How to Measure “What People Do For a Living” in Research on the Socioeconomic Correlates of Health

John Robert Warren Hsiang-Hui Kuo

Department of Sociology Center for Statistics and the Social Sciences Center for Studies in Demography and Ecology University of Washington

November 2000

Draft: Do Not Cite or Quote Without Permission

Running Head: “What People Do For a Living”

Key Words: Socio-Economic Status; Measurement

Word Count: 5,840

______Paper to be presented at the annual meetings of the American Sociological Association, Washington , D.C., August 2000. Support for this research was provided by a grant from the Center for Statistics and the Social Sciences at the University of Washington. We thank Richard Miech, Robert M. Hauser, and Adrian Raftery for comments and suggestions on this project. Errors or omissions are solely the responsibility of the authors, however. Data and documentation from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study are available online at http://dpls.dacc.wisc.edu/WLS. Direct correspondence to John Robert Warren, University of Washington, Department of Sociology, 202 Savery Hall, Box 353340, Seattle, WA 98195 or email [email protected]. How to Measure “What People Do For a Living” in Research on the Socioeconomic Correlates of Health

ABSTRACT

In this paper we ask two questions. First, is occupational standing associated with health outcomes when health-related criteria are used to establish the relative standing of occupations? This is especially interesting in light of recent evidence (Miech and Hauser

2000) that occupational standing is not independently associated with health outcomes when occupations are ranked using socioeconomic criteria. Second, are characteristics more closely related to health outcomes than occupational characteristics? We use data from the

Wisconsin Longitudinal Study, which includes a unique combination of occupational, job, and health measures. We find few independent relationships between occupational standing and health, using socioeconomic or health-related criteria. However, we do find many significant relationships between job characteristics and health outcomes. We conclude that what people do for a living does matter for their health, beyond the effects of educational attainment, and that to assess the effects of what people do for a living on their health we should measure the characteristics of their , not of their occupations.

How to Measure “What People Do For a Living” in Research on the Socioeconomic Correlates of Health

INTRODUCTION AND OBJECTIVES

Social and health scientists have long been concerned with the impact of socioeconomic circumstances on physical and mental health, and this interest seems to have grown markedly in recent years (Kaplan et al. 1997). Medical as well as social science journals now regularly feature research that considers the influence of education, occupation, income, and other variables on a variety of medical and public health problems. Our research is concerned with the measurement of socioeconomic factors – particularly occupational and job characteritics – in this area of study. In this paper we ask how conclusions about the health impacts of what people do for a living are affected by how occupational and job characteristics are measured and operationalized.

Occupations and Health

Innumerable observers have reported an inverse relationship between occupational standing (however measured) and mortality and morbidity rates (Marmot at al 1987; Kaplan and Keil 1993; Marmot et al. 1997; Gregario et al. 1997; Borooah 1999;

Goodman 1999; Manor et al. 1999). For just two examples, Marmot, et al. (1997) showed that occupation affects self-reported health, rates of depression, and self-esteem in the United States and England and Kitagawa and Hauser (1973) demonstrated that occupation affects mortality in the United States. The fact that occupational standing and health outcomes are inversely related is not surprising in light of research showing that other indicators of socioeconomic standing – such as income, educational attainment, and wealth – are also negatively associated with these outcomes. Marks and Shinberg (1997), for example, showed that education, income, and occupation are all associated with the odds that a woman received a hysterectomy by age 52.

However, socioeconomic variables such as education and income are certainly correlated with occupational standing. Thus a logical question is whether occupation, per se, directly matters for health or whether the apparent association between occupation and health is spurious owing to education, income, or other variables. Marks and

Shinberg (1997) found that occupation does have independent effects on the odds of receiving a hysterectomy after controlling for education and income, but – using the same set of data in an examination of different outcomes – Miech and Hauser (2000) found that

“associations of health outcomes with occupational standing net of educational attainment are mainly weak or non-existent.” As in most prior research, Miech and

Hauser (2000) utilized a variety of socioeconomic and class-based measures of the relative standing of occupations in their analyses. A central question in our research is whether occupation matters for a variety of health outcomes when health-related – as opposed to strictly socioeconomic – criteria are used to establish the relative standing of occupations.

Selecting a Criterion for Ranking Occupations

Although any number of observers have reported an association between occupational standing and health outcomes, very few of their analyses are directly comparable because of vast differences in how “occupation” has been measured and operationalized. This overabundance of measures of occupational standing is understandable since the kind of work

2

that a person does for a living can be (and has been) characterized in terms of a wide array of attributes of that social activity, any of which might matter for their health. Having a particular occupation – such as dishwasher, legal secretary, X-ray technician, or high school teacher – is associated with particular levels of financial remuneration; stress; social prestige; physical exertion; autonomy; non-monetary benefits; intellectual engagement; exposure to hazardous materials; scheduling flexibility; and so forth. These and other attributes of occupations are typically correlated – such that better paying occupations tend to be less physically demanding, less hazardous, and less stressful, for example – but these correlations are far from perfect.

Researchers who aim to assess the impact of occupation, broadly speaking, on health thus face the difficult task of measuring important aspects of occupation that might conceivably influence the health outcomes that interest them.

In their analyses, many researchers classify subjects’ occupations as belonging to one of several nominal categories, such as “white collar” or “blue collar.” Some of these categorization schemes are more theoretically grounded than others. The “white collar” vs. “blue collar” distinction, for example, seems to be based on simplicity or convenience, whereas Wright’s (1996) class typology is solidly grounded in Marxist theory. Our comments on categorical schemes for expressing occupations apply equally well to both. Using the former operationalization, the effects of occupation on health are expressed as the conditional difference between “white collar” workers and “blue collar” workers. This technique adequately captures the full impact of what people do for a living on health outcomes if and only if all workers in a particular occupational category are exposed to the same occupational hazards, are equally well paid, have equal levels of autonomy and

3

authority, and are equal with respect to every other job-related circumstance that might affect health. If workers within a particular occupational category are heterogeneous with respect to these job-related circumstances, then this operationalization likely leads to a biased assessment of the effects of occupation on health.

Some social scientists use more refined systems to classify subjects’ occupations into one of several hundred categories that can be ranked hierarchically in terms of some objective criteria and treated as a continuous variable. For example, subjects’ occupations might be classified as belonging to one of the 501 categories of the 1990 U.S. Census Occupational

Classification. Several distinct characteristics of these 501 occupations – for example, how much they typically pay (occupational earnings), how prestigious they are considered to be

(occupational prestige), how much education is required (occupational education), and how frequently occupational incumbents are injured ( rates) – have been ascertained from various sources, and can be matched to the Census classification. The effects of occupation on health are then expressed as the conditional association between health and a continuous measure of occupational standing.

The first problem with this approach is that it is not obvious which objective criteria researchers should use to rank occupations. Unfortunately, the decision about which criteria to use tends to be based more on convenience and tradition and less on theoretically derived notions of which aspect of occupations might matter most for health outcomes. Most analysts express occupational standing in terms of just one continuous measure (or in terms of a weighted average of two continuous measures, such as Duncan’s 1961 Socioeconomic Index or

SEI). How do conclusions about the effects of occupation on health depend on which objective

4

criteria is used to rank occupations? Miech and Hauser (2000) found few effects, net of educational attainment, of occupational education, occupational earnings, SEI, or prestige on a variety of health outcomes. We ask whether the use of health-relevant criteria for establishing the relative standing of occupations – specifically occupational injury rates – might reveal associations between occupation and health, even net of educational attainment.

The second potential problem with this approach is that if workers within a particular occupational category are heterogeneous with respect to important job-related circumstances or conditions, then this operationalization also leads to a biased assessment of the effects of occupation on health. Specific jobs within occupational categories may be quite diverse in terms of health-relevant variables.

These methodological issues surrounding the measurement of occupation are worth worrying about because there is evidence that they matter for substantive conclusions. For example, in the context of research on occupational stratification,

Warren, Hauser, and Sheridan (1998) showed that important substantive conclusions about gender differences in the process of occupational stratification were quite sensitive to which criteria was used to rank the 503 occupations in the 1980 U.S. Census classification. Their work follows on the heels of research showing that it matters (within the context of occupational stratification research) whether occupations are ranked on the basis of how prestigious they are or on the basis of SEI (Featherman and Hauser 1976;

Featherman and Stevens 1982) and on whether occupations are ranked on the basis of the characteristics of their male incumbents, their female incumbents, or both (Boyd 1986;

Nakao and Teas 1994).

5

There is also limited evidence that the way in which occupation is measured and expressed matters for substantive conclusions about the health consequences of occupational standing. For example, Miech and Hauser (2000) estimate the effects of occupation on a variety of health outcomes, and in so doing they compare the predictive validity of several measures of occupation: occupational prestige, SEI, occupational education, occupational earnings, Wright’s (1997) typology, and Erikson and

Goldthorpe’s (1992) social class typology. Although those authors found few independent effects of any of their measures of occupational standing on several health outcomes, they did find some interesting variability in the predictive power of their measures. Most notably, occupational education had the greatest predictive validity for some outcomes, while Wright’s and Erikson and Goldthorpe’s social class typologies had the least predictive validity.

Job-Based Versus Occupation-Based Measures of “What People Do For a Living”

Jobs and occupations are not the same thing (Hauser and Carr 1995). A job is a specific bundle of activities, responsibilities, and behaviors that typically results in financial remuneration; there may be as many jobs as there are employed people. An occupation, on the other hand, is a convenient aggregation of jobs that are similar in some respect. Regardless of how occupations are grouped or which criteria are used to rank them, within-occupation category heterogeneity in health-relevant aspects of jobs may bias the estimated effects of occupation on health outcomes. That is, to the extent that those aspects of jobs that affect health vary within occupational categories, our analyses of the effects of that aspect of jobs on health will likely be biased. While standard

6

practice has been to ascertain respondents’ occupations and then assign to each respondent the aggregate characteristics of people in that occupation, Hauser and Carr

(1995: 40) note that “the best way to collect job information relevant to health and well- being may be to ask directly about the conditions of work.” Thus a central question in our analysis is what difference it makes for research on the association between health and what people do for a living when the latter is expressed in terms of the characteristics of each respondent’s working conditions instead of in terms of the conditions typical of people who do similar kinds of work.

We wonder whether job-based (as opposed to occupation-based) measures of what people do for a living might be more closely associated with health outcomes.

Consider the findings of Link, Lennon, and Dohrenwend (1993), who conclude that occupational direction, control, and planning affect depression/distress; Link,

Dohrenwend, and Skodol (1986), who show that noisome work conditions are important in the etiology of schizophrenia; and Karasek and colleagues (Karasek al. 1981; Karasek et al. 1988), who argue that high job demands and decision-making latitude affect coronary hear disease. In each case, it seems plausible that there is a great deal of variation in relevant job characteristics within each of the 500 or so U.S. Census occupational categories. By aggregating to the occupation level, prior research may be missing important information about the relationship between “what people do for a living” and health.

In our research we ask two specific questions. First, is occupational standing associated with a variety of health outcomes when health-related criteria – occupational injury rates

7

– are used to establish the relative standing of occupations? Second, how might heterogeneity in what people do for a living within each of the hundreds of occupational categories influence conclusions about the association between what people do for a living and health? That is, might job characteristics be more closely related to health outcomes than occupational characteristics? Miech and Hauser (2000) concluded that when utilizing the socioeconomic criteria typically used to establish the relative standing of occupations – occupational education, occupational earnings, SEI, and prestige – there appear to be few and weak associations between occupation and health net of educational attainment. This implies that what people do for a living doesn’t matter for their health once their education has been accounted for. We ask whether this finding persists when we use alternate criteria to rank occupations or when we use job-based characteristics of what people do for a living.

DATA

We use data from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS). The WLS has followed a cohort of 10,317 Wisconsin high school graduates from their senior year of high school in

1957 through the early 1990s (with a remarkably high rate of sample retention). The WLS data are especially useful for our purposes. Respondents’ occupations at age 52-53 were coded to the standards of the 1990 Census, such that they can be mapped into occupation- based measures like those described above. In addition, the WLS questionnaire ascertained the details of respondents’ job characteristics – like like how much they get paid per hour, how often they get dirty, how much autonomy they have, how often they are exposed to hazardous materials, how they rate their jobs relative to an average job, and so forth. The 1993 mail survey of WLS respondents also contains rich information about a large selection of self-

8

reported health outcomes and medical conditions at age 52 or so. Each of these occupation, job, and health measures are described below.

Care must be taken when generalizing from results derived from analyses of WLS data. Sample members are all 52-53 year old high school graduates and are almost universally White. About one in five is of farm origins, and about 70 percent still lived in Wisconsin 35 years after their graduation from high school. However, prior research has profitably used WLS data for research on socioeconomic gradients in health (Hauser and Carr 1995; Marmot et al 1997; Marks and Shinberg 1997; Miech and Hauser 2000) and the WLS’s unique combination of occupational, job, and health measures make these data most suitable for our purposes.

In selecting our analysis sample, we began by determining the four most prevalent self-reported health problems and the five most prevalent self-reported medical conditions for both men and women. We then selected all cases with no missing data on these health outcome variables or on educational attainment, current or last occupation in

1992/3, or the job characteristic variables (including hourly wage rate) that we describe below. This sample selection scheme leaves us with 82% – 5,645 of 6,875 – of the respondents who participated in the 1993 WLS mail survey.

Measuring Occupational and Job Characteristics

Table 1 describes variables representing educational attainment and occupational standing, separately for men and women. Educational attainment is measured as years of school completed; by definition, all sample members have completed at least 12 years of schooling. We use four measures of the socioeconomic characteristics of occupations

9

and six measures of the injury-related characteristics of occupations. Occupational prestige represents the relative desirability or goodness of an occupation, and is based on the subjective ranking of occupations by respondents to the 1989 General Social Survey

(Nakao and Treas 1994). Prior work has established the invariability of these and other relative prestige ratings; regardless of sex, social class, industrialized country, or instructions about how to rank occupations, respondents tend to agree about the relative prestige position of detailed occupational categories. Nakao-Treas prestige scores range from 0 to 100, where higher numbers represent more desirable occupations. Our measures of occupational education and occupational earnings were constructed by

Hauser and Warren (1997) for use with the 1990 Census Occupational Classification.

Occupational education is based on the proportion of incumbents in each occupation who completed one or more years of college. Occupational earnings is based on the proportion of incumbents in each occupation who earned more than $25,000 in 1989.

Each of these proportions was transformed into a started logit of the form log[(p +

.01)/(1 - p + .01)], where p is the proportion of the occupational incumbents above the threshold. This transformation reduces heteroscedasticity in the transformed variable without creating extreme outliers (Mosteller and Tukey 1977:109-115; Hauser and

Warren 1997), and yields measures that range from about –4.5 to about 4.5 in our sample. Finally, we use a nominal categorization of occupations that further aggregates respondents’ occupations into 17 major occupation groups such as “professional and technical specialty occupations” and “administrative support occupations.” This

10

measure corresponds to (frequently less detailed) nominal classifications often used in research in this area of research.

All injury-related occupational characteristics are derived from the Bureau of

Labor Statistics’ 1994 Survey of Occupational Injuries and Illnesses. These data report the occupation-specific number and cause of nonfatal occupational injuries and illnesses involving days away from work in 1993, the year that most WLS respondents reported the characteristics of their current or most recent jobs and occupations. For each occupation category we computed rates of occupational injuries by dividing the number of injuries in that occupation in 1993 by the total number of people who held that occupation according to data from the 1990 U.S. Census. The “total occupational injury rate” simply reflects the number of occupational injuries per 10,000 occupational incumbents in 1993. The other five occupational injury rates are cause-specific. For example, the “slip or fall occupational injury rate” reflects the number of occupational injuries due to slipping or falling per 10,000 incumbents in 1993. Because of the skewed distributions of the occupational injury rate variables, we have transformed them by adding a small constant at taking their natural logarithm.

Although we wonder whether occupational injury rates might be more closely related to health outcomes than occupational socioeconomic characteristics, we acknowledge that these measures are not ideal. We would prefer to have measures of the extent to which occupational incumbents are exposed to dangerous conditions, do repetitive work, overexert themselves, and so forth. However, we are only able to use indirect indicators of these things. We are forced to assume that if injuries due to

11

overexertion, for example, are high in a particular occupation, then people who hold that occupation are required to overexert themselves more often than people in other occupations.

Table 2 reports the correlations among these educational attainment, base hourly wage rate (described below), and socioeconomic and injury-related occupational characteristics (with the exception of the Group II measure, which is a nominal variable).

The socioeconomic occupational characteristics are highly inter-correlated; likewise, the occupational injury rate measures are highly inter-correlated. More interestingly, the socioeconomic and injury-related characteristics of occupations are quite closely related.

The correlations of occupational injury rates with occupational prestige and occupational education range between –0.55 and –0.66; their correlations with occupational earnings are more modest, ranging from –0.32 to –0.53. Simply put, occupations that enjoy higher social esteem, that require more educational credentials, and that tend to pay better are typically much safer occupations. Interestingly, educational attainment and base hourly wage rate are each more closely related to the socioeconomic characteristics of occupations than to the injury-related occupational characteristics.

Table 3 describes variables that characterize respondents’ jobs. Many of the job characteristic measures are loosely parallel to measures of occupational characteristics.

For example, WLS respondents were asked how much education most people in jobs like their have (paralleling the occupational education measure), how many hours they spend on their jobs doing the same things over and over (paralleling the repetition occupational injury rate), and how often they are exposed to disease agents, dangerous equipment, or

12

hazardous chemicals (paralleling the exposure occupational injury rate). Other measures indicate whether respondents’ jobs require lots of physical effort, require intense concentration or attention, require working under the pressure of time, or involve getting dirty. Respondents were also asked how many hours they spend working with their hands, tools or equipment and how many hours they spend dealing with people about work. The base hourly wage rate was computed based on information about respondents’ earnings, hours worked per week, and weeks worked in the 12 months preceding the survey; to reduce skew in the distribution of this variable, we have taken its natural logarithm after adding a small constant. The WLS contains other measures of job characteristics, but we have selected a set of measures that might conceivably have implications for respondents’ health. For example, respondents who work in jobs that require lots of physical effort, that involve getting dirty, that are repetitious, and/or that involve exposure to dangerous or hazardous conditions might quite reasonably have poorer health outcomes than respondents who work in other jobs. Likewise, jobs that involve stressors – such as intense concentration, time pressure, or dealing with people – might have different consequences for jobholders than less stressful jobs.

Measuring Health Problems and Medical Conditions

Table 4 describes variables pertaining to respondents’ self-reported health problems and medical conditions. For both men and women we begin with self-reported overall health. Respondents were asked, “How would you rate your health at the present time?” Because of the distribution of responses, we group respondents into those who said “good” or “excellent” and those who said “fair,” “poor,” or “very poor.”

13

Interestingly, this self-reported overall health measure is closely related to clinically diagnosed health problems and is a better predictor of mortality than physician-assessed health (Mossey and Shapiro 1982).

WLS respondents were presented with a list of 24 health problems and asked whether they had experienced them in the past six months. They were then asked to report the frequency with which they experienced each problem and how much discomfort each problem had caused them in the past six months. For each self-reported health problem, we coded a respondent as having that problem if they reported experiencing it at least daily in the past six months and indicated that it caused them at least some discomfort. As noted earlier, our analyses consider only the four most frequently self-reported health problems for men and the four most frequently self- reported health problems for women. As it turns out, three of these health problems – back pain or strain, stiff or swollen joints, and aching muscles – are frequently experienced by both men and women. Male respondents also frequently report experiencing ringing in their ears and female respondents frequently report having trouble sleeping.

WLS respondents were also presented with a list of 17 medical conditions and asked whether a medical professional has said they have that condition. Here we selected the five medical conditions most frequently reported by men and the five medical conditions most frequently reported by women. Four medical conditions – high blood pressure, arthritis or rheumatism, allergies, and serious back problems – are frequently experienced by both men and women. Men also frequently report

14

experiencing heart problems and women frequently report circulation problems.

Respondents were also asked how much each condition interferes with what they like to do, but we have not taken this information into account; high blood pressure, for example, is still a serious medical condition even if it does not directly interfere with respondents’ daily lives.

RESULTS

We ask two central questions. First, net of educational attainment, is occupation associated with health outcomes when occupational injury rates are used to establish the relative standing of occupations? Second, are job characteristics more closely and independently associated with health outcomes than occupational characteristics? Following

Miech and Hauser (2000), we begin by estimating the effect of educational attainment on an outcome, and then ask whether the addition of a given occupational or job characteristic variables adds to the fit of the model. Our decisions about improvement in model fit are initially based on standard c2 tests, but we temper our conclusions by using the Bayesian

Information Criterion (BIC) as suggested by Raftery (1995). BIC provides a better-calibrated assessment of the evidence for a hypothesis than P-values in analyses of data from large samples. Our tests of statistical significance compare the fit of models that do not include job or occupational characteristics variables to those that do.

Table 5 reports the results of models predicting self-reported health problems and medical conditions for men, and Table 6 reports these results for women. The first row of each table suggests – with a few exceptions – that educational attainment has negative and statistically significant effects on these health outcomes. For men, educational

15

attainment is significantly related to 8 of 10 outcomes using conventional criteria for statistical significance and to 5 of these outcomes using the more conservative BIC. For women, conventional significance criteria show an effect of education in 7 of 10 cases, but BIC suggests that only 4 of these associations are very robust.

The next section of Tables 5 and 6 reports the improvement in model chi-square associated with adding measures of the socioeconomic characteristics of occupations.

This section of our analysis loosely replicates part of the work of Miech and Hauser

(2000), although they used the 1970 Census Occupational Classification and a slightly different subset of respondents. Our results, however, are quite similar. Using conventional standards of statistical significance, occupational prestige, occupational education, occupational earnings, and Group II occupation categories are associated with health outcomes about a quarter of the time for men (with those associations most frequently involving occupational education and occupational earnings) and almost never for women. In only one case for men and in no cases for women do any of these associations approach statistical significance using BIC. That is, like Miech and Hauser

(2000), we find that significant associations between health outcomes and socioeconomic measures of occupational standing are very much the exception to the rule.

Injury-related measures of occupational standing seem to fare little better. The next section of Tables 5 and 6 reports the improvement in model chi-square associated with adding injury-related measures of occupations standing. For men, only once does the addition of occupational injury rate information add to the fit of the model, and this

16

association is weak using BIC. For women, occupational injury rates appear to be particularly associated with aching muscles and arthritis or rheumatism, and overall – again, using standard criteria for assessing statistical significance – about one in six null hypotheses are rejected. However, using BIC, only one of these associations appears to be robust.

The final section of Tables 5 and 6 reports the improvement in model chi-square associated with adding job characteristic variables. For men, at least one job characteristic affects each of the 10 health outcomes using the conventional chi-squared criteria; indeed, we would reject the null hypothesis of no association in 33 of these 120 models. For aching muscles and arthritis or rheumatism, 7 of the 11 measures have significant effects for men. In the models for women, at least one job characteristic affects 9 of 10 health outcomes, and – still using conventional criteria for hypothesis testing – we would reject the null model in 35 of 110 cases. For women, 6 of the 11 measures affect aching muscles, 6 of 11 affect serious back problems, and 4 of 11 affect stiff or swollen joints. When we use BIC to assess statistical significance, we reject the null hypothesis in only 7 cases for men and 9 for women. That is, most of the associations that are significant by conventional standards are not significant using the better-calibrated BIC standards.

The associations that remain using the Bayesian Inference Criteria are interesting.

Of the 7 significant effects of job characteristics for men, 4 affect aching muscles. The typical education level of jobholders, hours spent working with hands, repetitive work, and exposure to dangerous equipment all affect the odds that a man experienced

17

discomfort from aching muscles at least daily in the past six months. Thus for men, dangerous, repetitive, hands-on work seems to be associated with aching muscles. Also, men with higher base hourly wage rates report better overall health, men whose jobs involve frequent physical effort are more likely to experience arthritis or rheumatism, and men who frequently deal with people and who are exposed to disease agents are more likely to suffer from allergies.

For women, frequent physical effort, frequent time pressure, and getting dirty all affect the odds of discomfort from aching muscles. Like men, women with higher base hourly wage rates report better overall health. Women who get dirty on the job are more likely to experience arthritis or rheumatism, women whose jobs require frequent physical effort are more likely to have been diagnosed with serious back problems, and women are more likely to suffer from circulation problems if their jobs require frequent physical effort or getting dirty.

From a public health point of view, we do not wish to make too much of the significant associations involving job characteristics shown in Tables 5 and 6. Many of the associations seem to have some face validity – it makes sense that men who do repetitive work have aching muscles, and that women whose jobs require frequent physical effort experience aching muscles and serious back problems – but our methods of selecting job characteristics and health outcomes were not designed to yield substantive findings about particular health problems. They were the data at our disposal. Tables 5 and 6 demonstrate that some job characteristics have independent effects on some health outcomes, whereas almost no occupational characteristics have

18

any independent effects. For any particular health outcome, our 12 job characteristics are not necessarily the most pertinent or consequential aspect of what people do for a living. For the medical condition “serious back problem” we might like to know, for example, how often subjects lift objects and how heavy those objects are. Conversely, for any particular job characteristic, these health outcomes are not necessarily the health outcomes most likely to be affected. We might speculate, for instance, that the job characteristic “frequent intense concentration” might affect the odds of discomfort from eyestrain.

DISCUSSION

We began by asking two questions. First, we asked whether occupational standing is associated with health outcomes when health-related criteria – occupational injury rates – are used to establish the relative standing of occupations. From the work of Miech and

Hauser (2000) we expected that socioeconomic characteristics of occupations

(occupational prestige, occupational education, and occupational earnings) would not be associated with these outcomes, but we wondered whether occupational standing might be associated with these outcomes when the criteria used to rank occupations is more proximally related to health. Our results conform to those of Miech and Hauser (2000), but the answer to our own question is clearly “no.” Occupational injury rates are not associated with the health outcomes that we considered after controlling for educational attainment. As discussed above, however, occupational injury rates may not be ideal measures of occupational incumbents’ exposure to risk factors for various health problems or medical conditions. The strong associations between occupational injury

19

rates and occupational socioeconomic characteristics suggest that such measures might be put to interesting use in research on occupational stratification, but their utility for research on health outcomes appears to be limited – at least for the outcomes we considered.

Second, we asked how heterogeneity in work experiences within occupational categories might influence conclusions about the relationships between what people do for a living and their health. To address this, we considered whether job – as opposed to occupational – characteristics are more closely related to health outcomes after controlling for educational attainment. Here, the answer is a qualified “yes.” Some job characteristics are independently related to some of the health outcomes that we considered, even using the more stringent BIC criteria for assessing statistical significance. The significant associations make intuitive sense, although not all of the intuitively sensible associations (e.g., between frequency of working with hands and arthritis or rheumatism) were significant. We are led to conclude that what people do for a living does matter for their health, above and beyond the effects of educational attainment, and that to assess the nature and magnitude of the effects of what people do for a living on their health we should measure the characteristics and requirements of their jobs, not of their occupations.

With that said, we are not willing to make any substantive conclusions about the relationships between any of the job characteristics that we used and any of the health outcomes that we considered. In order to do so, we would need to follow methods of selecting measures of job characteristics that are quite different from those employed in this research. If our goal were to assess the association between what people do for a

20

living and their odds of developing a particular medical condition, we would begin by cataloging the risk factors for that medical condition. We would then collect information from respondents about the extent to which they are exposed to each of those risk factors on the job. Subjects may well be exposed to those same risk factors away from the job as well, so we would need to be careful to assess the added and unique exposure to those risk factors associated with on the job activities and conditions.

With information about all job characteristics relating to risk factors for a particular medical condition in hand, we would then be in a position to assess the association between what people do for a living and that particular medical condition. Of course different medical conditions have different risk factors, and so the information that we would want to gather about job characteristics might differ if our focus is on another medical condition. In the present study, we simply selected a set of 12 job characteristics that seemed like they might reasonably have mattered for people’s health – whether they get dirty, how frequently they are exposed to dangerous chemicals, and so forth – but we had no a priori reason for expecting those specific job characteristics to matter for any particular health problem or medical condition.

The fact that some of them did matter should encourage researchers to collect and use information about what people do for a living in investigations of the relationship between socioeconomic status and health. What we are suggesting, of course, is that researchers who collect data for the purpose of assessing the association between socioeconomic status and health should include specific, detailed, and health outcome- specific survey questions about job characteristics in their surveys. It may well be

21

possible and desirable to develop a standard set of survey questions that captures everything we would want to know about people’s jobs in research on socioeconomic differentials in particular health outcomes. It means, however, that we must first put some thought and effort into determining what we would want to know about people’s jobs for research on any particular health problem or medical condition.

Beyond issues of measurement, we must also recognize that cross-sectional data are not perfectly suited to assessing the association between what people do for a living and their health. (Our data are, in fact, longitudinal, but measures of job characteristics and health outcomes are available only in the most recent wave of survey data.) People change jobs, and thus they change the extent to which they are exposed to health-relevant working conditions and the frequency with which they engage in health-relevant activities. It seems reasonable to suppose, furthermore, that people sometimes change jobs because of their health or because former jobs put their health at risk in some way.

A more complete understanding of the impact of what people do for a living on health outcomes would need to focus longitudinally on the characteristics of prior jobs as well as those of the jobs that respondents hold at the time that their health outcomes are assessed.

22

REFERENCES

Borooah, VK. 1999. “Occupational Class and the Probability of Long-Term Limiting Illness.”

Social Science and Medicine 49: 253-266.

Boyd, Monica. 1986. “Socioeconomic Indices and Sexual Inequality: A Tale of Scales.”

Canadian Review of Sociology and Anthropology 23:457-480.

Duncan, Otis D. 1961. “A Socioeconomic Index for All Occupations.” Pp. 109-138 in

Occupations and , edited by Albert J. Reiss Jr. New York: Free Press.

Featherman, David L. and Robert M. Hauser. 1976. “Prestige or Socioeconomic Scales in the

Study of Occupational Achievement?” Sociological Methods & Research 4:403-22.

Featherman, David L. and Gillian Stevens. 1982. “A Revised Socioeconomic Index of

Occupational Status: Application in Analysis of Sex Differences in Attainment.” Pp.

93-129 in Measures of Socioeconomic Status, edited by Mary Powers. Boulder:

Westview Press.

Gregorio, David I., Stephen J. Walsh, and Deborah Paturzo. 197. “The Effects of Occupation-

Based Social Position on Mortality in a Large American Cohort.” American Journal of

Public Health 87:1472-1475.

Hauser, Robert M. and Deborah Carr. 1995. “Measuring Poverty and Socioeconomic Status in

Studies of Health and Well-Being.” Working Paper 94-24, Revised 1995. Madison, WI:

Center for Demography and Ecology.

Hauser, Robert M. and John Robert Warren. 1997. “Socioeconomic Indexes of Occupational

Status: A Review, Update, and Critique.” Pp. 177-298 in Sociological Methodology,

1997, edited by Adrian Raftery. Cambridge: Blackwell.

23

Kaplan, GA and JW Lynch. 1997. “Editorial: Whither Studies on the Socioeconomic

Foundations of Population Health?” American Journal of Public Health 87:1409-1411.

Kaplan, GA and JE Keil. 1993. “Socioeconomic Factors and Cardiovascular Disease: A

Review of the Literature.” Circulation 88:1973-1988.

Karasek, R, D Baker, F Marxer, A Ahlbom, and T Theorell. 1981. “Job Decision Latitude, Job

Demands, and Cardiovascular Disease: A Prospective Study of Swedish Men.”

American Journal of Public Health 71:694-705.

Karasek, R, T Theorell, JE Schwartz, RL Schnall, CF Pieper, and JL Michela. 1988. “Job

Characteristics in Relation to the Relevance of Myocardial Infarction in the U.S. Health

Examination Survey (HES) and the Health and Nutrition Exaination Study (HANES).”

American Journal of Public Health 78:910-918.

Kitagawa, Evely and Philip M. Hauser. 1973. Differential Mortality in the United States: A

Study in Socioeconomic Epidemiology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Link, Bruce G., Bruce P. Dohrenwend, and Andrew E. Skodol. 1986. “Socioeconomic Status

and Scizophrenia: Noisome Occupational Characteristics as a Risk Factor.” American

Sociological Review 51:242-258.

Link, Bruce G., Mary Clare Lennon, and Bruce P. Dohrenwend. 1993. “Socioeconomic Status

and Depression: The Role of Occupational Direction, Control, and Planning.”

American Journal of Sociology 98:1351-1387.

Manor, O, Z Eisenbach, E Peritz, and Y Friedlander. 1999. “Mortality Differentials Among

Israeli Men.” American Journal of Public Health 89:1807-1813.

24

Marks, Nadine F. and Diane S. Shinberg. 1997. “Socioeconomic Differences in Hysterectomy:

The Wisconsin Longitudinal Study.” American Journal of Public Health 87:1507-

1514.

Marmot, MG, M Kogevinas, and MA Elsten. 1987. “Social/Economic Status and Disease.”

Annual Review of Public Health 8:111-135.

Marmot, Michael, Carol D. Ryff, Larry L. Bumpass, Martin Shipley, and Nadine F. Marks.

1997. “Social Inequalities in Health: Next Questions and Converging Evidence.” Social

Science and Medicine 44:901-910.

Miech, Richard A. and Robert M. Hauser. In Press. “Social Class Indicators and Health at

Midlife.” Annals of Epidemiology.

Mossey, JM and E Shapiro. 1982. “Self-Rated Health: A Predictor of Mortality among the

Elderly.” American Journal of Public Health 72:800-808.

Mosteller, Frederick, and John W. Tukey. 1977. Data Analysis and Regression: A Second

Course in Statistics. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley.

Nakao, Keiko and Judith Treas. 1994. “Updating Occupational Prestige and Socioeconomic

Scores: How the New Measures Measure Up.” Pp. 1-72 in Sociological Methodology,

1994, edited by Peter Marsden. Washington, D.C.: American Sociological Association.

Raftery, AE. 1995. “Bayesian Model Selection in Social Research.” Pp. 111-163 in

Sociological Methodology 1995, Peter Marsden, editor. Cambridge: Basil Blackwell.

Sewell, WH and RM Hauser. 1975. Education, Occupation, and Earnings: Achievement in the

Early Career. New York: Academic Press.

25

Warren, John Robert, Robert M. Hauser, and Jennifer T. Sheridan. 1998. “Choosing a Measure

of Occupational Standing: How Useful are Composite Measures I Analyses of Gender

Inequality in Occupational Attainment?” Sociological Methods & Research 27:1 – 76.

Wright, Erik O. 1996. Class Counts: Comparative Studies in Class Analysis. New York:

Cambridge University Press.

26

Table 1. Variables Expressing Educational Attainment and Occupational Characteristics

Men (n=2,735) Women (n=2,910)

Variable Definition Min. Max. Mean (s.d.) Mean (s.d.)

Years of Total years of schooling completed 12.0 20.0 14.0 (2.5) 13.1 (1.9) Education

Socioeconomic Occupational Characteristics

Occupational Nakao-Treas (1989) Prestige Rating 12.6 94.6 57.5 (21.9) 52.2 (21.2) Prestige

Occupational Proportion of occupational incumbents -4.6 4.5 0.6 (1.5) 0.6 (1.3) Education with at least some college^

Occupational Proportion of occupational incumbents -3.0 1.9 -0.5 (0.9) -1.5 (1.0) Earnings earnings at least $25,000^

Group II Nominal categorization of occupations into 1.0 17.0 Modal Category: Modal Category: Categories 17 major groups (such as "Sales Occupations Professional and Administrative in Retail Trade," etc.) Technical Spec- Support Occup- ialty Occupations ations Injury-Related Occupational Characteristics

Total Occupational Total rate (per 10,000 incumbents) of nonfatal 0.0 7.6 4.0 (1.8) 4.1 (1.5) Injury Rate occupational injuries and illnesses involving days away from work in 1993*

Object-Related Occ. Rate (per 10,000) of nonfatal occupational 0.0 6.7 2.7 (1.7) 2.4 (1.4) Injury Rate injuries and illnesses that were caused by being struck by, struck against, or caught in objects*

Slip or Fall Occ. Rate (per 10,000) of nonfatal occupational 0.0 6.6 2.7 (1.4) 2.8 (1.2) Injury Rate injuries and illnesses that were caused by falls or slips without falls*

Overexertion Occ. Rate (per 10,000) of nonfatal occupational 0.0 6.3 2.7 (1.7) 2.7 (1.5) Injury Rate injuries and illnesses that were caused by overexertion*

Repetition Occ. Rate (per 10,000) of nonfatal occupational 0.0 5.2 1.3 (1.2) 1.6 (1.1) Injury Rate injuries and illnesses that were caused by repetitive motion*

Exposure Occ. Rate (per 10,000) of nonfatal occupational 0.0 4.9 1.4 (1.2) 1.4 (1.1) Injury Rate injuries and illnesses that were caused by exposure to harmful substance, environment*

^ Occupational education and occupational earnings are transformed as ln((p+0.01)/(1-p+0.01)) as per Hauser and Warren (1997) * All occupational injury rates are transformed as ln(p+0.01) because of the skewed nature of the distributions

Note: Injury-related occupation characteristics are derived from the Bureau of Labor Statistics' 1993 Survey of Occupational Injuries and Illnesses. Table 2. Correlations Between Education, Measures of Occupational Characteristics, and Base Hourly Wage Rate

Total Occ. Object- Slip or Fall Overex- Repetition Exposure All WLS Respondents Years of Base Hourly Occup. Occup. Occup. Injury Related Occ. Occup. ertion Occ. Occ. Injury Occ. Injury (n=5,645) Education Wage Rate Prestige Education Earnings Rate Injury Rate Injury Rate Injury Rate Rate Rate

Years of Education 1.00

Base Hourly Wage Rate 0.32 1.00

Occupational Prestige 0.51 0.33 1.00

Occupational Education 0.60 0.32 0.85 1.00

Occupational Earnings 0.44 0.45 0.71 0.68 1.00

Total Occupational Injury Rate -0.41 -0.11 -0.59 -0.58 -0.37 1.00

Object-Related Occ. Injury Rate -0.41 -0.11 -0.64 -0.66 -0.33 0.93 1.00

Slip or Fall Occ. Injury Rate -0.42 -0.15 -0.62 -0.59 -0.43 0.95 0.89 1.00

Overexertion Occ. Injury Rate -0.42 -0.12 -0.56 -0.59 -0.32 0.94 0.92 0.88 1.00

Repetition Occ. Injury Rate -0.45 -0.18 -0.63 -0.65 -0.53 0.68 0.65 0.63 0.63 1.00

Exposure Occ. Injury Rate -0.39 -0.12 -0.55 -0.58 -0.35 0.83 0.85 0.81 0.83 0.63 1.00 Table 3. Variables Expressing Job Characteristics

Men (n=2,735) Women (n=2,910)

Variable Definition Min. Max. Mean (s.d.) Mean (s.d.)

Base Hourly Natural Log of Base Hourly Wage Rate -2.3 5.3 2.9 (0.8) 2.1 (1.0) Wage Rate

College is Typical Proportion who say that most people in jobs 0.0 1.0 0.56 (0.50) 0.45 (0.50) of Jobholders like theirs have completed at least some college

Frequent Physical Proportion who say their job always or 0.0 1.0 0.31 (0.46) 0.37 (0.48) Effort frequently requires lots of physical effort

Frequent Intense Proportion who say their job always requires 0.0 1.0 0.38 (0.49) 0.47 (0.50) Concentration intense concentration or attention

Frequent Time Proportion who say their job always requires 0.0 1.0 0.30 (0.46) 0.35 (0.48) Pressure working under the pressure of time

Hours Working Number of hours respondents spend on the job 0.0 96.0 21.2 (18.3) 22.9 (15.3) w/ Hands working with their hands, tools, or equipment

Hours Dealing Number of hours respondents spend on the job 0.0 96.0 22.5 (16.0) 23.4 (14.9) w/ People dealing with people about work --- not just passing the time of day

Hours Doing Number of hours respondents spend on the job 0.0 96.0 20.5 (18.7) 20.9 (15.8) Repetitive Work doing the same things over and over

Ever Gets Dirty Proportion who say they ever get dirty on 0.0 1.0 0.53 (0.50) 0.46 (0.50) the job

Exposure to Proportion who say that they are exposed to 0.0 1.0 0.01 (0.12) 0.06 (0.23) Disease Agents bacterial, viral, fungal, or other disease agents on the job

Exposure to Proportion who say that they use heavy 0.0 1.0 0.11 (0.31) 0.03 (0.18) Dangerous machinery, dangerous equipment, or Equipment transportation vehicles on the job

Exposure to Proportion who say that they are exposed to 0.0 1.0 0.18 (0.39) 0.10 (0.30) Hazardous hazardous chemicals on the job Chemicals Table 4. Variables Expressing Men's and Women's Most Frequently Self-Reported Health Problems and Medical Conditions*

Men (n=2,735) Women (n=2,910)

Variable Definition Min. Max. Mean (s.d.) Mean (s.d.)

Self-Reported Health Problems

Poor Overall Proportion who rate their health as fair or 0.0 1.0 0.11 (0.31) 0.12 (0.32) Health worse than fair at the present time

Back Pain, Proportion who in the past 6 months have been 0.0 1.0 0.08 (0.27) 0.10 (0.30) Strain bothered daily or more by back pain or strain

Stiff, Swollen Proportion who in the past 6 months have been 0.0 1.0 0.07 (0.25) 0.13 (0.33) Joints bothered daily or more by stiff, swollen joints

Aching Proportion who in the past 6 months have been 0.0 1.0 0.06 (0.24) 0.12 (0.32) Muscles bothered daily or more by aching muscles

Trouble Proportion who in the past 6 months have been 0.0 1.0 - - 0.07 (0.25) Sleeping bothered daily or more by sleeping problems

Ringing Proportion who in the past 6 months have been 0.0 1.0 0.05 (0.21) - - in Ears bothered daily or more by ringing in their ears

Self-Reported Medical Conditions

High Blood Proportion who report that a medical professional 0.0 1.0 0.23 (0.42) 0.20 (0.40) Pressure says they have high blood pressure

Arthritis or Proportion who report that a medical professional 0.0 1.0 0.18 (0.38) 0.27 (0.44) Rheumatism says they have arthritis or rheumatism

Allergies Proportion who report that a medical professional 0.0 1.0 0.10 (0.29) 0.18 (0.39) says they have allergies

Serious Back Proportion who report that a medical professional 0.0 1.0 0.09 (0.28) 0.08 (0.27) Problem says they have serious back trouble

Heart Proportion who report that a medical professional 0.0 1.0 0.07 (0.26) - - Problem says they have heart trouble

Circulation Proportion who report that a medical professional 0.0 1.0 - - 0.06 (0.24) Problem says they have circulation problems

Note: Frequencies of self-reports are given for men's 4 most commonly self-reported health problems, women's 4 most commonly self-reported health problems, men's 5 most commonly self-reported medical conditions, women's 5 most commonly self-reported medical conditions, and self-reported overall health for both men and women. Table 5. Education, Occupational Characteristics, and Job Characteristics: Effects on Self-Reported Health Problems and Medical Conditions, MEN

MEN (N = 2,735) Self-Reported Health Problems Self-Reported Medical Conditions

Poor Back Stiff, High Arthritis Serious Overall Pain, Swollen Aching Ringing Blood or Back Heart Health Strain Joints Muscles in Ears Press. Rheum. Allergies Prob. Prob. Logistic Regression of Outcomes on Years of Education Years of Odds Ratio 0.85 0.86 0.91 0.86 0.90 0.94 0.89 1.08 0.95 0.94 Education Model Chi-Square 35.08 ** 22.35 ** 9.37 ^ 19.11 ** 7.63 ^ 12.08 * 28.43 ** 8.43 ^ 3.40 3.58 Improvement in Model Chi-Square by Adding Socioeconomic Occupational Characteristics

Occupational Prestige 3.15 1.20 1.30 8.93 ^ 0.05 1.52 3.41 0.56 2.78 0.14 Occupational Education 7.23 ^ 3.78 3.19 14.94 ** 1.14 0.23 5.32 ^ 0.36 6.66 ^ 0.79 Occupational Earnings 6.45 ^ 1.20 3.36 6.27 ^ 0.16 0.92 5.64 ^ 0.48 2.91 0.05 Group II Categories 17.35 11.71 23.17 27.58 ^ 22.16 38.89 ^ 20.96 6.11 20.20 12.60 Improvement in Model Chi-Square by Adding Injury-Related Occupational Characteristics Total Occupational Injury Rate 0.21 1.56 1.16 0.75 0.97 0.54 0.02 0.17 1.90 0.78 Object-Related Occ. Injury Rate 1.67 3.07 0.01 3.57 1.48 0.53 1.28 0.34 3.55 0.88 Slip or Fall Occ. Injury Rate 0.36 1.77 1.37 0.89 1.02 0.66 0.09 1.14 3.19 0.36 Overexertion Occ. Injury Rate 1.50 2.49 0.56 0.79 0.33 0.09 0.55 1.03 2.89 0.41 Repetition Occ. Injury Rate 0.82 1.47 0.53 1.80 1.93 3.36 1.77 0.00 1.64 0.70 Exposure Occ. Injury Rate 2.00 2.29 0.96 2.43 2.47 0.08 3.09 0.62 4.63 ^ 0.30 Improvement in Model Chi-Square by Adding Job Characteristics

Base Hourly Wage Rate 15.34 ** 1.13 7.16 ^ 4.08 ^ 0.06 4.71 ^ 7.73 ^ 0.03 0.02 0.13 College is Typical of Jobholders 5.29 ^ 6.16 ^ 2.32 13.32 * 0.18 0.04 7.68 ^ 1.39 4.23 ^ 0.75 Frequent Physical Effort 1.99 4.24 ^ 0.07 2.34 0.05 0.00 13.30 * 3.50 3.56 1.13 Frequent Intense Concentration 3.64 2.06 1.74 3.51 0.41 1.15 5.55 ^ 0.75 4.18 ^ 1.87 Frequent Time Pressure 1.92 1.11 9.35 ^ 4.23 ^ 1.88 0.13 1.47 2.50 0.10 0.97 Hours Working w/ Hands 1.13 4.12 ^ 4.90 ^ 13.25 * 0.26 0.12 8.08 ^ 1.56 0.01 4.50 ^ Hours Dealing w/ People 2.98 1.09 0.72 0.31 0.30 0.16 1.80 12.59 * 0.12 0.61 Hours Doing Repetitive Work 4.51 ^ 1.38 9.49 ^ 13.53 * 0.02 2.58 1.65 2.13 0.08 1.66 Ever Gets Dirty 0.94 2.69 2.04 7.63 ^ 0.34 0.27 5.50 ^ 0.71 0.00 0.00 Exposure to Disease Agents 0.23 1.00 1.99 3.17 0.09 0.29 0.26 9.58 ^ 0.02 0.93 Exposure to Dangerous Equip. 1.16 6.52 ^ 3.00 16.21 ** 3.40 0.03 8.28 ^ 3.95 ^ 2.06 0.04 Exposure to Hazardous Chem. 3.00 2.47 0.50 1.77 3.97 ^ 0.02 3.25 0.06 2.45 0.05

^ p < 0.05 using conventional chi-squared criteria. * p < 0.05 using conventional chi-squared criteria, and association is "positive" using Bayesian Inference Criteria (BIC) ** p < 0.05 using conventional chi-squared criteria, and association is "strong" or "very strong" using Bayesian Inference Criteria (BIC) Note: All measures except Group II have one degree of freedom. Group II has 16 degrees of freedom. Table 6. Education, Occupational Characteristics, and Job Characteristics: Effects on Self-Reported Health Problems and Medical Conditions, WOMEN

WOMEN (N = 2,910) Self-Reported Health Problems Self-Reported Medical Conditions

Poor Stiff, Back Arthritis High Serious Circul- Overall Swollen Aching Pain, Trouble or Blood Back ation Health Joints Muscles Strain Sleeping Rheum. Press. Allergies Prob. Prob. Logistic Regression of Outcomes on Years of Education Years of Parameter Estimate 0.86 0.92 0.83 0.89 0.94 0.93 0.89 1.08 0.95 0.92 Education Model Chi-Square 20.04 ** 6.89 ^ 27.25 ** 11.30 * 2.50 9.18 ^ 18.23 ** 9.04 ^ 2.19 3.79 Improvement in Model Chi-Square by Adding Socioeconomic Occupational Characteristics Occupational Prestige 2.92 0.25 1.31 0.00 0.04 0.92 2.65 0.32 0.33 0.67 Occupational Education 1.75 0.80 3.71 0.53 1.62 1.44 2.90 0.06 1.44 1.64 Occupational Earnings 0.28 0.41 0.42 0.43 0.09 1.62 2.96 1.06 2.00 0.23 Group II Categories 19.63 21.04 26.68 ^ 10.09 19.58 22.64 13.26 14.82 22.94 31.62 ^ Improvement in Model Chi-Square by Adding Injury-Related Occupational Characteristics

Total Occupational Injury Rate 1.09 3.51 7.66 ^ 0.97 1.84 3.25 1.69 2.18 1.07 0.03 Object-Related Occ. Injury Rate 2.92 3.80 8.35 ^ 0.33 1.13 4.23 ^ 3.61 0.46 0.58 1.56 Slip or Fall Occ. Injury Rate 0.19 0.80 4.24 ^ 0.51 0.39 1.07 0.78 1.87 1.03 0.46 Overexertion Occ. Injury Rate 1.70 5.98 ^ 11.17 * 1.70 2.14 4.13 ^ 2.64 0.95 1.82 0.28 Repetition Occ. Injury Rate 1.06 0.10 2.42 0.01 3.00 0.06 1.49 0.00 0.20 1.15 Exposure Occ. Injury Rate 1.63 3.63 6.08 ^ 1.35 0.78 6.74 ^ 5.64 ^ 0.18 2.71 2.25 Improvement in Model Chi-Square by Adding Job Characteristics

Base Hourly Wage Rate 13.50 * 5.01 ^ 3.54 4.30 ^ 2.73 1.26 1.97 0.05 2.79 3.14 College is Typical of Jobholders 0.09 0.02 2.05 2.74 8.80 ^ 6.17 ^ 0.12 1.02 2.99 3.57 Frequent Physical Effort 3.64 2.49 22.49 ** 9.64 ^ 1.62 3.72 6.13 ^ 1.82 12.13 * 11.79 * Frequent Intense Concentration 0.04 0.24 2.32 0.76 6.65 ^ 2.29 1.48 0.69 6.33 ^ 0.01 Frequent Time Pressure 1.00 7.49 ^ 10.67 * 7.63 ^ 10.18 * 0.00 0.05 3.58 9.83 ^ 3.08 Hours Working w/ Hands 3.19 0.01 0.96 0.28 0.24 0.01 0.05 0.27 4.96 ^ 2.32 Hours Dealing w/ People 0.65 2.09 3.83 ^ 0.01 0.45 0.66 1.74 0.76 0.00 0.28 Hours Doing Repetitive Work 6.87 ^ 0.71 3.91 ^ 2.22 3.52 3.53 0.84 0.77 7.62 ^ 4.04 ^ Ever Gets Dirty 1.66 4.24 ^ 16.80 ** 3.82 0.06 13.35 * 5.99 ^ 1.44 4.60 ^ 19.08 ** Exposure to Disease Agents 8.98 ^ 1.15 4.99 ^ 1.28 1.06 0.86 1.06 2.95 0.09 0.01 Exposure to Dangerous Equip. 0.63 2.10 0.19 9.31 ^ 0.43 2.82 2.22 0.09 0.06 0.01 Exposure to Hazardous Chem. 1.56 3.88 ^ 2.93 0.04 4.29 ^ 4.90 ^ 0.85 0.20 2.03 2.57

^ p < 0.05 using conventional chi-squared criteria. * p < 0.05 using conventional chi-squared criteria, and association is "positive" using Bayesian Inference Criteria (BIC) ** p < 0.05 using conventional chi-squared criteria, and association is "strong" or "very strong" using Bayesian Inference Criteria (BIC) Note: All measures except Group II have one degree of freedom. Group II has 16 degrees of freedom.