<<

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993- VOL. 8, NO.3 213

Lisuride Reduces Psychomotor Retardation during Withdrawal from Chronic Intravenous Self-Administration in Rats LuigiPulvirenti, M.D. and George F. Koob, Ph.D.

Withdrawal from chronic use of psychostimulant in behavioral changes, the effect of repeated treatment with hI/mans induces a clinical syndrome characterized by the nonaddictive derivative during the fatigue, psychomotor depression, anhedonia, and withdrawal phase was evaluated. At a dose devoid of any disturbances of sleep. Spontaneous locomotor activity and effects on locomotor activity, lisuride completely CIIJIlepsy were assessed in rats during withdrawal from a prevented the reduction in locomotor activity and the rhedule of intravenous self-administration of high doses increase in catalepsy produced by amphetamine � amphetamine. At 2 and 4 days after cessation of withdrawal. These results suggest the need for further Imphetamine self-administration, rats showed a state of studies on lisuride as a possible novel treatment during psychomotor retardation as measured by reduction of withdrawal from psychostimulant drugs in humans. locomotor activity and increased catalepsy. In search of a [Neuropsychopharmacology 8:213-218, 1993J f»SSible pharmacologic means of intervention for such

lEY WORDS: Amphetamine; self-administration; with intense craving for the drug: such symptomatology ;Animal models of depression; Lisuride; usuallyencompasses various phases of withdrawal and Psychostimulant withdrawal lasts for a few weeks (Gawin and Kleber, 1986). Epi­ sodes of craving for the abused drug are particularly Thegrowth of psychostimulantdrug addiction in North pronounced and are considered to be one of the major America has reached epidemic proportions over the motivating factors leading to relapse in the addictive pastfew years. The parallel increase in the number of process. It is also believed that withdrawal hospitaladmissions with such diagnoses has allowed may contribute, as a negative reinforcer, to drug­ nosologicalide ntifIcation and characterization of the seeking behavior. In accordance with this hypothesis, withdrawalsyndrome that follows abrupt discontinu­ it is possible that the neural substrates of the limbic fore­ ationo f the use of cocaine, amphetamine, or metham­ brain responsible for drug reward may also mediate, phetamine.The syndrome is mainly characterized by to some extent, withdrawal dysphoria (Pulvirentiet al. fatigue, depression of mood, anhedonia, psychomotor 1991). Finally, only tricyclic have, to retardation, hypersomnia, and hyperphagia together date, proven clinically efficacious in reducing with­ drawal depression, with , a dopamine

Fromthe Department of Neuropharmacology, The ScrippsResearch (DA) , showing some efficacyin reducing crav­ Institute, LaJolla, California. ing for cocaine (Dackis and Gold 1985a; Gawin and Kle­ Addressreprint requests to: Luigi Pulvirenti, M.D., Biochemical ber 1984; Tennant and Sagherian 1987). Psychopharmacology Unit, "e. Mondino" Foundation, Department ciNeurology,University ofPavia, ViaPaiestro 3, Pavia 27100,. Lisuride is a nonaddictive ergot derivative known ReceivedAugus t 2, 1991; revised July 9, 1992; accepted July 15, 1992. to stimulate postsynaptic DA receptors (Uzumaki et al.

e 1993American College of Neuropsychopharmacology Publishedby Elsevier Science Publishing Co., Inc. 6/i5Avenue of the Americas, New York, NY 10010 0893-133X/93/$6.00 214 L. Pulvirenti and G.F. Koob NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993- VOL. 8, NO.3

1982; Horowski and Wachtel 1976; Schechter 1984; attached to the polyethylene assembly mounted on the Rosenfeld and Makman 1981). In a recent study, it was animal's back immediately prior to the start of each ses­ suggested that lisuride may reduce intravenous cocaine sion. The cannula connector was removed following self-administration in rats (Pulvirenti and Koob submit­ the completion of a self-administration session and ted). This is in accordance with previous fIndings sug­ replaced with the guide cannula stylet. gesting a critical role for dopamine in the maintenance Four days following surgery, 14 rats were allowed of intravenous psychostimulant self-administration in 15-hour access every day (5:00 P.M. to 8:00 A.M.) for 10 various species (Markou and Koob 1992; Roberts and days to a metal lever mounted on the side wall of a stan­ Vickers 1984; Roberts et al. 1977, 1980). In addition, ex­ dard operant-conditioning cage. The cages themselves perimental and clinical studies have suggested that a were housed inside sound-attenuating chambers. A le­ state of DA depletion may occur during cocaine with­ ver press resulted in an intravenous injection of 0.1 ml drawal (Wyatt et al. 1988; Gawin and Kleber 1985; of d-amphetamine sulphate (0.12 mg/kg per injection, Dackis and Gold 1985b). A DA agonist would therefore or 0.325 J.1mollkg per injection) dissolved in 0.9% phys­ be expected to exert a normalizing action on the endog­ iologic saline and delivered over a period of 4 seconds. enous DA tone temporarily disrupted by repeated expo­ A swivel system allowed free movement of the animal sure to amphetamine. in the cage. Coincident with the onset of the injection, The aim of the present study was: 1) to quantify a stimulus light was illuminated for 20 seconds during the psychomotor retardation that follows withdrawal which time the lever pressing didnot result in reinforce­ from chronic intravenous self-administration of high ment. Lever presses during the period when the signal doses of amphetamine, and 2) to study the effect of light was not lit were reinforced on a continuous rein­ treatment with lisuride after drug discontinuation on forcement schedule. Control rats (n = 11) were injected such behavioral abnormalities. with an equal volume of passive intravenous physio­ The results presented here show that withdrawal logic saline infusion by an experimenter. from self-administration of high-dose intravenous am­ Following completion of the 10-day self-adminis­ phetamine induced reduction of spontaneous locomo­ tration period, spontaneous locomotor activity was tor activity and increased catalepsy, and the develop­ tested on days 1, 2, and 4 during withdrawal. Since loco­ ment of this syndrome was prevented by repeated motor testing was performed during the dark phase treatment with lisuride. (5:00 P.M. to 8:00 A.M.) and the last self-administration session ended at 8.00 A.M., day 1 of withdrawal actu­ ally corresponded to 33 hours after the end of intrave­ nous self-administration. Photobeam interruption was MATERIALS AND METHODS recorded for 180 minutes inphotocell cages as described Male Wistar rats (Charles River, Kingston, NY),weigh­ previously (Joyce and Koob 1981). ing between 200 and 225g at the start of the experiment, A separate group of 24 rats was used in the second were housed three to a cage and provided with ad part of the study. Sixteen rats were trained to self­ libitum access to food and water and maintained on administer amphetamine as described above; eight rats a 12-hour light/dark cycle (lights on from 4:00 A.M.- received experimentally administered saline as de­ 4:00 P.M.). scribed above. At the end of the 10-day self-admini­ Allanimals for self-administrationstudies were sur­ stration period, the amphetamine rats were treated with gically prepared under with a either lisuride (n = 8) (0.2 mg/kg IP, or 0.44 J.1mollkg) chronic silastic catheter implanted into the external or saline (n = 8) (1 ml/kg IP) twice daily at (8:00 A.M. jugular vein. The catheter/polyethylene assembly con­ and 8:00 P.M. for 4 days). sisted of silastic tubing attached to a guide cannula that For the study designed to test the effect of such was bent at a right angle. This junction was glued and repeated treatment with lisuride on spontaneous loco­ the guide cannula was embedded into a l-inch square motor activity in naive rats, four animals were injected of marlex mesh that was secured with silex. The cathe­ with one of four differentdoses of lisuride (0, 0.05, OJ, ter was passed subcutaneously from the rat's back to and 0.2 mg/kg IP, or 0, 0.11, 0.22, and 0.44 J.1mollkg, the jugular vein where it was implanted. The polyeth­ respectively, n = 16 rats) twice daily at 8:00 A.M. and ylene assembly was then mounted on the animal's back. 8:00 P.M. for 4 days. Animals were then tested as de­ A stylet was inserted into the guide cannula protrud­ scribed above. The injection schedule was chosen to ing from the animal's back to maintain a closed system avoid the peak effectof lisuride action during locomo­ and, therefore, prevent clogging of the cannula. tor testing. For self-administrationtesting, a cannula-connector Catalepsy testing was performed in the same rats assembly that was connected to a swivel and syringe during day 4 of withdrawal, at the end of the locomo­ pump as described by Roberts et al. (1977, 1980) was tor testing. Both forepaws of each rat were placed on KEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993 - VOL. 8, NO. 3 Lisuride and Amphetamine Withdrawal 215

12 4

10· '0CII 3 .e �a 8 CII 0 E � 6 i= 2 • >- �

0 0 0 2 4 6 8 10 Control Amphetamine Day of IV Self-Administration Figure 3. Catalepsy time during withdrawal from chronic flprel. Amphetamine intake during daily IS-hour N self­ amphetamine self-administration in the same animals repre­ lllministration sessions. Values represent mean ± SEM for sented in Figure 2. The forepaws of each rat were placed on hats. The average intake was 7.37 mg/kg. a bar 9 cm from the floor. Values represent mean ± SEM of time elapsed until each rat (control: n = 11; amphetamine: n = 14) repositioned both forepaws on the floor. 'p < 0.05. i bar 9 cm from the floor. The time elapsed until each lit repositioned both forepaws on the floor was re­ activity was signifIcantlylower in rats withdrawing from corded by an experimenter blind to the treatment. amphetamine self-administration, compared to control Resultswere compared using a two-way analysis animals(Fig. 2). Analysis of variance revealed that there livariancewith repeated measures, followed by post­ was a main effectof treatment (F = 9.49, dt = 1,23, p < hocNewman-Keul's test or a Student's t-test, where 0.01), a main effect of time (F = 14.49, dt = 2,23, p < 'PJll'opriate. 0.01), and no signifIcant treatment x time interaction The d-amphetamine sulphate was obtained from (F < 1, dt = 2,46, NS). Comparison at the various levels SIGMA(St. Louis, MO), lisuridehydrogen maleate was revealed that statistical signifIcancewas reached at day igenerous gift of Schering AG, (Berlin, ). All 2 (F = 9.41, dt = 1,23, p < 0.01) and day 4 (F = 5.79, drugs were dissolved in physiologic saline solution. dt = 1,23, p < 0.05). Similarly, amphetamine-treated rats showed increased catalepsy compared to control

animals. (Fig. 3), as measured by the bar test (t = - 2.21, RESULTS dt = 23, p < 0.05). listhat were allowed 15-hour daily access to intravenous The second part of the study was designed to as­ IIIphetamine self-administration showed a drug intake sess the effectof lisuride administered during the with­ rangingbetween 5.9 and 9.6 mg/kg, with an average drawal phase on the behavioral changes observed dur­ m7.37mg/kg (Fig. 1). Following cessation of this sched­ ing the fIrst part of the study. The control experiment aleof drugself-admini stration, spontaneous locomotor illustrated in Figure 4 shows that chronic (4-day) treat-

2� r---�------, • Conlroi • Conlrof fa lis 0.05 o Amphetamine 1500 Oils 0.1 1/1 lSI lis 0.2 1: i 1000 j , 0 0 0 1000 U m i 0 .. 500 � 500

0 0 2 4 2 4 Day of Treatment Day During Withdrawal

Rpre 2. Locomotor activity duringwithdrawal from chronic Figure 4. Effect of repeated treatment with lisuride (ex­ IIIIPhetaminese lf-administration. Values represent mean ± pressed in mg/kg, IP twice daily)on locomotor activity in na­ sat(co ntrol: n = 11; amphetamine: n = 14) of total pho­ ive rats. Values represent mean ± SEM (n = 4 per group). llbeaminterruption during 180 minutes. 'p < 0.05. For details on treatment schedule, see text. 216 L. Pulvirenti and G.F. Koob NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993-VOL. 8, NO.3

4 • Control * 2000 * 1:3 Amph. U o Amph. + liS. CII 3 II) * .!!!. * - 1500 CII c :J E 0 j:: () 2 >- 1000 U) iii 0. - CII 0 t- (;j ni 500 u

0 0 Control Amph. Amph. + lis. 1 2 4 Figure 6. Effect of lisuride on catalepsy time during with­ Day During Withdrawal drawal from chronic amphetamine self-administration. For Figure 5. Effect of repeated treatment with lisuride (0.2 details, see Figure 3. Number of subjects = 8 per group. ' mg/kg IP twice daily for 4 days) on locomotor activity during p < 0.05. withdrawal from chronic amphetamine self-administration.

Values represent mean ± SEM (n = 8 per group) of total pho­ tobeam interruption during 180 minutes. 'p < 0.05 vs. by the administration of tricyclic antidepressants (Ga­ control. win and Kleber 1984). When comparing nonhuman models of "depression" to the course of the affective disorder in humans, usually little or no parallelism can ment with lisuride per se, at the schedule employed, be drawn between the natural history preceding hu­ did not affect spontaneous locomotor activity. There man depression and the experimental procedure used was no main effectof drug(F < 1, dt = 3,12, NS), a main to induce the behavioral state of"depression" in animals effect of time (F = 25.75, dt = 2,12, P < 0.01), and no (Willner 1984). In contrast, the present study provides signifIcant drug x time interaction (F < I, dt = 6,24, an animal model of depression that is drug-induced, NS). In contrast, repeated treatment with lisuride (0.2 quantitatively measurable, and has, at the same time, mg/kg IP) during withdrawal (Fig. 5) completely construct and face validity (Willner 1984). reversed the reduction of locomotor activity induced Several studies over the past few years have ad­ by cessation of amphetamine availability (F = 10.47, dressed the issue of postamphetamine depression in dt = 2,91, P < 0.001). Comparison at the various levels animals. Withdrawal from a chronic schedule of experi· revealed that statistical signifIcancewas reached at day menter-administered amphetamine has been shown to

1(F = 3.447, dt = 2,28, P < 0.05), day 2 (F = 4.525, dt = induce a variety of behavioral changes including re­

2,28, p<0.05), and day 4(F = 3.87, dt = 2,28, p<0.05). duced rate of responding for electrical self-stimulation This was accompanied by reversal of the postamphet­ reward in rats (Kokkinidis et al. 1986) and elevation of amine increased catalepsy in the same rats (F = 4.01, its threshold(Cassens et al. 1981), depression of general dt = 2,21, p < 0.05) (Fig. 6). activity in rats and cats (Tonge 1974; Feinberg and Ir­ win 1961), diminished reaction to novel stimuli (Kok­ kinidis and Anisman 1978), and increased immobility DISCUSSION in a forced-swim situation (Kokkinidis et al. 1986). All of these manifestations are considered animal correlates Withdrawal from a chronic schedule of intravenous self­ of "depression," and the behavioral syndrome that fol­ administration of high dose amphetamine induced a lows withdrawal from amphetamineadministration has state of psychomotor retardation as measured by reduc­ been suggested as an experimental model of depres­ tion of spontaneous locomotor activity and increased sion (Leith and Barret 1980). Some of these changes are catalepsy. Treatment with lisuride after amphetamine also reversed by tricyclic antidepressants (Markou et suspension completely prevented the development of al. in press; Simpson 1974). such behavioral changes. It should be noted, however, that some endocrine, Abrupt cessation of chronic administration of psy­ immune, and neurochemical changes observed during chostimulant drugs in man is followed by a clinical syn­ amphetamine withdrawal in rats do not parallel changes drome that includes depression of mood, anhedonia, in the same systems observed during endogenous and disturbances of sleep(Gawin and Kleber 1986). Un­ depression in humans (Swerdlow et al. 1991). In the like other forms of depression, the occurrence of these present study, a self-administration rather than experi· symptoms is fairly predictable and limited in time but, menter-administered procedure was employed. Under similarly to endogenous depression, can be reversed these conditions the animals showed a voluntary in- NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993-VOL. 8, NO. 3 Lisuride and Amphetamine Withdrawal 217

take of high doses of amphetamine. It is noteworthy motivating factors leading to relapse in drug use, the thata recent report suggests that rats self-administering availability of effectiveand nonaddictive means of phar­ cocaine assume and tolerate doses that are toxic or le­ macologic intervention is desirable. Lisuride has been thalfor yoked control individuals (Dworkin et al. 1988); extensively used as a -lowering (Delitala et al. incontrast, no deaths were recorded during the pres­ 1979) and antiparkinsonian agent (Schachter et al. 1979). ent study. The use of intravenous self-administration Remarkably, abrupt cessation of the administration of procedures would therefore have both the advantage lisuride does not induce withdrawal signs, drug crav­ of the self-titration of drug intake by each individual ing, or dysphoria in patients even after administration animaland a more "natural" temporal pattern of self­ at high doses through continuous subcutaneous infu­ injections (as compared to experimenter-administered sion (Bittkau and pzuntek 1988). Similarly, no reports procedures) and these may possibly be more closely of nonmedical use of lisuride are known (R. Horowski, relatedto the pattern of drug intake in human addicts. personal communication). It is therefore possible to Themajor fmdingof the present report is the effect speculate that lisuride may, through stimulation of post­ oflisurideon the behavioral changes described above. synaptic DA receptors, normalize the DA tone tem­ Usuride isan ergot derivative known to stimulate brain porarily disrupted by repeated exposure to high doses DA receptors of the D2 type (Uzumaki et al. 1982; of amphetamine. Horowskiand Wachtel 1976; Schechter 1984; Cunning­ In conclusion, the relative safety of lisuride and the ham et al. 1987; Rosenfeld and Makman 1981). Lisu­ experimental results of the present and previous studies ridealso seems to affect (5-HT) neurotrans­ (Pulvirenti and Koob submitted) may encourage clini­ mission(Fink and Morgenstern 1985;Cunningham and cal trials to assess potential efficacyof the drug in psy­ lekowski1990). Indeed, there appears to be a serotoner­ chostimulant addiction as a mood-stabilizing and "an­ giccomponent of its locomotor activity effects (Fink and ticraving" agent during withdrawal. Morgenstern 1985) and lisuride elicits the "" characteristic of 5-HT (Sielbergeld andHruska 1979). Furthermore, a signifIcant propor­ ACKNOWLEDGMENTS tionof humans maintained on lisuride therapy report The authors wish to thank Dr. R. Horowski for the generous , which are thought to be mediated by gift of lisuride and Dr. Neal R. Swerdlow for helpful discus­ 5oHT2 receptors (MacDonald and Horowski 1983). In sion. This work was partially supported by NlDA Grants DA addition, interference with 5-HT has 04398. This is publication number NP-6956 of The Scripps Re­ signifIcant effects on the self-administration of am­ search Institute. phetamine and cocaine (Carroll et al. 1990, Loh and Roberts1990). It is therefore possible that this interac­ tionof lisuride with the 5-HT system is of relevance for REFERENCES the effects of reported treatment with lisuride also

reported here. Bittkau S, pzuntek H (1988): Chronic subcutaneous lisuride In a recent report, lisuride reduced cocaine self­ in Parkinson's disease-Motor performance and avoid­ administration (Pulvirenti and Koob submitted). This ance of psychiatric side effects.J Neural Trans 27S:35-54 supportsthe hypothesis that amphetamine, and other Carroll ME, Leo ST, Asencio H, Kragh R (1990): psychostimu lant drugs, produce their rewarding action reduces intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. byactivating the forebrain DA system (for review, see Pharmacol Biochem Behav 35:237-244 Pulvirenti and Koob 1990). The rationale for using lisu­ CassensG, Actor C, Kling M, Schildkraut JJ (1981): Ampheta­ rideduring postamphetaminewithdrawal, in contrast, mine withdrawal: Effects on threshold of intracranialrein­ forcement. Psychopharmacology 33:318-322 stems from the evidence that a state of DA depletion may occur during the withdrawal phase, as shown by Cunningham KA, LekowskiJM (1990): The interaction of co­ caine with serotonin dorsal raphe neurons. Neuropsy­ experimental and clinical fmdings (Wyatt et al. 1988; chopharmacology 3:41-50 Gawinand Kleber, 1985; Dackis and Gold, 1985b). Ac­ Cunningham KA, Callahan PM, Appel JB (1987): Discrimina­ cordingly, bromocriptine, a DA receptor agonist, was tive stimulus properties of lisuride revisited: Involvement shown to decrease cocaine self-administration (Hub­ of dopamine D2 receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 241: ner andKoob 1990) and to reverse behavioral signs of 147-151 cocaine withdrawal in rats (Markou and Koob 1992). Dackis CA, Gold MS (1985a): Bromocriptine as treatment for Furthermore, clinical studies suggest that bromocrip­ cocaine abuse. Lancet 1:1151

tine showed some benefIcial effect during cocaine Dackis CA, Gold MS (1985b): New concepts in cocaine ad­ withdrawalin humans (Dackis and Gold 1985a). Since diction: The dopamine depletion hypothesis. Neurosci dysphoria and drug craving are particularly intense Biobehav Rev 9:469-477 during the initial phase of withdrawal from psycho­ DelitalaG, Wass JAH, StubbsWA,Jones A, Wiliams S, Besser stimulantdrugs, and these are considered important GM (1979): The effect of lisuride hydrogen maleate, an 218 L. Pulvirenti and G.F. Koob NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993-VOL. 8, NO.3

ergot derivative on anterior pituitary hormone secretion ing properties of psychostimulant drugs. In Willner p, in man. Clin Endocrinol 11:1-9 Scheel-Kruger J (eds), The DoparrtineSystem: FromMoti­ vation to Action. London, John Wiley and Sons, pp Dworkin SM, Volkmer C, Dworkin SI (1988): Toxic conse­ 131-140 quences of cocaineare augmented by noncontingent drug administration. Soc Neurosci Abstr 14:961 Pulvirenti L, KoobGF: Lisuride red:.tref mtravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. (Sub.rutted) Feinberg G, Irwin S (1961): Effect of chronic methampheta­ mine administration in the cat. Fed Proc (Abstr) 20:396. Roberts DCS, Vickers G (1984):Atypical neurolepticsincrease self-administration of cocaine: An evaluation of a be­ Fink H, Morgenstern R (1985): Locomotor effect of lisuride: havioral screen for activity. Psychophar· A consequence of doparrtinergic and actions. macology 82:135-139 Psychopharmacology 85:464-468 Roberts DCS, Corcoran ME, Fibiger HC (1977): On therole Gawin FH, KleberHD (1984): Cocaine abuse treatment: Open of ascending systems in intravenous pilot trial with and lithium carbonate. Arch self-administration of cocaine. Pharmacol Biochem Be­ Gen Psychiatry 41:903-909 hav 6:615-620 Gawin FH, Kleber HD (1985): Neuroendocrine fIndings in Roberts DCS, KoobGF, Klonoff P, Fibiger HC (1980):Extinc­ chronic cocaine abusers: A preliminary report. Br J Psy­ tion and recovery of cocaine self-administration foUow­ chiatry 147:569-573 ing 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accum­ Gawin FH, Kleber HD (1986): Abstinence symptomatology bens. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 12:781-787 and psychiatric diagnosis in cocaine abusers. Arch Gen Rosenfeld MR, Makman MH (1981): The interaction of lisu­ Psychiatry 43:103-107 ride, an ergot derivative, with serotonergic and dopa­ Horowski R, Wachtel H (1976): Direct action minergic receptors in rabbit brain. J Pharmacol Exp Ther of lisuride hydrogen-maleate, an ergot derivative, in mice. 216:526-531 Eur J PharmacoI36:373-383 Schacter M, BlackstockJ, Dick JPR,George RID, Marsden CD, Hubner CB, Koob GF (1990): Bromocriptine produces de­ Parkes ID (1979): Lisuride in Parkinson's disease. lan­ creases in cocaine self-administration in the rat. Neuro­ cet 2:1129 psychopharmacology 3: 101-108 Schechter MD(1984): Evidencefor a directdopam inergiceffect Joyce EM, Koob GF (1981): Amphetamine-, scopolarrtine- and of lisuride. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 21:185-189 -induced locomotor activityfollowing 6-hydroxy­ Sielbergeld EK, Hruska RE (1979): Lisuride and LSD dopa­ dopamine lesions of the mesolimbic dopamine system. minergic and serotoninergic interactions in the "seroto­ Psychopharmacology 73:311-313 nin syndrome." Psychopharmacology 65:233-237 Kokkinidis L, Anisman H (1978): Involvement of norepineph­ Simpson D (1974): Depressed rates of self-stirnulationfoUow­ rine in startle arousal after acute and chronic d-amphet­ ing chronic amphetamine in the rat: Evaluation of these amine administration. Psychopharmacology 59:285-292 depressed rates by desmethylimiprarrtine. Presented to Kokkinidis L, Zacharko RM, Anisman H (1986): Amphet­ the EasternPsychological Association.Washi ngton D.C, amine withdrawal: A behavioral evaluation. Life Sci April 19, 1974 38:1617-1623 Swerdlow NR,Hauger R, Irwin M, KoobGF, Britton KT, Pul­ Leith NJ, Barret RJ (1980): Effects of chronic amphetamine or virenti L (1991):Endocrine, immune and neurochemical reserpine on self-stimulation responding: Animal model changes in rats during withdrawal from chronic ampheta­ of depression? Psychopharmacology 72:9-15 mine intoxication. Neuropsychopharmacology 5:23-31

Loh EA, Roberts DC (1990): Break-points on a progressive Tennant FS, Sagherian AA(1987): Double-blind comparison ratio schedule reinforced by intravenous cocaineincrease of and bromocriptine for ambulatorywith­ following depletion of forebrain serotonin. Psychophar­ drawal from cocaine dependence_ Arch Intern Med macology 101:262-267 147:109-112

Markou A, Koob GF (1992): Bromocriptine reverses post­ Tonge SR (1974): Noradrenaline and 5-hydroxytryptamint cocaine anhedonia in a rat model of cocaine withdrawal. metabolism in six areas of the rat brain during postam­ Neuropsychopharmacology 7:213-224 phetarrtinedepression. Psychopharmacology 38:181-186

Markou A, Hauger RL, Koob GF: Desmethylirnipramine at­ Uzumaki H, Govoni S, Memo M, Carruba MO, Trabucchi tenuates cocaine withdrawal in rats. Psychopharmacol­ M, Spano PF (1982): Effectof GTP and sodium on ratstri­ ogy (In press) atal dopaminergic receptors labeled with lisuride. Brain McDonald RJ, Horowski R (1983): Lisuride in the treatment Res 248:185-187 of . Eur Neurol 22:240-255 Willner P (1984): The validity of animal models of depression. Pulvirenti L, Koob GF (1990): The neural substrates of drug Psychopharmacology 83:1-16 addiction and dependence. Funct NeuroI5:109-119 Wyatt RJ, Karoum F, Suddath R, Fawcett R (1988): Persis­ Pulvirenti L, Swerdlow NR, Hubner CB, Koob GF (1991):The tently decreased brain dopamine levels and cocaine. role of limbic-accumbens-pallidal circuitry in the activat- JAMA 259:2996