 2013 seen for longer periods, on average, than unattended afterimages. This occurs both when a feature of the afterimage is attended directly and when a frame surrounding the afterimage is attended. The results of Keywords: these experiments show that volitional attention can dramatically influence the contents of visual Visual attention experience. Monocular rivalry Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Consciousness Afterimages 1. Introduction Logothetis, 2002; Leopold & Logothetis, 1999). It has been shown to exhibit a characteristic distribution of dominance durations Can volitional attention alter the appearance of visual stimuli? and alternation rates that is similar to those observed in ambigu- Disagreement over this question has persisted for over a century, ous figure perception (Brascamp et al., 2005; van Ee, 2005; Zhao with, for example, Hermann von Helmholtz arguing in the affirma- et al., 2004). Based largely on the similar properties of binocular tive and Gustav Fechner in the negative (Helmholtz, 1866/1924). In rivalry and ambiguous figure perception, some have proposed that recent years, evidence has begun to accumulate in favor of the po- a single, supra-modal mechanism for resolving perceptual ambigu- sition that volitional attention can indeed alter the appearance of ity could drive the alternations in all forms of perceptual multista- visual stimuli (Abrams, Barbot, & Carrasco, 2010; Liu, Abrams, & bility (Leopold & Logothetis, 1999). If this is true, then it would Carrasco, 2009; Tse, 2005). Volitional attention can also alter the follow that manipulations which affect the alternation process in contents of visual experience by biasing perception toward a par- one form of multistability would likely have similar effects on ticular percept in binocular rivalry and ambiguous figure percep- the alternation process in other forms of multistability. tion, where visual experience alternates between several Binocular rivalry and ambiguous figure perception can both be different percepts (Chong, Tadin, & Blake, 2005; Hancock & influenced by volitional attention (Chong, Tadin, & Blake, 2005; Andrews, 2007; Meng & Tong, 2004; Ooi & He, 1999; van Ee, van Hancock & Andrews, 2007; Meng & Tong, 2004; Ooi & He, 1999; Dam, & Brouwer, 2005). Here we demonstrate for the first time van Ee, van Dam, & Brouwer, 2005). If shared mechanisms are an effect of volitional attention on another type of perceptual mul- responsible for the perceptual switching that occurs in monocular tistability: monocular rivalry. rivalry, binocular rivalry, and ambiguous figure perception, then In monocular rivalry, a single image of two spatially overlapping attention might have a similar effect on perceptual switching in transparent surfaces can lead to the experience of perceptual alter- monocular rivalry as it does in other types of multistable percep- nations between the two surfaces (Breese, 1899). Qualitatively, tion. To our knowledge, it remains an open question whether monocular rivalry resembles binocular rivalry, where different attention influences monocular rivalry (although O’Shea (2006), images presented to the two eyes alternate in visibility. Indeed, published an abstract reporting possible effects of attention on there is evidence that the alternation dynamics and suppression ef- monocular rivalry). fects which occur in monocular rivalry are similar to those of binoc- Here, we examine the influence of volitional attention on the vi- ular rivalry (Maier, Logothetis, & Leopold, 2005; O’Shea et al., 2009). sual experience of color afterimages engaged in monocular rivalry. Binocular rivalry has been characterized far more extensively Afterimages were induced by stimuli like those shown in Fig. 1. than monocular rivalry (for reviews, see Blake, 2001; Blake & Color afterimages typically occur when opponent-coded retinal ganglion cells undergo a rebound in firing following prolonged exposure to an unchanging stimulus (Zaidi et al., 2012). To the ⇑ Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (E.A. Reavis). viewer, the afterimages appear like a weak version of the color 0042-6989/$ - see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2013.03.002 E.A. Reavis et al. / Vision Research 83 (2013) 76–81 77 an outlier who, in several analyses, was more than three standard deviations from the group mean. Exclusion of the outlier did not change the direction or significance of any of the reported results. In this and subsequent experiments, all participants gave informed consent to participate, and all experimental procedures were ap- proved by the Dartmouth Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects. Participants viewed stimuli on a Mitsubishi Diamond Pro 2070SB CRT monitor from a distance of 57 cm, in a darkened room. Stimuli were generated and presented using the Psychophysics Toolbox, version 3, on a PC running MATLAB R2010a in Linux (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997; MathWorks, Natick, MA). Each participant completed 100 experimental trials. On each trial, participants fixated on a dot in the center of the screen and pressed a key to initiate the trial. While fixating, they passively viewed an adapting stimulus for 10 s (example in Fig. 1a). The adapting stimulus contained two overlapping rectangular frames forming a plus-sign (12 Â 12 deg vis. ang., area of overlap 8 Â 8 deg). Each frame was filled with a transparent texture of blue and yellow or red and green stripes. The background had a mean luminance of 102 cd/m2, and the adapting stimuli had a mean luminance of 64 cd/m2 in the overlapping region. The color of the texture within each frame was randomly assigned for each trial. The orientation of the stripes was counterbalanced: half of the tri- als contained stripes perpendicular to the rectangles, half parallel to the rectangles. After the adapting period of each trial, all stimuli except the fix- ation point disappeared, and a tone sounded for 50 ms as a non-vi- sual attentional cue. Participants were instructed to pay attention to the vertical frame on trials when they heard a high tone, and Fig. 1. Example stimuli. Please refer to the web version of the article for color to the horizontal frame on trials when they heard a low tone. stimuli. Panel A corresponds to a ‘parallel stripes’ trial in Experiment 1. Panel B Immediately after the tone, the rectangular frames reappeared shows an example from Experiment 2. Panel C illustrates a trial from Experiment 3. for 10 s. Participants reported what they saw at each moment The reader can experience a demonstration of the effects by fixating in the center of one of the three colored stimuli for ten or more seconds, then shifting fixation to the within the square region of overlap between the two rectangles. empty frames at right, and attending to one of the two frames (or stripe They held one key if they saw vertical stripes, another if they orientations, in Panel C) The afterimage corresponding to the attended surface saw horizontal stripes, and a third if they saw a mixture of both should tend to be seen for longer periods than the unattended surface. orientations. They were instructed to hold the third key anytime the overlapping region was not exclusively filled by just one of inverse of the adapting stimulus, but the exact contents of the per- the two afterimage patterns (i.e., if there was any amount of piece- cept can be influenced by contextual cues presented during after- meal rivalry). Participants switched keys as often as necessary to image perception (Daw, 1962; van Lier, Vergeer, & Anstis, 2009). accurately reflect their visual experience, and released all the keys For post-retinal visual processes such as attention, afterimages when they no longer saw an afterimage. are likely equivalent to direct retinal input. The main advantage of using afterimages to study monocular rivalry is that afterimage 2.2. Results percepts are stabilized on the retina, and are therefore conve- niently immune to the confounding effects of eye movements that The average probability of every possible response at each mo- can otherwise influence monocular rivalry alternations (Crassini & ment of the response period can be seen in Fig. 2a. Responses con- Broerse, 1982; Georgeson, 1984). gruent with the attended stimulus are in green, those congruent In three experiments, we show that the mean dominance dura- with the unattended stimulus are in red, and mixed responses tion of attended afterimage surfaces engaged in monocular rivalry in
Recommended publications
  • Monocular Rivalry Exhibits Three Hallmarks of Binocular Rivalry
    This article may not exactly replicate the final version published. It is not the copy of record. 1 Monocular rivalry exhibits three hallmarks of binocular rivalry Robert P. O’Shea 1* , David Alais 2, Amanda L. Parker 2 and David J. La Rooy 1 1Department of Psychology, University of Otago, PO Box 56, Dunedin, New Zealand 2 School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Australia * Corresponding author: e-mail: [email protected] Acknowledgements: We are grateful to Frank Tong for allowing us to use his stimuli for Experiment 2, and to Janine Mendola for helpful discussion. O'Shea, R. P., Parker, A. L., La Rooy, D. J. & Alais, D. (2009).Monocular rivalry exhibits three hallmarks of binocular rivalry: Evidence for common processes. Vision Research, 49, 671–681. http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/263/descri ption#description This article may not exactly replicate the final version published. It is not the copy of record. 2 Abstract Binocular rivalry occurs when different images are presented one to each eye: the images are visible only alternately. Monocular rivalry occurs when different images are presented both to the same eye: the clarity of the images fluctuates alternately. Could both sorts of rivalry reflect the operation of a general visual mechanism for dealing with perceptual ambiguity? We report four experiments showing similarities between the two phenomena. First, we show that monocular rivalry can occur with complex images, as with binocular rivalry, and that the two phenomena are affected similarly by the size and colour of the images. Second, we show that the distribution of dominance periods during monocular rivalry has a gamma shape and is stochastic.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatio-Temporal Integration of an Object's Surface Information in Mid-Level Vision
    Spatio-temporal integration of an object's surface information in mid-level vision A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY Shin Ho Cho IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Professor Daniel J. Kersten and Sheng He June 2015 © Copyright by Shin Ho Cho 2015 All right Reserved Acknowledgements I would like to express the deepest appreciation to my two academic advisers, Professor Daniel J. Kersten, and Professor Sheng He. Both have the attitude and the substance of a genius: they continually and convincingly conveyed a spirit of adventure in regard to research and scholarship, and an excitement to explore the unknown intellectual world. Without their guidance and persistent help this dissertation would not have been possible. I would like to thank my committee members Professor Stephen Engel and Professor Bin He, whose comments and encouragement were very helpful in progressing my research and enabling me to finish this dissertation project. Also, I thank the University of Minnesota for financial support, especially the Doctoral Dissertation Fellowship Program. Shinho Cho (조신호) i To my parents and wife ii Abstract The human visual system can construct a 3D viewpoint of visual objects based on their 2D contours. This process is presumably an essential part of the visual system that enables interaction with an environment, e.g., grasping an object. Despite its importance, it is not clearly understood to what extent conscious awareness is involved in constructing 3D information from visual input. Here we investigated whether the 3D viewpoint of the object could be extracted and represented by the visual system when observers were not aware of the object’s image.
    [Show full text]
  • A Primer on Binocular Rivalry, Including Current Controversies
    A Primer on Binocular Rivalry, Including Current Controversies Randolph Blake Vanderbilt Vision Research Center Vanderbilt University Nashville TN 37240 USA running head: Binocular Rivalry email: [email protected] voice: 615-343-7010 FAX: 615-343-5029 Binocular Rivalry Abstract. Among psychologists and vision scientists, binocular rivalry has enjoyed sustained interest for decades dating back to the 19th century. In recent years, however, rivalry’s audience has expanded to include neuroscientists who envision rivalry as a “tool” for exploring the neural concomitants of conscious visual awareness and perceptual organization. For rivalry’s potential to be realized, workers using this “tool” need to know details of this fascinating phenomenon, and providing those details is the purpose of this article. After placing rivalry in a historical context, I summarize major findings concerning the spatial characteristics and the temporal dynamics of rivalry, discuss two major theoretical accounts of rivalry (“eye” vs “stimulus” rivalry) and speculate on possible neural concomitants of binocular rivalry. key words: binocular rivalry, suppression, conscious awareness, neural model, perceptual organization 1. Introduction The human brain has been touted as the most complex structure in the known universe (Thompson, 1985). This may be true, but despite its awesome powers the brain can behave like a confused adolescent when it is confronted with conflicting visual messages. When dissimilar visual stimuli are imaged on corresponding retinal regions of the two eyes, the brain lapses into an unstable state characterized by alternating periods of perceptual dominance during which one visual stimulus or the other is seen at a time. This confusion is understandable, for the eyes are signalling the brain that two different objects exist at the same location in space at the same time.
    [Show full text]
  • Copyright and Use of This Thesis This Thesis Must Be Used in Accordance with the Provisions of the Copyright Act 1968
    COPYRIGHT AND USE OF THIS THESIS This thesis must be used in accordance with the provisions of the Copyright Act 1968. Reproduction of material protected by copyright may be an infringement of copyright and copyright owners may be entitled to take legal action against persons who infringe their copyright. Section 51 (2) of the Copyright Act permits an authorized officer of a university library or archives to provide a copy (by communication or otherwise) of an unpublished thesis kept in the library or archives, to a person who satisfies the authorized officer that he or she requires the reproduction for the purposes of research or study. The Copyright Act grants the creator of a work a number of moral rights, specifically the right of attribution, the right against false attribution and the right of integrity. You may infringe the author’s moral rights if you: - fail to acknowledge the author of this thesis if you quote sections from the work - attribute this thesis to another author - subject this thesis to derogatory treatment which may prejudice the author’s reputation For further information contact the University’s Director of Copyright Services sydney.edu.au/copyright A cross-modal investigation into the relationship between bistable perception and a global temporal mechanism by Amanda Louise Parker A thesis submitted in total fulfilment of the requirements of a Doctor of Philosophy in Science School of Psychology The University of Sydney 2013 Table of contents ORGANISATION OF THE THESIS ........................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • On the Dissociation Between Vision and Perception
    On the neuronal activity in the human brain during visual recognition, imagery and binocular rivalry Thesis by Gabriel Kreiman In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy California Institute of Technology Pasadena, California 2002 (Defended August 29, 2001) ii © 2002 Gabriel Kreiman All Rights Reserved iii To all those teachers who taught me to enjoy learning iv v Acknowledgments Haec ego non multis scribo, sed tibi: satis enim magnum alter alteri theatrum sumus1. All of the work described in the following chapters would not have been possible without the enthusiastic help of a large and nice group of colleagues and friends. First of all, I would like to thank all the patients who participated in these experiments. We have not paid them and they have cooperated for the advancement of science. Candi Maechtlen and Irene Wainwright have provided valuable and prompt help throughout these years in several of the fundamental details that keep things going. Irene has also provided excellent editorial assistance in several of our publications. I have been fortunate to be able to interact and learn from a large number of teachers, colleagues and friends throughout my entire life. While a number of them have not contributed directly to this thesis work, it seems evident that they have enormously influenced my education and my preparation. Among them, I would like to especially mention my chess teacher Cesar Corte who was perhaps among the first who showed me the way to indulge in the wonderful task of solving new problems. The long hours of scientific exploration clearly have a root in me in the uncountable hours that we spent in front of a chessboard trying to figure out the solution to a problem or the best move.
    [Show full text]