[This Letter Is Closed Now, Accepting No More Signatures]

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

[This Letter Is Closed Now, Accepting No More Signatures] March 31st, 2017 To: H.E. Réka Szemerkényi, Ambassador of Hungary to the United States of America Zoltán Balog, Minister of Human Capacities, Ministry of Human Capacities, Hungary László Palkovics, Minister of State for Education, Ministry of Human Capacities, Hungary Dear Ambassador Szemerkényi, Minister Balog, and Minister Palkovics, We are writing to express our dismay about the proposed legislation that would effectively end a 25-year history of scientific excellence in Budapest. As an international body of psychologists, neuroscientists, and cognitive scientists, we can tell you that our colleagues at Central European University are among the most respected in the world. Their intellectual legacy has had a global impact. We believe any city should count itself fortunate to have such a renowned center of academic excellence in its midst. The proposed legislation will make it effectively impossible for CEU to continue to occupy its current position as one of the foremost scientific institutions internationally. We respectfully ask that you preserve CEU’s ability to act as a center of leadership and innovation in Hungary and the world by withdrawing this legislation. [This letter is closed now, accepting no more signatures] Sincerely, 1. Laura Schulz, Professor of Cognitive Science, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT 2. Rebecca Saxe, Professor of Cognitive Science, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT 3. John E. Richards, Carolina Distinguished Professor, Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina. 4. Michael Tomasello, Duke University, Durham, NC, and Max Planck for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany. 5. Philippe G. Schyns, Professor of Visual Cognition, Director of the Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, university of Glasgow, UK 6. Janet F. Werker, Professor, Canada Research Chair, and University Killam Professor, The University of British Columbia, CANADA 7. Charles A. Nelson, Professor of Pediatrics,Neuroscience and Education, Harvard University 8. Joshua B. Tenenbaum, Professor of Computational Cognitive Science, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT 9. Andrew N. Meltzoff, Professor of Psychology, Co-Director of the Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA 10. Tania Singer, Director of the Department for Social Neuroscience, Max Planck Institut for Human Cognitive and Brain Science, Leipzig, Germany. 11. Mary C. Potter, Professor of Psychology Emerita. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT. 12. Tamsin C. German, Professor of Psychology, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara. 13. Alison Gopnik, Professor of Psychology, Dept. of Psychology, University of California at Berkeley 14. Vittorio Gallese, Professor of Physiology, Dept. of Medicine & Surgery - Unit of Neuroscience, University of Parma, Italy; Professor in Experimental Aesthetics, Institute of Philosophy, School of Advanced Study, University of London, UK. 15. Paul Bloom, Brooks and Suzanne Ragen Professor of Psychology, Yale University 16. Nancy Kanwisher, Walter A. Rosenblith Professor of Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, MIT; Member, National Academy of Sciences, USA 17. Miguel P. Eckstein, Professor, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara. 18. Masaki Tomonaga, Professor, PhD, Language and Intelligence Section, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi, JAPAN 19. Susan Gelman, Heinz Werner Distinguished University Professor of Psychology and Linguistics, University of Michigan; Member, National Academy of Sciences, USA 20. Michael I. Jordan, Distinguished Professor of Statistics and Electrical Engineering and Computer Sciences, University of California, Berkeley, Member of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, Member of the National Academy of Engineering, USA 21. Marjorie Rhodes, Associate Professor of Psychology, New York University 22. Athena Vouloumanos, Associate Professor of Psychology, New York University. 23. Catherine Hartley, Assistant Professor of Psychology, New York University. 24. Patrick Shafto, Henry Rutgers Term Chair in Data Science and Associate Professor of Mathematics and Computer Science, Rutgers University--Newark 25. Elizabeth Bonawitz, Assistant Professor of Psychology, Rutgers University--Newark 26. Koleen McCrink, Assistant Professor of Psychology, Barnard College- Columbia University 27. Lisa Feigenson, Professor of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University 28. Justin Halberda, Professor of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University 29. Edward Gibson, Professor of Cognitive Science, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology 30. William Graves, Assistant Professor of Psychology, Rutgers University -- Newark 31. David Sobel, Professor of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Brown University 32. Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Lefkowitz Faculty Fellow and Professor of Psychology, Temple University 33. Jesse Snedeker, Professor of Psychology, Harvard University 34. Hyowon Gweon, Assistant Professor of Psychology, Stanford University 35. Cristine Legare, Associate Professor of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin 36. Renée Baillargeon, Alumni Distinguished Professor of Psychology, University of Illinois 37. Steven Pinker, Johnstone Professor of Psychology, Harvard University, and Member, National Academy of Sciences, USA 38. Yusuke Moriguchi, Associate Professor, Department of Education, Kyoto University 39. Bernhard Hommel, Professor of General Psychology, Leiden University 40. Victoria Southgate, Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen 41. Leda Cosmides, Distinguished Professor of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara 42. Raffaella I. Rumiati, Professor of Cognitive Neuroscience, SISSA, Trieste Italy 43. Kazuhide Hashiya, PhD. Associate Professor, Faculty of Human Environment Studies, Kyushu University, Japan 44. Hiromi Kobayashi, PhD. Research Fellow, Faculty of Human Environment Studies, Kyushu University, Japan 45. Emmanuel Dupoux, Professor of Cognitive Science, Ecole des Hautes Etudes en Sciences Sociales, Ecole Normale Supérieure, CNRS, Paris. 46. Daniel Wolpert FRS FMedSci, Royal Society Noreen Murray Professor in Neurobiology and Professor of Engineering, University of Cambridge, UK 47. Albert Costa. ICREA Research Professor. University Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain. 48. Anne Christophe, CNRS Research Director, Ecole normale supérieure, Paris. 49. Christian Lorenzi, Professor of Experimental Psychology, Ecole normale supérieure, Paris. 50. Deborah Kelemen, Professor of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, USA 51. John Tooby, Distinguished Professor of Anthropology & Psychological & Brain Sciences, Co-Director, Center for Evolutionary Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, USA 52. Jean-Rémy Hochmann, CNRS Researcher, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, Lyon, France 53. Corrado Sinigaglia, Professor of Philosophy of Science, Department of Philosophy, University of Milan, Italy 54. Kazuo Hiraki, Professor, Department of General Systems Studies, University of Tokyo, Japan 55. Luca L. Bonatti, ICREA Professor, Center for Brain and Cognition, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain 56. Judit Gervain, CNRS Researcher, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Paris, France 57. Stefano Palminteri, Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives, École Normale Supérieure, Paris, France 58. Ian Apperly, Professor of Cognition and Development, University of Birmingham, UK. 59. Giorgio Vallortigara, Professor of Neuroscience, University of Trento, Italy 60. Masako Myowa-Yamakoshi, Professor, Graduate School of Education, Kyoto University, Japan. 61. Bruce Hood, Professor of Developmental Psychology in Society, University of Bristol, UK. 62. Rita Astuti, Professor of Anthropology, London School of Economics, UK 63. Rachael Jack, Lecturer in Psychology, Institute of Neuroscience & Psychology, University of Glasgow, UK 64. Atsushi Senju, Reader in Social Neuroscience, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, UK 65. Giacomo Rizzolatti, Prof.Human Physiology, University of Parma, Parma Italy 66. Daniel C. Richardson, Reader, Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, UK. 67. Tony Charman, Chair in Clinical Child Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, UK 68. Olivier Mascaro, CNRS Researcher, Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, Lyon, France 69. Pierre Jacob, Senior Researcher, CNRS, Institut Jean Nicod, Ecole Normale Supérieure, Paris, France. 70. Olivier Morin, research group leader, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany. 71. Willem Frankenhuis, Assistant Professor of Developmental Psychology, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands 72. Gustaf Gredebäck, Professor of Developmental Psychology, Uppsala University, Sweden 73. Tim Shallice FRS, Emeritus Professor, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, UK 74. Laura Visu-Petra, Associate Professor, Developmental Psychology Lab, Babeș-Bolyai University, Romania 75. Véronique Izard, CNRS Researcher, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France. 76. Iroise Dumontheil, Reader in Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck,
Recommended publications
  • Tracing Autism’ Ambiguity and Difference in a Neuroscientific Research Practice
    The London School of Economics and Political Science ‘Tracing Autism’ Ambiguity and difference in a neuroscientific research practice Patrick D. Fitzgerald A thesis submitted to the Department of Sociology of the London School of Economics for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy London, September 2012 1 Declaration I certify that the thesis I have presented for examination for the PhD degree of the London School of Economics and Political Science is solely my own work other than where I have clearly indicated that it is the work of others (in which case the extent of any work carried out jointly by me and any other person is clearly identified in it). The copyright of this thesis rests with the author. Quotation from it is permitted, provided that full acknowledgement is made. This thesis may not be reproduced without my prior written consent. I warrant that this authorisation does not, to the best of my belief, infringe the rights of any third party. I declare that my thesis consists of 88,543 words. I can confirm that portions of this thesis were copy-edited for conventions of language, spelling and grammar by John MacArtney, Megan Clinch, Joanne Kalogeras, Juljan Krause and Neasa Terry. ________________________________________________________ Des Fitzgerald, September 2012 2 Abstract Tracing Autism is about neuroscientists’ on-going search for a brain-based biomarker for autism. While much recent sociological work has looked at the ‘cerebralization’ of such diverse diagnostic categories as depression, bipolar disorder, psychopathy, addiction, and even autism itself, surprisingly little light has yet been shed on the mundane ways that researchers in the new brain sciences actually think about, reason through, and hold together neurological accounts of complex and emerging diagnostic entities .
    [Show full text]
  • June, 2009 VITA Thomas G. Bever Education
    June, 2009 VITA Thomas G. Bever Education Harvard College - A.B., 1961 Massachusetts Institute of Technology - Ph.D., 1967 Honors and Awards Phi Beta Kappa - Harvard University - 1961 "Magna cum laude with highest honors in Linguistics and Psychology"- Harvard College - 1961 NIH Predoctoral Fellowship - 1962-1964 Elected to Harvard Society of Fellows - 1964-1967 NSF Faculty Fellowship - 1974-1977 (Summers) Guggenheim Fellowship - 1976/77 Fellow, Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences - 1984/85 The Foreign Language Teaching Research Article Award – 2004 – Society for Foreign Language Teaching in China. (Given every 2 years). The Compassionate Friends Award – 2005- “Compassionate employer of the year” Teaching Experience Lecturer, M.I.T., Psychology Department, 1964-1966 Assistant Professor, The Rockefeller University, 1967-1969 Associate Professor, The Rockefeller University, 1969-1970 Professor of Linguistics and Psychology, Columbia University, 1970-1986 Pulse Professor of Psychology, University of Rochester, 1985-1995 Professor of Linguistics, University of Rochester, 1985-1995 Research Professor of Cognitive Science, Linguistics, Neuroscience, Psychology, Language Reading and Culture. University of Arizona, 1995 - present Visiting Professor, USC, Spring 2005 Visiting Professor, University of Leipzig, Fall 2005 Visiting Professor, University of California, Irvine, Spring 2006 Administrative-Academic Activities Vice President, The Rockefeller University Chapter of American Association of University Professors, 1969-1970
    [Show full text]
  • 1 CURRICULUM VITAE Daniel Mark Wolpert Fmedsci FRS Royal
    CURRICULUM VITAE Daniel Mark Wolpert FMedSci FRS Royal Society Noreen Murray Research Professor & Professor of Engineering (1875) Department of Engineering Tel: +44 (0)1223 748530 University of Cambridge Fax: +44 (0)1223 332662 Trumpington Street Email [email protected] Cambridge CB2 1PZ Homepage www.wolpertlab.com Education/Qualifications 1985 Medical Sciences BA University of Cambridge 1988 Clinical Medicine BM BCh University of Oxford 1992 Physiology D. Phil University of Oxford Professional History 1988-89 Medical House Officer, Oxford 1989-92 Medical Research Council Training Fellow University Laboratory of Physiology, University of Oxford (Supervisor: John Stein/Chris Miall) 1992-94 Postdoctoral Associate, Department of Brain & Cognitive Science Massachusetts Institute of Technology (Supervisor: Michael Jordan) 1994-95 McDonnell-Pew Fellow in Cognitive Neuroscience, Brain & Cognitive Sciences Massachusetts Institute of Technology (Supervisor: Michael Jordan) 1995-99 Lecturer in Neurophysiology, Institute of Neurology University College London 1999-02 Reader in Motor Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, University College London 1999-05 Co-director, Institute of Movement Neuroscience 2002-05 Professor of Motor Neuroscience Vice-Chair, Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience & Movement Disorders, Institute of Neurology, University College London 2005-08 Honorary Senior Research Fellow, UCL 2005- Professor of Engineering (1875), Department of Engineering, University of Cambridge 2005- Professorial Fellow, Trinity College, University
    [Show full text]
  • Outline of Michael Tomasello, a Natural History of Human Morality (Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press, 2016)
    Outline of Michael Tomasello, A Natural History of Human Morality (Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press, 2016). John Protevi LSU HNRS 2030.2: “Evolution and Biology of Morality” I Chapter 1: The Interdependence Hypothesis A) Parallels of natural and moral cooperation 1) Natural cooperation (a) Altruistic helping (b) Mutualist collaboration 2) Human morality (a) Parallel human morality types: (i) Altruistic helping via compassion, concern, benevolence: ethic of the good / sympathy (ii) Mutualist collaboration via fairness: ethic of right / justice (b) Simplicity vs complexity (i) Sympathetic altruism is simpler and more basic: (i) Pure cooperation (ii) Proximate mechanisms: based in mammalian parental care / kin selection (ii) Fair collaboration is more complex: interactions of multiple individuals w/ different interests (i) “cooperativization of competition” (ii) Proximate mechanisms: moral emotions / judgments 1. Deservingness 2. Punishment: feelings of resentment / indignation toWard Wrong-doers 3. Accountability: judgments of responsibility, obligation, etc B) Goal of the book: evolutionary account of emergence of human morality of sympathy and fairness 1) Morality is “form of cooperation” (a) Emerging via human adaptation to neW social forms required by neW ecological / economic needs (b) Bringing With it “species-unique proximate mechanisms” or psychological processes (i) cognition (ii) social interaction (iii) self-regulation 2) Based on these assumptions, tWo goals: (a) Specify hoW human cooperation differs from other primates
    [Show full text]
  • Know Thy Selves: Learning to Understand Oneself Increases the Ability to Understand Others
    JCognEnhanc DOI 10.1007/s41465-017-0023-6 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Know Thy Selves: Learning to Understand Oneself Increases the Ability to Understand Others Anne Böckler1,2 & Lukas Herrmann 1 & Fynn-Mathis Trautwein1 & Tom Holmes3 & Tania Singer1 Received: 7 December 2016 /Accepted: 10 April 2017 # The Author(s) 2017. This article is an open access publication Abstract Understanding others’ feelings, intentions, and be- of identified parts that were negatively valenced showed a liefs is a crucial social skill both for our personal lives and for strong relation with enhanced ToM capacities. This finding meeting the challenges of a globalized world. Recent evidence suggests a close link between getting better in understanding suggests that the ability to represent and infer others’ mental oneself and improvement in social intelligence. states (Theory of Mind, ToM) can be enhanced by mental training in healthy adults. The present study investigated the Keywords Internal Family System . Theory of Mind . Self . role of training-induced understanding of oneself for the en- Inner parts . Contemplative mental training hanced understanding of others. In a large-scale longitudinal study, two independent participant samples (N =80and N = 81) received a 3-month contemplative training. This train- Introduction ing focused on perspective taking and was inspired by the Internal Family Systems model that conceives the self as be- As citizens of the twenty-first century, we face many problems ing composed of a complex system of inner personality as- of an industrialized and globalized world. Tensions between pects. Specifically, participants practiced perspective taking countries, cultures, and religions are rising; wars and political on their own inner states by learning to identify and classify instabilities drive millions of people to leave their homes and different inner personality parts.
    [Show full text]
  • 2010 Program Meeting Schedule
    Vision Sciences Society 10th Annual Meeting, May 7-12, 2010 Naples Grande Resort & Club, Naples, Florida Program Contents Board, Review Committee & Staff . 2 Satellite Events. 22 Keynote Address . 3 Club Vision Dance Party. 22 Meeting Schedule . 4 Open House . 23 Schedule-at-a-Glance . 6 Member-Initiated Symposia . 24 Poster Schedule . 8 Friday Sessions . 29 Talk Schedule . 10 Saturday Sessions . 33 Young Investigator Award . 11 Sunday Sessions . 43 Abstract Numbering System. 11 Monday Sessions . 53 VSS Dinner and Demo Night . 12 Tuesday Sessions . 58 VSS Public Lecture . 15 Wednesday Sessions . 68 VSS at ARVO . 15 Topic Index. 71 Attendee Resources . 16 Author Index . 74 Exhibitors . 19 Hotel Floorplan . 86 Travel Awards . 21 Advertisements. 89 Board, Review Committee & Staff Board of Directors Abstract Review Committee Tony Movshon (2011), President David Alais Laurence Maloney New York University Marty Banks Ennio Mingolla Pascal Mamassian (2012), President Elect Irving Biederman Cathleen Moore CNRS & Université Paris 5 Geoff Boynton Shin’ya Nishida Eli Brenner Tony Norcia Bill Geisler (2010), Past President Angela Brown Aude Oliva University of Texas, Austin David Burr Alice O’Toole Marisa Carrasco (2012), Treasurer Patrick Cavanagh John Reynolds New York University Marvin Chun Anna Roe Barbara Dosher (2013) Jody Culham Brian Rogers University of California, Irvine Greg DeAngelis Jeff Schall James Elder Brian Scholl Karl Gegenfurtner (2013) Steve Engel David Sheinberg Justus-Liebig Universität Giessen, Germany Jim Enns Daniel Simons
    [Show full text]
  • Empathy and Altruism. the Hypothesis of Somasia
    A Clinical Perspective on “Theory of Mind”, Empathy and Altruism The Hypothesis of Somasia Jean-Michel Le Bot PhD, University Rennes 2, Rennes doi: 10.7358/rela-2014-001-lebo [email protected] ABSTRACT The article starts by recalling the results of recent experiments that have revealed that, to a certain extent, the “ability to simultaneously distinguish between different possible perspec- tives on the same situation” (Decety and Lamm 2007) exists in chimpanzees. It then describes a case study of spatial and temporal disorientation in a young man following a cerebral lesion in order to introduce the hypothesis that this ability is based on a specific process of somasia. By permitting self-other awareness, this process also provides subjects with anchor points in time and space from which they can perform the mental decentring that enables them to adopt various perspectives. This process seems to be shared by humans and certain animal species and appears to be subdivided into the processing of the identity of experienced situa- tions, on the one hand, and of their unity on the other. The article concludes with a critique of overly reflexive and “representational” conceptions of theory of mind which do not distin- guish adequately between the ability to “theorise” about the mental states of others and the self-other awareness ability (which is automatic and non-reflexive). Keywords: Social cognition, theory of mind, empathy, altruism, neuropsychology, somasia, humans, animals, mental states, decentring. 1. INTRODUCTION In their seminal paper published in 1978, Premack and Woodruff stated that “an individual has a theory of mind [ToM] if he imputes mental states to himself and others” (Premack and Woodruff 1978).
    [Show full text]
  • Brain, Body and Culture: a Biocultural Theory of Religion1
    METHOD & THEORY in the STUDY OF RELIGION Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 22 (2010) 304-321 brill.nl/mtsr Brain, Body and Culture: A Biocultural Theory of Religion1 Armin W. Geertz Religion, Cognition and Culture Research Unit (RCC), Department of the Study of Religion, Aarhus University, Denmark [email protected] Abstract This essay sketches out a biocultural theory of religion which is based on an expanded view of cognition that is anchored in brain and body (embrained and embodied), deeply dependent on culture (enculturated) and extended and distributed beyond the borders of individual brains. Such an approach uniquely accommodates contemporary cultural and neurobiological sciences. Since the challenge that the study of religion faces, in my opinion, is at the interstices of these sciences, I have tried to develop a theory of religion which acknowledges the fact. My hope is that the theory can be of use to scholars of religion and be submitted to further hypotheses and tests by cognitive scientists. Keywords biocultural theory, embrainment, embodiment, enculturation, extended mind, distributed cog- nition, neuroscience, religion Introduction At the Religion, Cognition and Culture Research Unit (RCC) in Aarhus, our central axiom is that cognition is not just what goes on in the individual mind. In adapting our approach to contemporary research in neurobiology, archaeol- ogy, anthropology, comparative religion and philosophy of science, we hold that cognition is embrained, embodied, encultured, extended and distributed.2 1 My warmest thanks are extended to Michael Stausberg, Jesper Sørensen, Jeppe Sinding Jensen and Aaron Hughes for comments and critiques of earlier drafts of this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • How Babies Think
    osq216Gpnk3p.indd 4 3/9/16 5:37 PM RAISE GREAT Thirty years ago most psychologists, KIDS philosophers and psychiatrists thought that babies and young children were ir- rational, egocentric and amoral. They believed children were locked in the con- crete here and now—unable to under- Even the stand cause and effect, imagine the ex - periences of other people, or appreciate youngest the difference between reality and fanta- sy. People still often think of children as children know, defective adults. experience and But in the past three decades scien- tists have discovered that even the young- learn far more est children know more than we would ever have thought possible. Moreover, than scientists studies suggest that children learn about the world in much the same way that sci- Photographs by ever thought entists do—by conducting experiments, Timothy analyzing statistics, and forming intuitive Archibald possible theories of the physical, biological and psychological realms. Since about 2000, researchers have started to understand the underlying computational, evolutionary and neurological mechanisms that under- pin these remarkable early abilities. These revolutionary ndings not only change our ideas about babies, they give us a fresh perspective on human nature itself. PHYSICS FOR BABIES Why were we so wrong about babies for so long? If you look cursorily at children who are four years old and younger (the age range I will discuss in this article), you might indeed conclude that not much is going on. Babies, after all, cannot talk. And even preschoolers are not good at re- porting what they think. Ask your aver- age three-year-old an open-ended ques- tion, and you are likely to get a beautiful but incomprehensible stream-of-con- sciousness monologue.
    [Show full text]
  • Compassion Or Empathy? a Way Forward to Reduce GP Stress and Burnout
    Out of Hours Compassion or empathy? A way forward to reduce GP stress and burnout I have often heard from colleagues and ADDREss FOR CORREsPONDENCE felt myself the sense of psychological Manohar Thakur exhaustion when dealing with the Derby Open Access Centre, 207 St Thomas Road, particularly stressful life problems of our Normanton, Derby DE3 9BL, UK. patients. This is part of the ‘burnout’ so E-mail: [email protected] often talked about in general practice. It is common experience that listening to the patient with empathy goes a long way In the light of this new research showing towards helping many of them: being the that compassion is a virtue that can be ‘drug doctor’ of Balint. Empathy, according passed on by training, it may be worth to the Oxford English Dictionary means: thinking of ways to train ourselves in techniques such as meditation, which will ‘... the ability to understand and share the Photo: The Mind and Life Institute. help not only our patients but ourselves feelings of another.’ as well. The Royal College of General Practitioners could play a guiding role in While this is obviously good for the to have been able to make a difference to exploring this dimension of GP training patient, the person at the giving end of this their lives and, rather than stressing me at the undergraduate, postgraduate, empathy can find themselves emotionally out, these formerly difficult patients have and professional levels. The Mind and drained and fatigued. This can be repeated become a source of professional, as well as Life Institute (http://www.mindandlife.
    [Show full text]
  • Children's Causal Inferences from Indirect Evidence
    Running head: CHILDREN’S CAUSAL INFERENCES Children’s causal inferences from indirect evidence: Backwards blocking and Bayesian reasoning in preschoolers David M. Sobel Department of Cognitive and Linguistic Sciences, Brown University Joshua B. Tenenbaum Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT Alison Gopnik Department of Psychology, University of California at Berkeley Cognitive Science in press. Children’s causal 2 Abstract Previous research suggests that children can infer causal relations from patterns of events. However, what appear to be cases of causal inference may simply reduce to children recognizing relevant associations among events, and responding based on those associations. To examine this claim, in Experiments 1 and 2, children were introduced to a “blicket detector”, a machine that lit up and played music when certain objects were placed upon it. Children observed patterns of contingency between objects and the machine’s activation that required them to use indirect evidence to make causal inferences. Critically, associative models either made no predictions, or made incorrect predictions about these inferences. In general, children were able to make these inferences, but some developmental differences between 3- and 4- year-olds were found. We suggest that children’s causal inferences are not based on recognizing associations, but rather that children develop a mechanism for Bayesian structure learning. Experiment 3 explicitly tests a prediction of this account. Children were asked to make an inference about ambiguous data based on the base-rate of certain events occurring. Four- year-olds, but not 3-year-olds were able to make this inference. Children’s causal 3 Children’s causal inferences from indirect evidence: Backwards blocking and Bayesian reasoning in preschoolers As adults, we know a remarkable amount about the causal structure of the world.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Morality a Gadget? Nature, Nurture and Culture in Moral Development
    Author Accepted Manuscript. To appear in a special issue of Synthese on The Cultural Evolution of Human Social Cognition, edited by Richard Moore, Rachael Brown & Cecilia Heyes. 24 July 2019 IS MORALITY A GADGET? NATURE, NURTURE AND CULTURE IN MORAL DEVELOPMENT Cecilia Heyes All Souls College & Department of Experimental Psychology University of Oxford Oxford OX1 4AL United Kingdom [email protected] 1 Abstract Research on ‘moral learning’ examines the roles of domain-general processes, such as Bayesian inference and reinforcement learning, in the development of moral beliefs and values. Alert to the power of these processes and equipped with both the analytic resources of philosophy and the empirical methods of psychology, ‘moral learners’ are ideally placed to discover the contributions of nature, nurture and culture to moral development. However, I argue that to achieve these objectives research on moral learning needs to 1) overcome nativist bias, and 2) distinguish two kinds of social learning: learning from and learning about. An agent learns from others when there is transfer of competence - what the learner learns is similar to, and causally dependent on, what the model knows. When an agent learns about the social world there is no transfer of competence - observable features of other agents are just the content of what-is-learned. Learning from does not require explicit instruction. A novice can learn from an expert who is ‘leaking’ her morality in the form of emotionally charged behaviour or involuntary use of vocabulary. To the extent that moral development depends on learning from other agents, there is the potential for cultural selection of moral beliefs and values.
    [Show full text]