Ego Depletion Induces Mental Passivity: Behavioral Effects Beyond Impulse Control

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Ego Depletion Induces Mental Passivity: Behavioral Effects Beyond Impulse Control Motivation Science © 2017 American Psychological Association 2017, Vol. 3, No. 4, 321–336 2333-8113/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/mot0000058 Ego Depletion Induces Mental Passivity: Behavioral Effects Beyond Impulse Control Andrew J. Vonasch Kathleen D. Vohs University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill University of Minnesota Anastasiya Pocheptsova Ghosh Roy F. Baumeister University of Arizona Florida State University Three studies supported the hypothesis that people can become mentally and physically passive when resources are depleted by prior acts of self-control. Feeling depleted and recent self-regulatory exertion were associated with preferences for passive behaviors like resting and watching TV. Participants who had to maintain attention in the face of distractions preferred to avoid making consumer decisions as compared with partici- pants who did not. Breaking a habit caused hungry participants to eat more peanuts when doing so was easy (the peanuts lacked shells) and to eat less when eating required minor preparatory action (the peanuts had shells). Mental passivity induced by deple- tion of self-control causes both passive behavior and impulsive behavior, depending on the situation. Keywords: self-regulation, passivity, ego depletion The difference between active and passive status quo. In other words, passive responding responding is one of the most fundamental di- means letting things happen and not intervening mensions among the countless ways that living to change things. creatures can respond to environmental events. The view of self-regulation as an effortful ac- Active responding means that the creature re- tivity that depletes an energy resource has stimu- acts to the external circumstances by seeking to lated much research (for meta-analysis, see Hag- steer the course of events in one rather than ger, Wood, Stiff, & Chatzisarantis, 2010; for another direction. In contrast, passive respond- recent overview of theoretical disputes and issues, ing involves allowing events to unfurl without see Baumeister & Vohs, 2016). The underlying attempting to control the outcome or alter the assumption has been that a particular resource, akin to the folk notion of willpower, is expended in regulating behavior such as by inhibiting im- pulses. The present paper tests an extension to this This article was published Online First July 13, 2017. theory. Moving beyond resisting temptation, it Andrew J. Vonasch, Department of Psychology and Neu- tests the idea that the difference between active roscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; and passive responding involves the expenditure Kathleen D. Vohs, Carlson School of Management, Univer- of this same resource. We reasoned that top-down sity of Minnesota; Anastasiya Pocheptsova Ghosh, Eller This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. College of Management, University of Arizona; Roy F. control takes energy and will decline when regu- This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individualBaumeister, user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Department of Psychology, Florida State Uni- latory resources are low. This may result in in- versity. creased inaction insofar as action requires initia- This research was supported in part by Land O’ Lakes tive, but it may also produce increased behavior Excellence in Marketing funds and a grant from the John insofar as inhibitions or stopping the behavior Templeton Foundation. We thank Korey Jones, Katie Lewis, and Marco Romero for data collection and Colin DeYoung for requires top-down control. helpful comments. Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- Passivity dressed to Andrew J. Vonasch, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, 235 East, Cameron Avenue, Chapel Hill, NC Living things vary widely in their capacity 27599. E-mail: [email protected] for active response. It is arguable that plants 321 322 VONASCH, VOHS, POCHEPTSOVA GHOSH, AND BAUMEISTER have almost no active responses. As a revealing expensive form of control is curtailed, to con- example, they remain inactive even while other serve energy, and this could mean broadly a creatures eat them. Animals, in contrast, move passive attitude toward life and events. If men- to eat and to resist being eaten. Among simple tal passivity is a central aspect of depletion, then animals, passive responding means physical depletion may produce physical passivity, espe- stillness, and active responding means physical cially when there are no automatic impulses to movement. take action, and when action requires exerting In humans, the active versus passive distinc- top-down control (e.g., difficult decisions, or tion occurs at both the physical level (stillness initiative). vs. movement) and at the mental level (relying In sum, our investigation focused on the hy- on automatic vs. controlled processes). Mental pothesis that ego depletion produces mental passivity is the absence of active processing to passivity, which entails an abdication of top- override one’s incipient or automatic responses. down mental control over responses. This could Physical passivity is usually a result of mental produce physical passivity and inaction (as in passivity, such as during rest or sleep. To be Banker, Ainsworth, Baumeister, Ariely, & sure, physical passivity can stem from active Vohs, 2017; Baumeister et al., 1998, Study 4; mental processing, such as when someone tries Giacomantonio, Jordan, Fennis, & Panno, 2014; to hold still while being tickled or injected. Job, Bernecker, Miketta, & Friese, 2015)—but Being mentally passive does not necessarily under some circumstances could yield increases mean one’s body is unmoving. A passive person in physical activity (as in most other depletion might simply follow orders, such as walking to studies summarized in Hagger et al., 2010). a specific place. Mental passivity involves let- ting one’s automatic responses to the environ- Active Versus Passive Mental Processes ment drive one’s behavior. Depending on one’s and Behaviors automatic impulses, mentally passive behavior can be either physically passive or physically Either action or inaction can result from men- active. tal passivity. Carver, Johnson, and Joormann Mentally active versus passive responding (2008) proposed that lack of executive function seemingly goes to the essence of volition. Many control leaves the person responding reflexively human decisions and experiences depend on the to environmental cues. That sort of a mentally difference between active and passive respond- passive response style can theoretically produce ing. A health warning may be passively ignored responses ranging from physical lassitude to or met with an active change of behavior. Peo- disinhibited violence. ple’s moral judgments depend greatly on this The hypothesis that ego depletion can pro- difference. Most people judge actively killing duce mental passivity fits some previous find- someone to be much more reprehensible than ings. For example, judges on parole boards can passively letting someone die, even if the out- make an easy, default decision (to keep a pris- come and motive are the same (Rachels, 2007). oner in prison) or a risky, difficult decision (to Indeed, basic linguistic structure attests to the grant a parole and allow the convict to rejoin importance of the active-passive dimension: ac- society, where he might commit a crime). Dan- tive versus passive verbs can express the same ziger, Levav, and Avnaim-Pesso (2011) found thought often with substantially different mean- that judges granted parole at reasonably high This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. ings (such as “Mistakes were made”). rates when the judges had been rested or fed, This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Depletion has often been understood as trans- but as the day wore on they became increasingly fer of control from conscious to unconscious, likely to favor the default response of denying from central executive to automatic (Hofmann, parole. Likewise, over the course of making Friese, & Strack, 2009). One way of summariz- many decisions while buying a car, buyers in- ing this diverse literature is to suggest that top- creasingly tend to select the standard or default down mental control is generally reduced or option (Levav, Heitmann, Herrmann, & Iyen- even abdicated during ego depletion. The view gar, 2010). Polman and Vohs (2016) confirmed that depletion brings abdication of control fits that decision fatigue is associated with prefer- many findings, such as increased yielding to ence for the status quo option. Banker, Ain- impulse (Hagger et al., 2010). The resource- sworth, Baumeister, Ariely, and Vohs (2017) EGO DEPLETION INDUCES MENTAL PASSIVITY 323 found that even when people’s decisions di- sive responding, in contrast to the more studied verge from the status quo, they diverge less effect of depletion increasing disinhibition. when people are depleted. In making decisions, Although many published studies support the then, depletion increases the tendency to stick claims of the limited resource model, the exis- close to a safe, default response. tence of the ego depletion effect has recently The limited resource model of self-regulation been disputed (Carter, Kofler, Forster, & Mc- suggests that people conserve resources by re- Cullough,
Recommended publications
  • (MA) in Counseling Psychology with a Concentration in Drama Therapy
    Master of Arts (MA) in Counseling Psychology with a concentration in Drama Therapy Program Learning Outcomes – MCP PDT MA Drama Therapy (PDT) – Program Learning Outcomes 1.0 Students will be able to evaluate and apply drama therapy approaches in working with clients. 1.1 Situate drama therapy within a critical historical context focusing on the relationship between dramatic art and healing traditions from various world cultures. 1.2 Evaluate theories and methods of major drama therapy approaches, and ways in which they overlap, diverge, and complement one another. 1.3 Integrate foundational concepts underlying drama therapy and action techniques to support and complement the Recovery Model and other major models of psychotherapy in therapeutic practice. 1.4 Develop drama therapy interventions that address the needs of different clinical populations and age groups in a culturally sensitive manner (taking into account factors of human difference including gender, race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, sexual orientation, gender identification/expression, and spiritual/religious practices). 1.5 Use drama therapy and action methods skillfully as a means of clinical intervention. 1.6 Use a drama therapy framework to assess clients and implement treatment plans, individually and in groups. 1.7 Translate drama therapy concepts and terminology in professional communication (oral and written) that is clear and relevant to both clients and allied professionals. 1.8 Integrate drama therapy and action methods with best practices from verbal, somatic, and expressive arts-based approaches into clinical work with individuals and groups. Masters in Counseling Psychology (MCP)– Clinical Skills 1.0 Students will develop the clinical and personal skills to serve as professional therapists at the MA level.
    [Show full text]
  • SMARTPHONE-USE and ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE How Students' Self-Control and Smartphone-Use Explain Their Academic Performance Eve S
    SMARTPHONE-USE AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE How students’ self-control and smartphone-use explain their academic performance Eve Sarah Troll a*, Malte Friese b, David D. Loschelder a a. Leuphana University of Lüneburg. Universitätsallee 1, 21335 Lüneburg, Germany b. Saarland University, 66123 Saarbrücken, Germany Author Note Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Eve Sarah Troll or David D. Loschelder, Institute of Management & Organization, Leuphana University of Lüneburg, 21335 Lüneburg, Germany. Email: [email protected] or [email protected]. This work was supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research and the European Social Fund [grant number 01PZ16001B]. Declarations of interest: none. November 24th 2020 © 2020, Elsevier. This paper is not the copy of record and may not exactly replicate the final, authoritative version of the article. Please do not copy or cite without authors’ permission. The final article is available via its DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106624. SMARTPHONE-USE AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE 2 Abstract Smartphones cause self-control challenges in people’s everyday lives. Supporting this notion, our studies corroborate that trait self-control is negatively associated (1) with students’ distraction (via smartphones) during their learning endeavors (Study 1, N = 446) and (2) with several aspects of problematic smartphone-use (Study 2, N = 421). Study 3 (N = 106) investigated whether distinct aspects of smartphone-use also account for the link between students’ trait self-control and academic performance. Specifically, we examined (1) smartphone procrastination (i.e., irrational task delays via smartphone), (2) beneficial smartphone habits (placing in a bag [placement habit] or turning the sound off [setting habit]), and (3) the objective amount of smartphone-use (minutes spent on the smartphone [screentime] and times picked up [pickups]).
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology of Irresistible Impulse Jess Spirer
    Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology Volume 33 | Issue 6 Article 3 1943 Psychology of Irresistible Impulse Jess Spirer Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/jclc Part of the Criminal Law Commons, Criminology Commons, and the Criminology and Criminal Justice Commons Recommended Citation Jess Spirer, Psychology of Irresistible Impulse, 33 J. Crim. L. & Criminology 457 (1942-1943) This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Northwestern University School of Law Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology by an authorized editor of Northwestern University School of Law Scholarly Commons. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF IRRESISTIBLE IMPULSE Jess Spirer "It is a fundamental principle of the criminal law that every" crime, either common law or statutory, with the exception of public nuisances and breaches of what are commonly called police regula- tions, includes a mental element.' 2 We know this mental element by diverse names,-mens rea, criminal intent, vicious will, guilty mind, and so forth-but its most important characteristic is a wilfulness to commit the crime or act in question. And when this wilfulness or volition is actually or constructively absent, there is generally no crime. Of the several conditions which negative the element of volition or criminal intent, probably the best known is that of insanity. In the case of the grossly insane, the standards of responsibility seem to be fairly well defined, for as a general rule the law holds that if the misdoer does not know right from wrong with respect to his particular act, then he is not to be held accountable for what he has done.
    [Show full text]
  • Knowledge, Belief, and the Impulse to Natural Theology
    Science in Context 20(3), 381–400 (2007). Copyright C Cambridge University Press doi:10.1017/S0269889707001354 Printed in the United Kingdom Introduction: Knowledge, Belief, and the Impulse to Natural Theology Fernando Vidal Max Planck Institute for the History of Science, Berlin Bernhard Kleeberg Justus-Liebig-Universitat¨ Giessen The title of this issue of Science in Context – “Believing Nature, Knowing God” – is intended to suggest the moral, emotional, and cognitive conditions in which the historical alliance of “nature” and “God” operated, and to make a more general point about knowing and believing. The production of scientific knowledge includes mechanisms for bringing about acceptance that such knowledge is true, and thus for generating a psychological state of belief. Toclaim to have knowledge of nature involves an attitude of belief in certain epistemic values, in the procedures associated with them, and in the results to which they lead.1 “Nature,” both as a totality to be known, and as the sum of the results of research directed towards it, turns out to be an object of belief. Reciprocally, even when the operations of nature are ultimately attributed to the action or will of a transcendent power, the production of justified statements about the existence, properties, and intentions of such power entails evidentiary and argumentative practices of the sort associated with the production of knowledge. Hence, whether we accept certain rational propositions and ways of reasoning because we adhere to the epistemic values of modern science or because we trust divine wisdom and benevolence, what is crucial for the validity of an argument is not that it rests on “knowledge” or “belief,” but the authority we ascribe to the relevant practices of substantiating claims, be they in the laboratory or the church.
    [Show full text]
  • Network Analysis of Impulse Dyscontrol in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Subjective Cognitive Decline
    Network analysis of impulse dyscontrol in mild cognitive impairment and subjective cognitive decline T. Saari1*, E. E. Smith23, Z. Ismail234 1Department of Neurology, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland 2Hotchkiss Brain Institute, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada 3Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada 4Department of Psychiatry, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada This manuscript has been published in final, edited form in International Psychogeriatrics. Please cite as: Saari, T., Smith, E., & Ismail, Z. (2021). Network analysis of impulse dyscontrol in mild cognitive impairment and subjective cognitive decline. International Psychogeriatrics, 1-10. doi:10.1017/S1041610220004123 Word count: 4010 Objectives: To investigate conditional dependence relationships of impulse dyscontrol symptoms in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and subjective cognitive decline (SCD). Design: Prospective, observational study. Participants: 235 patients with mild cognitive impairment (n=159) or subjective cognitive decline (n=76) from the Prospective Study for Persons with Memory Symptoms dataset. Measurements: Items of the Mild Behavioral Impairment-Checklist impulse dyscontrol subscale. Results: Stubbornness/rigidity, agitation/aggressiveness, and argumentativeness were frequent and the most central symptoms in the network. Impulsivity, the fourth most central symptom in the network, served as the bridge between these common symptoms and less central and rare symptoms. * Corresponding
    [Show full text]
  • I PSYCHOPATHY and the INSANITY DEFENSE
    i PSYCHOPATHY AND THE INSANITY DEFENSE: A GROUNDED THEORY EXPLORATION OF PUBLIC PERCEPTION BY ELISABETH KNOPP A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Forensic Psychology California Baptist University School of Behavioral Sciences 2017 ii © 2017 Knopp, Elisabeth All Rights Reserved iii Dedication This thesis is dedicated to Mitchell, the love of my life. Thank you for all the love and support throughout this writing process, all the pep talks, and helping me fight procrastination! You are my best friend, my favorite study buddy, and my best accountability partner! Without your positive attitude and encouragement, I may not have gotten through my numerous late night writing sessions. I would also like to dedicate this to my parents who have been there for me all the life and have always pushed me to do my best. I can’t imagine my life without your support and encouragement. You gave me so many opportunities to succeed and wouldn’t ever let me settle for less than my best. You have helped shape me into the person I am today. I wouldn’t be here without you! iv Acknowledgements I would like to thank my wonderful thesis chair, Dr. Anne-Marie Larsen, for her immense help while writing this thesis. Without your guidance, our brainstorming sessions, and all your assistance, I likely wouldn’t even have a completed thesis. Thank you for pushing me to take opportunities to present at conferences and colloquiums and better my resume through research. You have been an immense support to me during these two years and I have always valued your advice and encouragement.
    [Show full text]
  • Impact of Demographic Factors on Impulse Buying Behavior of Consumers in Multan-Pakistan
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by International Institute for Science, Technology and Education (IISTE): E-Journals European Journal of Business and Management www.iiste.org ISSN 2222-1905 (Paper) ISSN 2222-2839 (Online) Vol.7, No.22, 2015 Impact of Demographic Factors on Impulse Buying Behavior of Consumers in Multan-Pakistan Prof. Dr. Abdul Ghafoor Awan Dean, Faculty of Management and Social Sciences, Institute of Southern Punjab, Multan Nayyar Abbas MS Scholar, Department of Business Administration, Institute of Southern Punjab, Multan Abstract The purpose of this study is to investigate the effect of demographic factors (gender, age, income, and education) on impulse buying behavior of consumers in Multan. The study adopted quantitative approach. A structured questionnaire was used to survey 250 respondents (104 males and 146 females) who were selected using non- probability convenience sampling. Data were collected in different educational institutions of Multan. Different statistical methods like multiple regression, chi-square test and simple descriptive techniques were used to derive results from the data collected with the help of SPSS 17.0. The multiple regression and chi-square test results revealed that gender and age were significantly and inversely associated with impulse buying behavior of consumers. The results further indicated that income and education were significantly and directly associated with impulse buying behavior of consumers. The ANOVA results indicated that consumers’ demographic characteristic had significant influence on impulse buying and demographic characteristics (gender, age, income, and education) affect simultaneously impulse buying behavior of consumers. The findings of the study were consistent and supported by previous studies.
    [Show full text]
  • Reflection and Impulse
    1 REFLECTION AND IMPULSE A Framework for Basic Research and Applied Science Roland Deutsch, Bertram Gawronski, and Wilhelm Hofmann Behaving in ways that optimize the satisfaction of an organism's needs is a challenging task. Not only do organisms face a tremendous degree of uncertainty about what kinds of behaviors will be successful in a given environment; they are also faced with numerous decision dilemmas (Geschke, 2013), one of the most important being the dilemma caused by conflicts between short-term pay­ offi and long-term goals. The distinction between impulsive (i.e., short sighted, fast) decisions and behaviors on the one hand and reflected (i.e., far sighted, slow) decisions and behaviors on the other hand is at the core ofmany everyday problems and captured by multiple psychological theories. Impulsive decisions and behaviors are typically defined as occurring quickly, with litde care for accuracy, and without consideration of future outcomes. Reflective decisions and behaviors, on the other hand, are typically defmed in terms of the opposite characteristics, involving slower responses that are based on careful consideration of multiple outcomes, including abstract and future consequences (Evenden, 1999; Kagan, 1966). Is it better to quickly act on the spur of the moment? Or is it better to reflect on things and act in accordance with what the future might bring? There is no universal answer to this question. Being overly future-oriented might come at great risk, because events become more uncertain with increasing temporal distance. Yet, being overly oriented towards short-term outcomes can undermine successful need fulfillment iflong-term consequences are ignored (Geschke, 2013; Green & Myerson, 2004).
    [Show full text]
  • A Focus on Impulse Buying
    Articles Papeles del Psicólogo / Psychologist Papers , 2019 Vol. 40(2), pp. 149-156 https://doi.org/10.23923/pap.psicol2019.2893 http://www.papelesdelpsicologo.es http:// www.psychologistpapers.com HOW DOES SELF-CONTROL OPERATE? A FOCUS ON IMPULSE BUYING Meysam Moayery, Lorea Narvaiza Cantín and Juan José Gibaja Martíns Deusto Business School, University of Deusto Existe abundante evidencia en la psicología social que atestigua el autocontrol como moldeador del comportamiento humano. Sin embargo, todavía hay margen para llevar a cabo más exploraciones sobre la interacción entre el sistema de autocontrol y el comportamiento del consumidor, en concreto sobre la compra por impulso. Esta investigación busca arrojar luz sobre la importancia del estudio de la compra por impulso que, como en cualquier otro ámbito de la vida humana, se ve como resultado del proceso de autocontrol. Este artículo considera diferentes tipos de fracaso de autocontrol que resultan de normas contradictorias, del punto hasta el que las personas monitorizan su comportamiento y del agotamiento de recursos autorregulatorios. A esto le sigue una explicación de los actuales enfoques sobre el estudio del autocontrol en estudios que versan sobre la compra por impulso. De manera adicional, a través de la integración de diferentes modelos de auto control, el presente artículo ofrece un marco general del proceso de autocontrol con especial énfasis en la compra por impulso. Este marco integra componentes de impulsos, el conflicto, la resistencia y el resultado del autocontrol, los cuales deberían ser tenidos en cuenta de manera conjunta al analizar fenómenos de autocontrol. Palabras clave: Autocontrol, Compra por impulso, Comportamiento del consumidor, Marco integrador.
    [Show full text]
  • The Human Choice: Individuation, Reason, and Impulse, and Chaos
    nebraska symposium on motivation 1969 The Human Choice: WILLIAM J. ARNOLD and DAVID LEVINE, Editors Individuation, Reason, and Order versus Deindividuation, Dalbir Bindra Professor of Psychology- McGill b'liiversily Impulse, and Chaos Professor of Psychology Edward L. Wike PHILIP G. ZIMBARDO University of Kansas Stanjord University Roger W. Black Professor of Psychology University oj South Carolina Omtrol. That’s what current psychology is all about. The use ol powerful schedules of reinforcement, probing neurophysiological techniques, computer simulation, and the new behavior therapies Elliot Aronson Professor of Psychology University of Texas at Austin (among other advances) enable psychologists to manipulate the responses of a wide range of research subjects in order to improve learning and discrimination; to arouse, rechannel, and satisfy Stuart W. Cook Professor of Psyi hology drives; and to redirect abnormal or deviant behavior. It has, in fact, Univernty of Colorado become the all-consuming task of most psychologists to learn how to bring behavior under stimulus control. Philip G. Zimbardo Prolessor of Psychology It is especially impressive to note (in any volume of the Nebraska Stanford University Symposium on Motivation) the relative case with which laboratory researchers can induce motives which have an immediate and often demonstrably pervasive effect on a vast array of response measures. On the other hand, one must reconcile this with the observation ’ that in the “real world” people often.show considerable tolerance university
    [Show full text]
  • Epigenetics and the Biological Definition of Gene × Environment
    Child Development, January/February 2010, Volume 81, Number 1, Pages 41–79 Epigenetics and the Biological Definition of Gene · Environment Interactions Michael J. Meaney McGill University Variations in phenotype reflect the influence of environmental conditions during development on cellular functions, including that of the genome. The recent integration of epigenetics into developmental psychobiol- ogy illustrates the processes by which environmental conditions in early life structurally alter DNA, provid- ing a physical basis for the influence of the perinatal environmental signals on phenotype over the life of the individual. This review focuses on the enduring effects of naturally occurring variations in maternal care on gene expression and phenotype to provide an example of environmentally driven plasticity at the level of the DNA, revealing the interdependence of gene and environmental in the regulation of phenotype. The nature–nurture debate is essentially a question prevention and intervention programs. In the social of the determinants of individual differences in the sciences, and particularly psychology, there has expression of specific traits among members of the generally been an understandable bias in favor of same species. The origin of the terms nature and explanations derived from the ‘‘nurture’’ perspec- nurture has been credited to Richard Mulcaster, a tive, which emphasizes the capacity for environ- British teacher who imagined these influences as mentally induced plasticity in brain structure and collaborative forces that shape child development function. Nevertheless, over recent decades there (West & King, 1987). History has conspired to per- has been a gradual, if at times reluctant acceptance vert Mulcaster’s intent, casting genetic and environ- that genomic variations contribute to individual dif- mental influences as independent agents in the ferences in brain development and function.
    [Show full text]
  • Ego Depletion” Or: How the Brain Can Help Us Understand Why Self
    Neuroscience of depletion 1 The neuroscience of “ego depletion” or: How the brain can help us understand why self- control seems limited Michael Inzlicht1,2, Elliot Berkman3, & Nathaniel Elkins-Brown1 1Department of Psychology, University of Toronto 2Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto 3Department of Psychology, University of Oregon Word count (including references): 10,885 Please send correspondence to: Michael Inzlicht University of Toronto Department of Psychology 1265 Military Trail Toronto, Ontario M1C 1A4, Canada E-mail: [email protected] Telephone: 416-208-4862 Fax: 416-287-7642 Neuroscience of depletion 2 The neuroscience of ego depletion or: How the brain can help us understand why self- control seems limited The past decade has witnessed an explosion of research on the topic of self-control. And this is a good thing. After all, self-control is thought to sustain the better angels of our nature while discouraging our more demonic sides. This could be why self-control failure is thought to be at the root of societal ills ranging from financial debt to marital infidelity to criminality (Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). It could also be why poor self-control has been blamed for up to 40% of preventable deaths (Schroeder, 2007). What is self-control? Self-control, known colloquially as willpower, refers to the mental processes that allow people to override any of their thoughts, emotions, or behaviors that compete with their overarching goals. At its heart, self-control is instigated when a person faces a conflict between two competing desires or response tendencies—say, when a dieter is conflicted between ordering a salad or a juicy hamburger for lunch; or when a writer is conflicted between putting pen to paper or checking her favorite social media websites; or even when a study participant is conflicted between naming the color of a word and reading the word in a Stroop task.
    [Show full text]