Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

Health Professions Education ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

www.elsevier.com/locate/hpe

Mastery of skills. Does matter? Jeroen Kuntzea,n, Henk T. van der Molenb, Marise Ph. Bornc aDepartment of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Open University, The Netherlands bFaculty of Social Sciences, Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands cDepartment of Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands Received 14 July 2016; accepted 1 August 2016

Abstract

Background: Insight in the influence of intelligence on the mastery of communication skills is important for improving the microcounselling method, an effective training programme for acquiring these skills. Method: Participants were 323 bachelor psychology students. The participants' level of verbal, spatial and numerical intelligence was determined. Participants followed either a course in basic skills or a course in advanced skills. Their level of mastery of these skills was assessed with a video test. Results: Both training programmes proved to be effective in training communication skills. As expected, numerical and spatial intelligence were no significant predictors for the mastery of these skills. Verbal intelligence did matter for the mastery of basic communication skills, but only when students are not acquainted with the use of these skills. Discussion: Students profit from training these skills, regardless of their intelligence level. & 2016 King Saud bin AbdulAziz University for Health Sciences. Production and Hosting by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Keywords: Communication skills; Assessment; Intelligence; Training; Education

1. Introduction acquisition of these skills is microcounselling.2-5 Sev- eral studies6-13 and meta-analyses14-17 found large For many psychologists professional interviewing is overall effect sizes for this method. Improving this one of the key components of their profession. There- method, however, still remains important. For instance, fore psychology curricula offer their students training the training method was adjusted to the use of new programmes in communication skills. Moreover, the computer techniques.18-19 In the context of improving mastery of these skills is one of the requirements to be this training method it is also important to gain insight considered for the diploma of a registered European into factors that might influence the acquisition of psychologist.1 A successful training method for the communication skills. One such a factor might be intelligence. nCorresponding author. Postal address: Studiecentrum Utrecht, Nowadays there is reasonable consensus that intelli- Vondellaan 202, 3521 GZ Utrecht, The Netherlands. gence is the best predictor of work performance, based þ Fax: 31 30 2518254. on the outcomes of several longitudinal studies20-27 and E-mail address: [email protected] (J. Kuntze). 28-30 Peer review under responsibility of AMEEMR: The Association meta-analyses. General intelligence predicts, apart 31 for Medical Education in the Eastern Mediterranean Region from working performance, also training success. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hpe.2016.08.002 2452-3011/& 2016 King Saud bin AbdulAziz University for Health Sciences. Production and Hosting by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Please cite this article as: Kuntze J, et al. Mastery of communication skills. Does intelligence matter? Health Professions Education (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hpe.2016.08.002 2 J. Kuntze et al. / Health Professions Education ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] Students and soldiers scoring high on general intelli- unravelled into small meaningful communication skills. gence tests learned more from the same training Following Ivey and Authier4 and Egan,39,40 Lang and programme than persons scoring low on general Van der Molen38 distinguished seven basic commu- intelligence. This finding was confirmed by other nication skills, namely minimal encouragements, ask- studies21,32 and a meta-analysis.30 Apart from being ing questions, paraphrasing, reflection of feeling, an unsurpassed predictor of work performance, general concreteness, summarizing and situation clarification intelligence also is the best predictor of training and five advanced skills, namely advanced accurate success. empathy, confrontation, positive relabelling, examples Therefore, one would expect intelligence also to of one's own and directness. predict success in communication skills training. How- These skills fit within Egan's helping model39,40 of ever, studies investigating whether intelligence predicts three stages: (1) problem clarification, (2) gaining new success in communication skills training are scarce. insights and (3) strategies for treatment. The goal of the Nonetheless, the possible effect of first stage is to clarify the problem of the client. In this intelligence on the mastery of these skills could help stage helpers mainly use the basic communication improve training programmes for the acquisition of skills. During the second stage, gaining new insights, these skills. Therefore, the focus of this study is on helpers try to have clients gain new insights in their intelligence as predictor of the mastery of communica- situation, predominantly using the five advanced com- tion skills after a basic and advanced training in these munication skills. Finally, in the third stage, strategies skills. for treatment, client and helper seek strategies to solve After Spearman's distinction between general intelli- the client's problems. gence or the g-factor and specific intelligence or the s- The basic communication skills are often taught in factor,33,34 intelligence research primarily has concen- the first or second bachelor year of a psychology trated on tests for general intelligence, because this was curriculum and the advanced communication skills in to be the best indicator of someone's true consecutive years. Subjects of the present study were intelligence.21 However, since the second half of the psychology students who in their first bachelor year 20th century researchers also attended to more specific followed a course in basic communication skills and in abilities, for instance verbal intelligence or spatial their second bachelor year a course in advanced intelligence. Specific job demands, e.g. bookkeeping, communication skills. These courses were built up not only appeal to general intelligence, but also to according to the Cumulative Microtraining (CMT) specific abilities, e.g. numerical intelligence.21,24 method.41 In this method students first receive theore- According to Lubinski24 general intelligence can be tical instruction about one communication skill (e.g. divided into three more specific ability domains: summarizing) and its function in a professional inter- verbal, numerical and spatial intelligence. The aggrega- view. Next, video clips show examples of inadequate tion of these three separate abilities should give a good and adequate performance of the skill to the students impression of someone's general intelligence. In this (modelling). Subsequently, students exercise the skill study we will maintain Lubinski's distinction and separately (like ‘dry swimming’). For instance, with investigate verbal, numerical and spatial intelligence regard to the basic skill ‘asking questions’ students as predictors of the mastery of communication skills. have to try to change a closed question (‘Do you feel Only a few studies have investigated these relation- sad?’) into an open question (‘How do you feel?’), after ships. These studies have concentrated mostly on the which they practice the skill in a role-play with another correlation of verbal intelligence with communication student. Finally, fellow-students and the trainer give skills. For instance, verbal intelligence admission tests feedback on the student's performance in the role-play predicted medical students' mastery of communication and the student formulates points according to skills.35 Well-developed verbal intelligence at early age this feedback. While students practice one or two new predicts well-developed communication and in each session, CMT's ultimate purpose is to skills later on in life.36 Finally, found that students integrate the communication skills that have been dealt scoring high on verbal intelligence also scored higher with up to that moment in the training programme. on behavioural tests for communication skills.37 Both courses aim at increasing the students' adequate Students in this study followed a microcounselling application of the (basic or advanced) communication training programme in communication skills, also skills. known as microskills.2-5,38 This means that the com- According to the studies that found positive relation- plex skill of professionally interviewing a client is ships between verbal intelligence and communication

Please cite this article as: Kuntze J, et al. Mastery of communication skills. Does intelligence matter? Health Professions Education (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hpe.2016.08.002 J. Kuntze et al. / Health Professions Education ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 3 skills,35-37 we expect verbal intelligence to predict the The test takes 30 minutes and for this test the authors mastery of communication skills. Furthermore, we report a stability coefficient of .87 and a split-half expect persons scoring high on verbal intelligence to reliability of .73. show more progress in the level of mastery of com- munication skills after training these skills than person 2.2.3. Test for Non-Verbal (TNVA) scoring low on verbal intelligence.21,30-32 Finally, we The TNVA42 assesses spatial intelligence. It consists expect that spatial and numerical intelligence do not of 40 items. Each item shows four figures with a sort of predict the mastery of communication skills, since there resemblance. The participant has to choose, out of six seems to be no logical relationship between these other figures, those two that are most similar to the first intelligence factors and mastery of communication four figures. Participants have 20 min to answer the skills. items. The items gradually increase in difficulty. For this test the authors found a stability coefficient of .81 2. Method and a split-half reliability of .77.

2.1. Participants 2.2.4. Counselling Communication Skills Progress Test (CSPT) A total of 323 bachelor students of a psychology The counselling communication skills progress curriculum at a large Dutch university participated in test11-13 is a video based behavioural test, established this study; 182 participants were in their first year of according to the results of Smit and Van der Molen,43- the bachelor programme, 141 were in the second year 45 who investigated different methods for the assess- of that programme. The average age of the first year ment of communication skills. The CSPT assesses the students was 19.5 (SD¼2,1) and 80% of them were participants’ level of mastery of the communication female. The second year students’ average age was skills. It has forty-two items. Each item is a video clip 20.3 (SD¼1.8) and 81% were female. in which a client, played by an actor, tells something concerning his or her problem. After watching each 2.2. Instruments clip participants write down literally what they would say to the client, having received the instruction to use The participants' verbal, numerical and spatial intel- a communication skill (e.g. ‘Give a paraphrase in an ligence were measured with three tests that are part of appropriate manner’). the Drenth Test Series Higher Level,42 a Dutch The first 30 items assess the level of mastery of the intelligence test battery often used in the Netherlands. seven basic communication skills and the next twelve The participants' mastery of the basic and advanced items the level of mastery of the five advanced communication skills was assessed with the Counsel- communication skills. The number of items for the ling Communication Skills Progress Test (CSPT).11–13 assessment of the basic skills is higher than the number of items for the assessment of the advanced skills, 2.2.1. Verbal Analogies (VA) because there are more basic skills than advanced skills The VA42 is a verbal analogies test and assesses and because the assessment of an advanced skill takes verbal intelligence. It consists of 40 multiple choice more time than the assessment of a basic skill. items and measures to what extent participants can Three trained assessors using an instruction guide with words. They have to determine how words developed for the assessment of the application by the are related, for instance ‘… is to calf, as horse is to…’. students of every communication skill evaluated the Participants have 40 minutes for answering the 40 participants' answers. This guide is based on the skill items. The authors report a split-half reliability of .87 definitions in Lang and Van der Molen38 and on expert and a KR-20 of .77. counsellors’’ specifications for the requirements of an adequate answer, a moderately adequate answer and an 2.2.2. Numeric series (NS) inadequate answer. Students acquired two points for an Numerical series42 estimates the participants’ numer- adequate answer, one point for a moderately adequate ical intelligence. It has 26 items, each presenting a answer and zero points for an inadequate answer. In the series of numbers. The participant has to choose from end, mean scores can be calculated on the Basic Skill four numbers, which number logically follows the last Factor (BSF) and the Advanced Skill Factor (ASF), the number of the series. For instance: 1, 3, 6, 10, 15, 21, ? two underlying dimensions of the CSPT. The range of Possible answers: 26, 27, 28, 29, 30. the scores on these two dimensions is 0–2.

Please cite this article as: Kuntze J, et al. Mastery of communication skills. Does intelligence matter? Health Professions Education (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hpe.2016.08.002 4 J. Kuntze et al. / Health Professions Education ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]]

Table 1 in advanced communication skills the mean scores of Mean scores on the CSPT of freshmen (no training), 1st year students the first year students on the Advanced Skills Factor at (training basic skills) and second year students (training advanced T1 were compared with the mean scores of the second skills). year students on this factor at T2. The mean scores on Freshmen (T0) 1st year students (T1) 2nd year students (T2) the CSPT of all students are displayed in Table 1. n¼182 n¼182 n¼141 The results in Table 1 show that first year students score higher on both the BSF and the ASF at T1 in BSF .45 1.41 1.57 ASF .09 .19 .87 comparison with their scores on these factors when they were freshmen at T0. These within-subjects BSF Mean score on the Basic Skill Factor (range 0–2). differences on both the BSF and the ASF were – ASF Mean score on the Advanced Skill Factor (range 0 2). significant across time, respectively F (1, 181)¼ 1779.8; po.001 and F (1, 140)¼33.1; p o.001. In order to control for testing effects two comparable Having followed the basic communication skills versions of the CSPT were developed. There were no training first year students improved most on the Basic significant differences in the students' scores between Skill Factor. The effect size (d) was 4.43. This is a large these two versions. Cronbach's α for both versions was effect according to Cohen.46,47 After this course first high (.91 and .92) and interrater-reliabilities ranged year students showed moderate progress on the between .89 and .97.11,13 Finally, age and gender had Advanced Skill Factor, d¼.6, but this is still significant. no significant influence on the CSPT-scores. Furthermore, the mean scores in Table 1 reveal that second year students score higher on both factors than 2.3. Procedure first year students. These between-subjects differences on the BSF and the ASF were significant, respectively For this study we used data of 182 first year students T (327)¼5.9; po.001 and T (327)¼22.8; who took the three intelligence tests in their first year of po.001. Second year students score highest on the the bachelor psychology curriculum. They received the BSF. However, the highest mean difference between CSPT two times, first, at the start of their study first year and second year students is on the ASF, psychology as freshmen, roughly three weeks after the which leads to an effect size (d) of 2.5, which is a large start of the academic year (T0) and second, six months effect. The mean difference on the BSF between first later in their first year, just after they had followed a year and second year students is to be considered as a course in basic communication skills (T1). Furthermore, moderate effect size (d) of .6. we gathered data of 141 second year students who took Next, regression analyses were performed to inves- the three intelligence tests in their first year of the tigate whether verbal, numerical and spatial intelligence bachelor psychology curriculum. They received the CSPT could predict the mastery of the basic and advanced after six months in their second year, when they just had skills of the first year students at T0 and T1 and finished a course in advanced communication skills (T2). whether these factors could predict success in training The basic communication skills training consists of five basic communication skills. Because of the high sessions, spread over five weeks and the advanced number of regression analyses a significance level of communication skill training has four sessions, spread .01 was used. Table 2 shows the results. over four weeks. The first year and second year students As can be seen from Table 2 only the standardized took the CSPT as a formal examination after respectively regression weight of verbal intelligence on the BSF at the course in basic communication skills and the course in T0 was significant (β¼.28; p¼.001). All other stan- advanced communication skills. Minimal scores to pass dardized regression weights of verbal, numerical and the examination were determined for both groups. spatial intelligence on the BSF and the ASF at T0 and T1 were not significant. This means that from the three 3. Results specific intelligence factors only verbal intelligence influenced the mastery of communication skills, but First, the training effects of both courses in commu- this influence applied to the basic communication skills nication skills were determined. To calculate the effect alone and before the first year students received any size of the course in basic communication skills the training in these skills. Having received the course in mean scores of the first year students on the Basic Skill basic communication skills verbal intelligent students Factor at T0 and T1 were analysed using a repeated did not score higher or lower than less verbal intelligent measures analysis. To evaluate the effect of the course students. This seems to imply that less verbal

Please cite this article as: Kuntze J, et al. Mastery of communication skills. Does intelligence matter? Health Professions Education (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hpe.2016.08.002 J. Kuntze et al. / Health Professions Education ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 5

Table 2 Table 3 Regression coefficients of verbal, spatial and numerical intelligence Regression coefficients of verbal, spatial and numerical intelligence as predictors of 1st year students' scores on the Basic Skill Factor and as predictors of 2nd year students' scores on the Basic Skill Factor on the Advanced Skill Factor (N¼182). and on the Advanced Skill Factor (N¼141).

Verbal Intelligence Numerical Spatial Verbal Intelligence Numerical Spatial Intelligence Intelligence Intelligence or ‘g’ Intelligence or ‘g’

β p β p β p β p β p β p

BSF0 .28* .001 .17 .031 .06 .497 BSF2 .01 .964 .01 .899 .19 .036 BSF1 .18 .049 .08 .305 .01 .941 ASF2 .18 .065 .13 .122 .08 .389 ASF0 .08 .379 .08 .338 .01 .919 ASF1 .18 .055 .08 .333 .01 .921 BSF2¼Score on Basic Skill Factor at T2. BSF .06 .491 .06 .463 .05 .592 ASF2¼Score on Advanced Skill Factor at T2. T1-T0 ASF .12 .205 .10 .214 .01 .898 T1-T0 4. Discussion

BSF0¼Score on Basic Skill Factor at T0. BSF1¼Score on Basic Skill Factor at T1. The main aim of this study was to investigate ASF0¼Score on Advanced Skill Factor at T0. whether the mastery level of communication skills ASF1¼Score on Advanced Skill Factor at T1. and progress in mastery of those skills after commu- BSF T1–T0¼Contrast scores on Basic Skill Factor between T1 nication skills training could be predicted by verbal, and T0. numerical and spatial intelligence. It was expected that ASFT1–T0¼Contrast scores on Advanced Skill Factor between T1 and T0. verbal intelligence would positively, and that numerical *po.01. and spatial intelligence would not relate to the level of mastery and training success in these skills. intelligent students gained more of the course in basic First, the effect sizes of a basic communication skills communication skills than verbal intelligent students. and an advanced communication skills microcounsel- However, the results displayed in the last two rows of ling training programme were determined. A large Table 2 reveal that neither verbal intelligence nor effect size was found for the course in basic commu- numerical or spatial intelligence predicted the contrast nication skills with respect to the mastery level of these scores between T0 and T1 significantly. So, as skills. On the mastery level of the advanced skills this expected, success in communication skills training course had a moderate effect. The course in advanced was not influenced by numerical or spatial intelligence. communication skills showed a large effect on the level However, contrary to our expectation, it also was not of these skills and a moderate effect on the basic predicted by verbal intelligence. communication skills. Although in this study no con- Finally, regression analyses were performed to trol group and, with respect to the course in advanced investigate if the second year students’ scores on the skills, no pre-test was used, these findings correspond BSF and ASF could be predicted by verbal, spatial or with results found in other studies that did use such numerical intelligence after following the course in designs7,8,11-13 and with effect sizes reported by several advanced communication skills. The results are dis- meta-analyses.14-17 Therefore, it can be concluded that played in Table 3. Again, since the number of regres- the course in basic communication skills and the course sion analyses is high, a significance level of .01 in advanced communication skills have positive effects was used. on the students' mastery level of these skills. Table 3 shows that verbal, numerical and spatial Second, the relationship between verbal, numerical intelligence did neither significantly predict the scores and spatial intelligence and the mastery level of com- on the Basic Skill Factor nor on the Advanced Skill munication skills was investigated. As expected, numer- Factor. Standardized regression weights ranged from ical and spatial intelligence did neither predict the .13 (numerical intelligence on ASF2; p¼.12) to .19 mastery level of basic or advanced communication (spatial intelligence on BSF2; p¼.04). So, verbal, skills, nor progress in the mastery of these skills. Verbal numerical and spatial intelligence did not influence intelligence, however, did predict the mastery level of the mastery of basic and advanced communication basic communication skills, but only when students had skills of second year students after a course in not received any training in these skills. Yet, after the advanced communication skills. course in basic or advanced communication skills, verbal

Please cite this article as: Kuntze J, et al. Mastery of communication skills. Does intelligence matter? Health Professions Education (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hpe.2016.08.002 6 J. Kuntze et al. / Health Professions Education ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] intelligence did not influence the mastery level of basic study motivation could moderate the relationship communication skills anymore. Verbal intelligence did between intelligence and the acquisition of these skills. neither predict success in training basic or advanced communication skills, nor did it predict the mastery level Disclosure of the advanced communication skills before or after the course in basic or advanced communication skills. So, Ethical approval: Non Applicable. the results seem to imply that verbal intelligence is of Funding: None. some importance in the application of basic commu- Other disclosure: None. nication skills, but only before a professional training in these skills. In other words, verbal intelligence seems to References matter in unfamiliar situations, as applying basic com- munication skills is an unfamiliar situation for freshmen 1. EuroPsy-the European Diploma in Psychology. European Fed- when they are tested on their pre-training basic level. eration of Psychology Associations. 〈http://www.efpa.be/doc/ 〉 This corresponds with the idea that intelligence is EuroPsyJune%202006.pdf Published June 20, 2006. Retrieved ‘ ’48 Sept 26, 2007. adaptability to new situations or the ability to 2. Daniels T, Ivey AE, editors. Microcounseling: making skills ‘respond to new situations which [people] have not training work in a multicultural world. Springfield, IL: Charles 24 practiced’. However, when students do practice basic C. Thomas; 2007. communication skills the influence of verbal intelligence 3. Ivey AE. Microcounseling. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas; disappears. An explanation for the fact that not a similar 1971. 4. Ivey AE, Authier J, editors. Microcounseling: Innovations in sort of pattern was found with regard to the mastery interviewing, counseling, psychotherapy and psychoeducation. level of advanced communication skills - verbal intelli- Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas; 1978. gence did not predict the pre-training mastery level of 5. Ivey AE, Bradford-Ivey M. Intentional interviewing and coun- advanced communication skills - might be that applying seling: facilitating client development in a multicultural society, these skills is a complex matter. The low means on the 6th ed., Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole; 2007. 6. Bögels SM. Teaching and assessing diagnostic interviewing performance of these skills before and after the course in skills. An application to the mental health field. The Netherlands: basic communication skills seem to correspond with this University of Limburg; 1994. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. idea. Finally, verbal intelligence did not predict progress 7. Evans B, Coman S, Burrows G. Psychological tests to measure in the mastery of basic communication skills after the effects of medical education on student’s personal skills. Med – following a course in these skills. From an educational Educ 1989;23(6):492 497. fi 8. Evans B, Sweet B, Coman G. Behavioral assessment of the point of view this can be regarded as a positive nding. effectiveness of a communication program for medical students. It seems that students, regardless of their level of verbal Med Educ 1993;27(4):344–350. intelligence, profit from the basic communication skills 9. Gask L. Small group interactive techniques utilizing video training programme. feedback. Int J Psychiatr Med 1998;28(1):97–113. 10. Toriello P, Strohmer D. Addiction counselor’s credibility: The impact of interactional style, recovery status, and non-verbal 4.1. Limitation of the study behavior. J Addict Offender Couns 2004;25(1):43–57. 11. Kuntze J, Van der Molen HT. Born MPh. Progress in mastery of An important limitation of this study lies in the counseling communication skills. Development and evaluation of homogeneity of the group of participants. It consisted a new instrument for the assessment of counseling communica- – of academic psychology students only. Possibly, using tion skills. Eur Psychol 2007;12(4):301 313. 12. Kuntze J, Van der Molen HT, Born MPh. Increase in counselling a more heterogeneous sample with regard to the level communication skills after basic and advanced microskills of verbal intelligence, for instance by including stu- training. Br J Educ Psychol 2009:175–188. dents from lower and higher vocational studies, could 13. Kuntze J, Van der Molen HT, Born MPh. Assessing progress in show more influence of verbal intelligence on the level mastery of counselling communication skills. Saarbrücken: VDM of mastery in communication skills. Verlag Dr. Müller; 2009. 14. Baker SB, Daniels TG. Integrating research on the microcounsel- ing program – a meta analysis. J Couns Psychol 1989;36(2): 4.2. Future research 213–222. 15. Baker SB, Daniels TG, Greeley AT. Systematic training of Future research should reiterate this study and use a graduate-level counselors – narrative and meta-analytic reviews – more heterogeneous sample. Further, it is recom- of 3 major programs. Couns Psychol 1990;18(3):355 421. 16. Daniels T. A review of research on microcounseling: 1967 – mended to investigate whether other variables are present. In: Ivey AE, Ivey MB, editors. Intentional interviewing possible moderators between intelligence and the and counseling: Your interactive resource CD-ROM. Pacific mastery level of communication skills. For instance, Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole; 2003.

Please cite this article as: Kuntze J, et al. Mastery of communication skills. Does intelligence matter? Health Professions Education (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hpe.2016.08.002 J. Kuntze et al. / Health Professions Education ] (]]]]) ]]]–]]] 7

17. Van der Molen HT, Smit GN, Hommes MA, Lang G. Two 33. Spearman C. General intelligence: objectively determined and decades of cumulative microtraining in The Netherlands: an measured. Am J Psychol 1904;15:201–293. overview. Educ Res Eval 1995;1(4):347–378. 34. Spearman C. The thing of two factors. Psychol Rev 1914;21: 18. Schönrock-Adema J. De ontwikkeling en evaluatie van een 101–115. zelfinstructieprogramma voor een training in basisgespreksvaar- 35. Kulatunga-Moruzi C, Norman G. Validity of admissions mea- digheden. [The development and evaluation of a self-instruction sures in predicting performance outcomes: the contribution of program for a training in basic communication skills] Ph. D. cognitive and non-cognitive dimensions. Teach Learn Med Dissertation. Groningen: Rijksuniversiteit; 2002. 2002;14:34–42. 19. Van der Molen HT, Hommes MA, Hulshof C, De Jong M, 36. Chiat S, Roy P. Early phonological and sociocognitive skills as Kuntze J, Mayer BM, Osseweijer E, Pieters J, Schmidt HG, predictors of and social communication outcomes. J Soppe M. Zelfinstructieprogramma’s voor de Basisaantekening Child Psychol Psychiatr 2008;49(6):635–645. Psychodiagnostiek. [Self instruction programs for the Basic 37. Smit GN. De Beoordeling van Professionele Gespreksvaardigh- Certificate Psychodiagnostics] Suppl. 43, p. 7.4-10.01-7.4- eden. Constructie en evaluatie van rollenspel-, video- en 10.13. In: Schramde PWJ, editor. Handboek Effectief Opleiden, schriftelijke toetsen. [The Assessment of Professional Commu- [Manual Effective Training]. Den Haag: Reed Business Informa- nication Skills. Construction and evaluation of simulations, tion; 2007. video- and paper and-pencil tests]. Baarn, The Netherlands: H. 20. Gottfredson LS. Why g matters: the complexity of everyday life. Nelissen; 1995. – Intelligence 1997;24:79 132. 38. Lang G, Van der Molen HT. Psychologische Gespreksvoering. 21. Gottfredson LS. Intelligence. In: Borgatta EF, Montgomery (red) Een basis voor hulpverlening. [Psychological Communication. A Encyclopedia of sociology 2 RJV, editors. , . Macmillian Refer- basis for the helping process]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: ence USA; 2000. p. 1359–1386. Uitgeverij Boom/Nelissen; 2012. 22. Keating P. A search for . J Educ Psychol 39. Egan G. The skilled helper, a model for systematic and 1987;70:218–223. interpersonal relating. Montery, California: Brooks/Cole Pub- 23. Lubinski D. Assessing individual differences in human behavior: lishing Company; 1975. sinking shafts at a few critical points. Annu Rev Psychol 2000;51: 40. Egan G. The skilled helper: a problem-management approach to 405–444. helping. Pacific Grove, California: Brooks/Cole Publishing 24. Lubinski D. Introduction to the special section on cognitive Company; 1994. abilities: 100 years after Spearman's (1904) "'General Intelli- 41. Lang G, Van der Molen HT. Methodiek van Gespreks training gence, 'Objectively Determined and Measured". J Pers Soc [Methodology of communication skills training]. Baarn, The Psychol 2004;86:96–111. Netherlands: H. Nelissen; 1992. 25. Murphy KR. Is the relationship between cognitive ability and job 42. Drenth PJD, Van Wieringen PCW, Hoolwerf G. Drenth Testserie performance stable over time?. Hum Perform 1989;2:183–200. 26. Schmidt FL, Hunter JE. General mental ability in the world of Hoger Niveau: NAT, VAT, TNVA. Handleiding. [Drenth Test- work: occupational attainment and job performance. J Pers Soc series Higher Level: NAT, VAT, TNVA. Professional manual]. Psychol 2004;86:162–173. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2001. 27. Thorndike RL. The central role of general ability in prediction. 43. Smit GN, Van der Molen HT. De constructie en evaluatie van Multivar Behav Res 1985;20:241–254. een videotoets voor de beoordeling van bekwaamheid in 28. Hunter J, Hunter R. Validity and utility of alternative predictors professionele gespreksvoering. [The construction and evaluation of job performance. Psychol Bull 1984;96:72–98. of a video test for the assessment of mastery in professional 29. Kuncel NR, Hezlett SA, Ones DS. Academic performance, career communication]. Tijdschr voor Onderwijsres [(Dutch) J Educ potential, and job performance: can one construct Res] 1996;21:182–190. predict them all?. J Pers Soc Psychol 2004;12:301–313. 44. Smit GN, Van der Molen HT. Three methods for the assessment 30. Schmidt FL, Hunter JE. The validity and utility of selection of communication skills. Br J Educ Psychol 1996;66(4): methods in personnel psychology: practical and theoretical 543–555. implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychol Bull 45. Smit GN, Van der Molen HT. Toetsvormen voor de beoordeling 1998;124:262–274. van professionele gespreksvaardigheden. [Different tests for the 31. Fox WL, Taylor JE, Caylor JS. Aptitude Level and the Acquisi- assessment of professional communication skills]. Nederlands tion of Skills and in a Variety of Military Training Tijdschr voor Psychol [(Dutch) J Psychol] 1996;51:154–167. Tasks (Technical Report 69-6). Prepared for the Department of 46. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. the Army by the Human Resources Research Office. Washing- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. ton, D.C.: The George Washington University; 1969. 47. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull 1992;112(1):155–159. 32. Ree MJ, Earles JA. Predicting training success: not much more 48. Cattell RB. Intelligence: its structure, growth and action. than g. Pers Psychol 1991;44:321–332. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; 1987.

Please cite this article as: Kuntze J, et al. Mastery of communication skills. Does intelligence matter? Health Professions Education (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.hpe.2016.08.002