Journal of Pollination Ecology, 23(12), 2018, pp 102-118

FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY: A NOVEL DIGITAL APPROACH TO VISUALIZE THE BEE VIEW OF FLOWERS

Christian Verhoeven1, Zong-Xin Ren2, and Klaus Lunau1 1Institute of Sensory Ecology, Department Biology, Heinrich-Heine-University Düsseldorf, Universitätsstr. 1, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany 2Key Laboratory for Plant Diversity and Biogeography of East Asia, Kunming Institute of Botany, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Kunming, Yunnan 650201, People’s Republic of China

Abstract—The colour vision system of bees and humans differs mainly in that, contrary to humans, bees are sensitive to ultraviolet light and insensitive to red light. The synopsis of a colour picture and a UV picture is inappropriate to illustrate the bee view of flowers, since the colour picture does not exclude red light. In this study false-colour pictures in bee view are assembled from digital photos taken through a UV, a blue, and a green filter matching the spectral sensitivity of the bees’ photoreceptors. False-colour pictures demonstrate small-sized colour patterns in flowers, e.g. based on pollen grains, anthers, filamental hairs, and other tiny structures that are inaccessible to spectrophotometry. Moreover, false-colour pictures are suited to demonstrate flowers and floral parts that are conspicuous or inconspicuous to bees. False-colour pictures also direct the attention to other ranges of wavelength besides ultraviolet demonstrating for example blue and yellow bulls’ eyes in addition to UV bulls’ eyes which previously have been overlooked. False-colour photography is a robust method that can be used under field conditions, with various equipment and with simple colour editing. Keywords: bee, colour vision, flower colour, colour pattern, bee view, false colour

INTRODUCTION Flowers serve as filters signalling attractants for pollinators as well as deterrents for flower antagonists (Junker & Blüthgen 2010). Due to differences in the number of photoreceptors types, the spectral range of sensitivity of the colour photoreceptors, and differences in the colour preference of various flower visitors, flowers can be attractive to pollinators and unattractive to illegitimate visitors, i.e. pollen thieves and nectar robbers, at the same time (Lunau & Maier 1995; Lunau et al. 2011). Since many flower visitors, such as hummingbirds (Herrera et al. 2008), bats (Winter et al. 2003), bees (Peitsch et al. 1992), hoverflies (Lunau 2014), beetles (Martínez-Harms et al. 2012), butterflies and moths (Kelber et al. 2002; Arikawa 2003) are sensitive to ultraviolet light, these flower visitors necessarily view different aspects of the spectral reflectance of flowers as compared to UV-blind humans. Many of these flower visitors possess a different number of colour photoreceptor types compared to humans, FIGURE 1. Spectral sensitivity of colour photoreceptors. Top: e.g. one in bats, two in beetles, three in bees, four in Relative sensitivity of the three human photoreceptor types. The hummingbirds and flies, and from three up to fifteen in curves peak at 420 nm, 534 nm and 564 nm (Bowmaker et al. 1980). butterflies (Briscoe 2008; Chen et al. 2016). This results in In the UV range of wavelength between 300 nm and 400 nm the colour vision systems hardly conceivable to that in humans. photoreceptors are not sensitive (modified from Stockman et al. However, bees have trichromatic colour vision just like 1993). Bottom: Relative sensitivity of the three photoreceptor types humans, but are sensitive to ultraviolet, blue and green light of the honeybee (Apis mellifera). The curves peak at 344 nm, 436 rather than blue, green and red light as humans (Peitsch et al. nm and 544 nm. In the red range of wavelength between 630 nm and 1992; Fig. 1). Neuronal processing of colour photoreceptor 700 nm (red light) the photoreceptors are not sensitive (modified from Peitsch et al. 1992).

inputs in bees and humans is similar, both bees and humans Received 22 January 2018, accepted 22 May 2018 have colour opponent neurons (Backhaus 1991) and colour *Corresponding author: [email protected] constancy (Neumeyer 1981). To understand flower colours and floral colours pattern as bees might see them Kevan

102 June 2018 FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY IN BEE VIEW 103

(1978, 1979, 1983) claimed the need for a bee-subjective made by a UV-sensitive camera and specially selected set of colorimetric analysis of flower colours. filters to reconstruct images of flowers as bees might see them by calculating the quantum catches of the honeybee Three methods are frequently used to study floral colours photoreceptors for each ommatidium dependent on the and colour patterns as perceived by bees. The measurement of distance between bee and flower. Multispectral images, a series the spectral reflectance by a spectrophotometer allows for the of black-and-white photos taken through different bandpass comparison of the spectral reflectance of a floral tissue with filters (Kevan 1972; Chittka and Kevan 2005) from alpine that of a black standard and of a white standard. A database flowers of the Rocky Mountains, Colorado, are available from provides access to spectral reflectance measurements for many the Floral Image Database (http://liu.edu/flower). Benitez- plant species (Arnold et al. 2010). Reflection spectroscopy is Vieyra et al. (2007), Williams & Dyer (2007), Brito et al. limited by the size and surface properties of the measuring (2015) and Hempel de Ibarra et al. (2015) presented false spot, which does not allow for measuring the reflectance of colour photos of flowers in bee view, some of which require small-sized, curved or rough-textured structures like pollen sophisticated calculations or calibrations. Photographic grains, stamens, and filamental hairs. The standard method of methods have also been used for simultaneous quantitative reflection spectroscopy is a measurement angle of 45° without measurements of spatial and spectral components of floral integrating sphere (Chittka & Kevan 2005; but see Stavenga colour patterns as seen by bees (Vorobyev et al. 1997; Benitez- & van der Kooi 2016). This method largely ignores the impact Vieyra et al. 2007; Williams & Dyer 2007; Hempel de Ibarra of the surface of petals being flat, conical or other, since most & Vorobyev 2009; Garcia et al. 2014; Hempel de Ibarra et al. white standards possess a smooth if not glossy surface. 2015; White et al. 2015). Moreover, the transparency of the petals is ignored, since the flower, e.g. a petal, is placed on different backgrounds being Recent studies presenting multispectral images from black, white or another petal for measurements, which flowers in bee view (Garcia et al. 2014; Brito et al. 2015; disregards light shining through the petal from the backside Hempel de Ibarra et al. 2015; White et al. 2015; Vasas et al. and the impact of the colour chosen as background. 2017) are mostly focusing on one or few target species and /or require sophisticated calculations or calibrations. In this Another method is the synopsis of a colour picture and a study we introduce a simple and cheap method for general use UV-picture showing the flower as appearing to humans and based on a slightly modified and supplemented standard the ultraviolet reflection pattern on different pictures camera equipment to create UV-pictures and false-colour (Daumer 1958; Silberglied 1979). This method possesses the pictures in bee view and to highlight the rationale of this disadvantage that the readers might be successful in notionally method for field studies. The estimated prices for a low-cost superimposing the colour picture and the UV-picture but fail and a standard equipment as used in this study are shown in to eliminate the red content of the colour picture at the same Supplement S1. This equipment is much less expensive than time. Red, however, is a component of yellow, magenta, purple custom-built multispectral cameras, e.g. Spectrocam and white colours. Moreover, the interpretation of UV- Multispectral Wheel Cameras (Ocean Optics). The pictures pictures showing UV-absorbing parts of flowers is sometimes are intended to demonstrate the bee-subjective colour patterns misleading, e.g. if it is concluded that red and UV-absorbing of flowers in bee view with details that cannot be measured parts are invisible to bees (Chittka & Waser 1997). A using a normal spectrophotometer. Moreover, it is television camera equipped with UV-transmitting lens and demonstrated how false-colour pictures in bee view are used UV-transmitting filter allowed direct observation and to better understand the impact of the bees’ sensitivity for recording on video tape (Eisner et al. 1969; Silberglied 1976; ultraviolet and insensitivity for red light. For this purpose, Gronquist et al. 2001). Also, a modified image converter and false-colour pictures of floral colour changes and small-sized image amplifier combined with UV-transmitting filter and floral structures are shown as false-colour pictures. In UV-transmitting lens enabled to screen the UV-reflectance addition, yet undescribed forms of colour patterns are properties of many flowers (Rosen & Barthlott 1991) in the described using false colour pictures such as blue bull’s eyes field. However, classical UV-photography using a UV- rather than ultraviolet bull’s eyes (Silberglied 1979; Koski & sensitive camera mounted on a tripod, UV-transmitting filter Ashman 2014). The robustness of this method is tested by and UV-transmitting lens and a UV-sensitive film were still the comparison of false-colour pictures taken with different preferred to produce UV-photos with high quality (Biedinger cameras, lenses, filters, different picture editing programs and & Barthlott 1993; Burr & Barthlott 1993; Burr et al. 1995; in field conditions. Lunau 2000). With the turn of the millennium, digital cameras were used mostly after having their internal UV- and MATERIALS AND METHODS IR-blocking filter removed and sometimes with the support of a UV-lamp for illumination (Koski & Ashman 2013). False-colour photography of flowers and floral colour Several attempts have been made to develop methods to patterns in bee view is an extension to the well-established create colour pictures in bee view. As early as in 1958 Daumer UV-photography. The editing of digital pictures allows to tried to evaluate floral colours considering the bees’ colour split a colour picture into the colour channels and to perception. He took photographs with a yellow, a blue and a reassemble a colour picture from the colour channels and thus UV filter and calculated the “bee-colour” (Daumer 1958), to use a UV-channel instead of blue, a blue channel instead of but of course he did not have the option to merge the green and a green channel instead of red in order to shift the photographs to a bee view-picture. Vorobyev et al. (1997) range of wavelength for about 100 nm. To match the bee developed a method based on video recordings of flowers photoreceptors’ sensibility as close as possible specific filters

104 LUNAU ET AL. J Poll Ecol 23(12) might be used. Here the principal method of false-colour photography is described and the quality of results obtained with different equipment and with different processing is compared. Equipment To capture photos in bee view we used a modified Panasonic GH-1 camera. The low-pass filter was removed in order to increase the sensitivity for ultraviolet light. We used an Ultra-Achromatic-Takumar 1:4.5/85 lens made of fused quartz that transmits UV light. Since the modified camera is sensitive to ultraviolet and infrared light, we used a UV-/IR- Cut filter to capture a normal reference picture. This filter transmits light between 400 nm and 700 nm only (Fig. 2). To simulate the spectral sensitivity of typical bee photoreceptor FIGURE 3. Relative transmission of the three colour filters types we used special filters that only transmit light of certain (Baader). Violet: UV-filter transmits UV light only. Blue: Blue filter that only transmits blue light. Green: Green filter that only transmits ranges of wavelengths (Fig. 3). Other equipment used light of the green part of the spectrum (changed according to Schmitt included a Nikon D40 camera with low-pass filter, a 18-55 K., personal communication, 21. September 2015). mm Nikkor lens and a 105 mm UV-Nikkor lens. In addition, only a UV picture and a colour photo split into the colour channels were used for picture processing (see below). A robust tripod and calm conditions are necessary for taking the different photos since changing the filter in between can be time-consuming. A white Teflon disc reflecting equal amounts of light in a range of wavelength from 300 nm to 700 nm was used for manual white balance before taking photos. Alternatively, a 20% Spectralon Grey Standard was used (see below). Photography Since most flowers are small we set the aperture values between f/8 and f/16 to ensure good image resolution. The exposure time and ISO value of the camera were adjusted to the daylight. All pictures were focused manually because the viewfinder is completely dark after attaching the UV transmitting filter. Four photos were taken, one photo with UV-/IR-Cut filter, one photo with the UV filter, one photo with the blue, and one photo with the green filter. All pictures were saved in the camera's RAW format. This allows to adjust the white balance in post-production. As a reference we used a Spectralon Grey Standard that reflects all wavelengths of the needed 300 nm to 700 nm spectrum with 20%. The grey

FIGURE 4. The splitting and merging process in detail.

reference was captured for every channel (normal, blue, green, UV) with the flowers or in separate pictures and always under the identical light and with identical camera settings. Picture editing The pictures taken with the UV, blue, and green filter were split into the colour channels in order to convert the merged picture into the RGB system which is visible for humans. From the picture with UV filter the red channel, from that with blue filter the blue channel, and from that with FIGURE 2. Relative transmission of the UV-/IR-Cut filter green filter the green channel was used for merging (Fig. 4). (Baader). Only light between 400 nm and 700 nm can transmit the In windy conditions the frequent change of filters might not filter (changed according to Baader Planetarium 2017). be useful. The alternative option is to take only two photos, one with the UV-/IR-Cut filter and one with the UV-filter. The photo taken with the UV-/IR-Cut filter then is split to

June 2018 FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY IN BEE VIEW 105

FIGURE 5. Colour picture of the ram’s horn Proboscidea louisianica (Martyniaceae) showing the human view and false-colour picture showing the bee view. The merging of the false-colour picture was done using the UV, blue and green channel and by omitting the red channel.

the colour channels and the merging of the false-colour photo Grey Standard is done with the split colour photo and the split UV photo. All pictures were saved in the camera's RAW format. This To merge the channels to a bee view picture all captured allows to adjust the white balance in post-production. As a pictures were pre-adjusted using Adobe Lightroom CC reference we used a Spectralon Grey Standard Labsphere SRS- (version 2015 or newer). The merging was done in Photoshop 20-010 that reflects all wavelengths of the needed 300 nm to or ImageJ. Moreover, preset white balance values can be used 700 nm spectrum with 20%. The grey reference was captured to reduce time between the two photos. for every photo, resp. channel (normal, blue, green, UV) with If only two photos were taken using manual white balance the flowers or in separate pictures and always under the with a white and UV-reflecting Teflon disc, one photo was identical light and with identical camera settings. To merge taken with UV-/IR-Cut filter and one with the UV-filter. the channels to a bee view picture all captured pictures were Then the picture taken with the UV-/IR-Cut filter was split pre-adjusted using Adobe Lightroom CC (version 2015 or into the colour channels and the blue and green channels were newer). used for merging. The red channel was discarded. Also, the photo taken with the UV-filter was split into the colour Preparing the pictures using Adobe Lightroom channels and the red channel was used for merging. In By using the histogram in Adobe Lightroom we adjusted addition, we resigned the use of the RAW format or used a the pictures so that the containing grey standard has the same modified Nikon D40 camera and a plastic lens Nikon AF-S brightness in all pictures. The histogram shows how many DX NIKKOR 18-55 mm. The merging was done in pixels in a picture (y-axis) have a certain brightness (x-axis). Photoshop or ImageJ. This is shown for the blue, the green and the red channel. For producing false-colour pictures the UV channel was Since we only use the blue channel of the picture captured converted to the new blue channel, the blue channel to the with the blue filter, the brightness of the picture was adjusted green, and green channel to the new red channel (Fig. 5). To so that the peak of the blue channel is exactly in the middle of analyse the pictures the following colour translation chart is the histogram (Fig. 6). We did the same with the peak of the useful (Tab. 1). For a picture that shows colours in bee view green channel in the picture that was captured using the green we need a UV, a blue and a green colour channel. These three filter. Because red channel of our cameras seems to be more colour channels need to be converted into the RGB system, UV sensitive than the other channels we adjusted the UV which is visible for humans. Since the red channel is not picture so that the peak of the red channel is in the middle of needed it can be discarded. The bees’ S-type of photoreceptors the histogram. In pictures where we captured the grey standard is sensitive for UV light. The humans’ S-type of separately we adjusted the picture containing the grey standard photoreceptors is sensitive for blue light. As a consequence, and copied the values to the picture containing just the flower. we convert the UV channel to the new blue channel, the blue channel is shown as green and green as red.

106 LUNAU ET AL. J Poll Ecol 23(12)

Real colour Colour we see Colour the bees see Colour we see in the false-colour picture

UV Black Bee-Ultraviolet Blue

Blue +UV Blue Bee-UV-Blue Cyan

Green +UV Green Bee-Purple Purple

Red +UV Red Bee-Ultraviolet Blue

Blue Blue Bee-Blue Green

Blue +Green Cyan Bee-Blue-Green Yellow

Blue +Red Purple Bee-Blue Green

Green Green Bee-Green Red

Green +Red Yellow Bee-Green Red

Red Red Bee-Black Black

White -UV White Bee-Blue-Green Yellow

White +UV White Bee-White White

Black -UV Black Bee-Black Black

Black +UV Black Bee-Ultraviolet Blue

Grey -UV Grey Bee-Blue-Green Yellow TABLE 1. Colour Grey +UV Grey Bee-Grey Grey Translation Chart

FIGURE 6. Histogram of a picture taken with the blue filter. On the left: Picture without adjustments. It is clearly visible that the blue channel of the picture is overexposed. On the right: The same picture after adjusting the brightness. The peak of the blue channel is now located in the middle and has the correct brightness.

the green and the red channel of the blue image/layer. This For the normal picture we used the white balance dropper command overwrites the blue image with its own blue colour tool with the grey standard as a reference point to set the white channel. balance. We used the same dialogue to overwrite the green image Merging the photos in Adobe Photoshop with its own green colour channel and to overwrite the UV The prepared green, blue and UV pictures were opened in image with its own red channel. Photoshop as layers. At this point every picture still consists Using the command “Layer > Layer Style > Blending of three colour channels (RGB). By using the command Options” we converted the layers into their new colour “Image > Apply Image” we discarded the channels not channels. Under “Advanced Blending” we unchecked the needed. Figure 7 shows the settings that we used to discard colour channels not needed. For example: The blue image

June 2018 FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY IN BEE VIEW 107

Layers”. We used the projection method “Reposition” or “Auto”. If this method did not work we aligned the layers manually using the “Move Tool”. If the grey standard was photographed next to the flower it should be grey in the false-colour image as well. If it shows a colour tint the new created colour channels are not perfectly equally bright. These small colour shifts were corrected by using a “Levels Adjustment Layer”. By creating a new empty layer in the uppermost position and using the shortcut “Ctrl + Alt + Shift + E” (on Windows) we merged the whole false- colour picture to a single layer. With the “Marquee Tool” we selected the surface of grey standard and blurred the selected FIGURE 7: The “Apply Image” dialogue in Photoshop. The area with the command “Filter > Blur > Average”. This layer with the blue image gets overwritten with its own blue colour ensures that the influence of the grainy structure of the channel. The not needed green and red channels get discarded. standard and noise from the camera's sensor are as small as possible. becomes the new green channel so we unchecked “R” and “B” We created a “Levels Adjustment Layer” by clicking (Fig. 8). “Layer > New Adjustment Layer > Levels” in the uppermost In some cases, the images are a little bit shifted to one position. By using the Tool “Sample in image to set grey another. To compensate this, we re-aligned the images by point” on the blurred area this layer corrects all the colours in clicking “Select > All Layers” and “Edit > Auto-Align the image using the grey standard as a reference (Fig. 9).

FIGURE 8. The “Blending Options” dialogue in Photoshop. Under “Advanced Blending” we can decide to which colour channel the image/layer gets converted to. This figure shows the settings for the blue image/layer that gets converted to the new green channel.

FIGURE 9. Levels Adjustment Layer. If the grey standard was photographed in separate pictures and not next to the flower we also converted these photos into a false-colour picture and followed all described steps. Then we copied the “Levels Adjustment Layer” to the false- colour picture of the flower. The pictures were saved in the Photoshop PSD format and as JPGs (8 Bit, sRGB).

108 LUNAU ET AL. J Poll Ecol 23(12)

Merging the photos in ImageJ To verify the merging process and to show an alternative to Photoshop we also merged some images using ImageJ and the plugin “MICA Toolbox” (Troscianko & Stevens 2015). Because we used special colour filters we had to create a custom *.txt file that tells the program which colour channels from which image are getting used (Fig. 10). How this works shall not be explained in detail in this paper because there is a comprehensive user guide available on the programmer’s website. To merge the images, we opened ImageJ used the command “Plugins > Multispectral Imaging > Generate Multispectral Image”. In the upcoming dialogue we adjusted some settings. Under “Settings” we chose our custom *.txt file. All other settings are visible in Fig. 11. After starting the process by clicking “OK” ImageJ automatically asks for the different pictures and guides the user through different steps that we followed. First, we opened the green image containing the grey standard and selected the standards surface area. Then the program asks for the green image without the standard. The steps repeat with the blue and the UV image. In between we manually aligned the images since we selected “Manual Align” (Fig. 11). When all images were opened and aligned we used the command “Plugins > Multispectral Imaging > Tools > Make Presentation Image“. In the following dialogue box, we decided that the green channel of the green image becomes the new red channel, the blue channel of the blue image becomes the new green channel and the red channel of the UV image becomes the new blue channel. We also chose “Square Root” FIGURE 11. The settings that were used to create a false-colour under “Transform” and checked the “Convert to RGB image with ImageJ. We told the program that the grey standard was colour” box (Fig. 12). captured in separate photos and that is moves between them. We also By clicking “OK” the program merges the images to a set the reflectance to 20%. Under ‘Alignment & Scaling’ we chose manual align. We set the output to ‘Aligned Normalised 32-bit’. false-colour image. We saved the images as JPEG files. Interpretation of false-colour pictures RESULTS In order to test the value of false-colour photography we studied floral colour changes and floral colour patterns and Colour change compared false-colour pictures and UV-pictures as a basis for Floral colour changes in the Shrubby Milkwort Polygala the interpretation of the flower colours as seen by bees. A chamaebuxus (Polygalaceae) affects the conspicuousness of series of false-colour pictures of sub-alpine and alpine flowers the post-change colour in the false-colour picture but is not were taken to test the reliability of this method. Whether the detectable in the ultraviolet range of wavelength (Fig. 13 A, B, results of false-colour photography are similar with deviant C). The pre-change as well as the post-change colour of the equipment, deviant picture processing and different filters Common Lungwort Pulmonaria officinale (Boraginaceae) are were also tested by using the selected flowers. reflecting ultraviolet light and are conspicuous for bees according to the false-colour picture (Fig. 13 D, E, F). The Field Forget-me-not Myosotis arvensis (Boraginaceae) displays a yet undescribed threefold colour change. The purplish early flowing phase and middle bluish flowering phase display a colour change visible to the human eyes. But the flower petals also change from UV-absorbing to UV- reflecting. This colour change is visible to bees’ eyes but invisible to the human eyes. The colour change from the FIGURE 10: The custom *.txt file created to tell the program bluish and UV-absorbing middle flowering phase to the which colour channel to use. equally bluish late UV-reflecting flowering phase is visible to bees’ eyes as is the colour change from human-yellow to human-white of the floral guide (Fig. 13 G, H, I).

June 2018 FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY IN BEE VIEW 109

phlomoides (Scrophulariaceae) has all-yellow flowers and displays an ultraviolet bull’s eye. Due to an additional blue bull’s eye the floral colour pattern is rather complex in bee view (Fig. 17). Tiny structures False-colour pictures are able to display small-scale floral colour patterns visible to bees as shown for stamens and florets of the Cornflower Centaurea cyanus (Asteraceae) (Fig. 18 A, B, C) pollen and white staminal protuberances of the Flower- of-an-hour Hibiscus trionum (Malvaceae) (Fig. 18 D, E, F), a colour gradient of small-sized floral guides of the Round- Leaved Saxifrage Saxifraga rotundifolia (Saxifragaceae) (Fig. 18 G, H, I) and the androeceum of the Wood Anemone Anemone nemorosa (Ranunculaceae) (Fig. 18 J, K, L). Didactics The conspicuousness or inconspicuousness of human-red flowers for bees’ eyes is dependent of the ultraviolet reflection as demonstrated for the UV-reflecting flower of the Winged Pea Lotus tetragonolobus (Fig. 19 A, B, C; Fabaceae) and the Red Poppy Papaver rhoeas (Fig. 19 G, H, I; Papaveraceae) as well as the UV-absorbing flower of Forrests Rhododendron Rhododendron forrestii (Fig. 19 D, E, F; Ericaceae). The underside of the petals in the Lousewort Pedicularis gruina (Orobanchaceae) display a rare white and UV-reflecting colour (Fig. 19 J, K, L), whereas the white flowers Common Snowdrop Galanthus nivalis (Amaryllidaceae) stand out against the snow due to absorption of ultraviolet light (Fig. 19 M, N, O). The Bittersweet Nightshade Solanum dulcamara (Solanaceae) displays yellow anthers and green floral guides on the petals with possess a similar colour for FIGURE 12. Dialogue box to select that the green channel from bees (Fig. 19 P, Q, R). the picture taken with the green filter becomes the new red channel, blue channel from the picture taken with the blue filter becomes Colour pattern of sub-alpine and alpine flowers on green and the red channel from the picture taken with the UV filter the mountains of eastern Himalayas becomes blue. A series of photos taken under field conditions in the surroundings of the Lijiang Forest Ecosystem Research Bull’s eyes Station operated by the Kunming Institute of Botany, Chinese Bull’s eye refers to colour patterns that highlight the Academy of Sciences (China) using automatically controlled rewarding center of a flower. Some of these bull’s eyes are exposure to light for UV-photos and for colour photos, a visible to bees but invisible to humans (e. g. UV bull’s eyes). single white balance value for all colour photos and another Some colour patterns can be visible to humans too, but there for all UV-photos and later manual adjustment of the are also non-UV colour patterns that are invisible to humans. brightness of the channels reveals satisfactory results (Fig. 20, The Yellow Archangel Lamium galeobdolon (Lamiacae) 21). This equipment was easily carried on an international displays a typical all-yellow flower colour and an additional flight and proved to stay functional at high altitude with low ultraviolet bull’s eye resulting in a conspicuous colour pattern temperature, low oxygen and high UV-radiation. The in bee view (Fig. 14 A, B, C). The flower displays two shades Edelweiss flower of Leontopodium sp. (Asteraceae) possesses of yellow which are congruent with the UV-pattern (Fig. 14 hairy petals inaccessible to spectrophotometry (Fig. 20 A, B, A, B, C). The Marshmarigold Caltha palustris C). The white part of the fused dorsal petals of the Lousewort (Ranunculaceae) is uniformly yellow, but nevertheless displays Pedicularis cephalantha (Orobanchaceae) displays a colour a bee-visible colour pattern due to a pattern of reflection in that is rarely found in bee-pollinated plants, i.e. a white and the ultraviolet light (Fig. 14 D, E, F). The Yellow Daffodil UV-reflecting colour (Fig. 20 D, E, F). The flower of Tibetia Narcissus pseudonarcissus (Amaryllidaceae) shows no yunnanensis (Fabaceae) displays a white, yet UV-absorbing ultraviolet, but a blue bull’s eye resulting in a colour pattern floral guide similar to pollen-mimicking floral guides visible to bees (Fig. 15). The Key of Heaven Primula veris described above (Fig. 20 G, H, I). The flower of Primula (Fig. 16 A, B, C) and the Oxlip Primula elatior (Fig. 16 D, E, poissonii (Primulaceae) possesses no UV-pattern, yet a bee- F; Primulaceae) have all-yellow flowers, reflect uniform in the visible floral guide (Fig. 20 J, K, L). The flower of Salvia ultraviolet range of wavelength, but nevertheless display a hylocharis (Lamiaceae) displays an unusual type of floral colour pattern visible to bees. The Great Mullein Verbascum guide absorbing all visible wavelengths as well as ultraviolet

110 LUNAU ET AL. J Poll Ecol 23(12)

FIGURE 13. Colour pictures (left), UV-pictures (middle) and false-colour pictures (right) of floral colour change in Polygala chamaebuxus L. (A, B, C), Pulmonaria officinalis L. (D, E, F), and Myosotis arvensis Hill (G, H, I).

FIGURE 14. Colour pictures (left), UV-pictures (middle) and false-colour pictures (right) of bull’s eyes in Lamium galeobdolon (L.) (A, B, C) and Caltha palustris L. (D, E, F).

FIGURE 15: Colour picture, UV-picture and false-colour picture of Narcissus pseudonarcissuss L.. Additional pictures show the blue channel and the green channel.

June 2018 FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY IN BEE VIEW 111

light (Fig. 20 M, N, O). The pollinators of Paris polyphylla (Melanthiaceae) are still unknown (Ren et al. 2014); although the flower displays no UV-pattern it has a bee-visible colour pattern (Fig. 20 P, Q, R). The Saxifrage Saxifraga signata (Saxifragaceae) possesses flowers with tiny yellow protuberances that uncommonly reflect ultraviolet light and might mimic the colour and gloss of nectar in addition to orange and UV-absorbing floral guides (Fig. 21 A, B, C). The yellow flower of Potentilla griffithii (Rosaceae) displays basal orange floral guides on each petal that are also visible to bees which, however, cannot be read from the UV-picture (Fig. 21 D, E, F). The yellow flower of the Lousewort Pedicularis FIGURE 16. Colour pictures (left), UV-pictures (middle) and longiflora (Orobanchaceae) displays two red floral guides false-colour pictures (right) of Primula elatior (L.) (A, B, C) and which are invisible on the UV-picture, but clearly visible on Primula veris L. (D, E, F). the false-colour picture (Fig. 21 G, H, I). The flowers of the Lousewort Pedicularis tricolor (Fig. 21 J, K, L; Orobanchaceae) and of Trollius yunnanensis (Fig. 21 M, N, O; Ranunculaceae) display different shades of yellow and no UV-pattern which results in clearly bee visible colour pattern. Robustness to changes in equipment and processing We compared different false-colour images using the three colour filters with false-colour images that were taken with just the UV-/IR-Cut filter and the UV filter (Fig. 22 A, B). The principal details about the colour change of Myosotis arvensis (Fig. 13, see above) are also found using deviant techniques. For the merging of the false-colour photos Adobe Photoshop (Fig. 22 C) and ImageJ (Fig. 22 D) were FIGURE 17. Colour picture, UV-picture and false-colour compared. picture of Verbascum phlomoides L.. Additional pictures show the Using different cameras and lenses also revealed similar blue channel and the green channel. results; even the use of a cheap plastic lens with less transmittance of ultraviolet light than the quartz lenses seem adequate for creating false-colour pictures (Fig. 23). To see if the method works without setting the white balance we created a false-colour picture without any brightness adjustments. The camera was set to auto (Fig. 24 A). We also used styrofoam as a reference standard to match the brightness levels in post (Fig. 24 C) to see if styrofoam can be a cheap alternative to grey standards. For reference we also took a false-colour image by using a spectralon grey standard and the described method (Fig. 24 D). The result shows that the automatically exposed false- colour picture without adjustments has a green colour cast (Fig. 24 A). The camera overexposed the blue channel because the flower and the background don’t reflect a lot of blue light. Because of the blue filter the camera sees a dark picture and tries to compensate by overexposing the picture. This leads to a green colour cast in the false-colour image (Fig. 24 A). This problem can be solved by not using the blue and green filter and instead splitting the normal RGB image that were also taken by using the automatic exposure (Fig. 24 B). The resulting image looks similar to the false-colour picture taken with the grey reference standard (Fig. 24 D). This method FIGURE 18: Colour pictures (left), UV-pictures (middle) and however can still lead to colour casts since camera sees a dark false-colour pictures (right) of Centaurea cyanus L. (A, B, C), picture through the UV filter and compensates by Hibiscus trionum L. (D, E, F), Saxifraga rotundifolia L. (G, H, I) overexposing. and Anemone nemorosa L. (J, K, L).

112 LUNAU ET AL. J Poll Ecol 23(12)

(Dyer et al. 2008). Naïve and inexperienced bees’ innate colour preferences guide them towards promising flowers (Lunau & Maier 1995; Lunau et al. 1996). Bees exhibit a preference for flowers displaying colour contrast against the background (Lunau et al. 1996), flower colours of high bee- subjective saturation (Lunau et al. 1996), and prefer distinct colour hues, bee-subjective blue in the first place (Giurfa et al. 1995; Gumbert 2000; Milet-Pinheiro et al. 2015). When bees have approached a distinct flower, floral colour patterns are important for the flower to guide the bees towards a suitable site for the first physical contact with the flower by means of the antennae, landing (Lunau et al. 1996) and proboscis extension (Wilmsen et al. 2017). Floral colour patterns may include small-sized structures such as floral guides, veins, staminodes, stamens, anthers, filamental hairs and others (Lunau & Wester 2017; Lunau et al. 2017). False colour images show flower colour patterns as seen by bees although the bee colours are visualized as human visible colours, UV as blue, blue as green and green as red (Kevan 1978, 1983; Kevan and Backhaus 1998). False colour images of flowers in bee view have been introduced several times (Kevan 1972; Vorobyev et al. 1997; Chittka and Kevan 2005; Benitez-Vieyra et al. 2007; Williams & Dyer 2007; Brito et al. 2015; Hempel de Ibarra et al. 2015). In this study, the

FIGURE 19. Colour pictures (left), UV-pictures (middle) and false-colour pictures (right) of Lotus tetragonolobus L. (A, B, C), Rhododendron forrestii B. Balfour ex Diels (D, E, F), Papaver rhoeas L. (G, H, I), Pedicularis gruina Franch ex Maxim. (J, K, L), Galanthus nivalis L. (M, N, O) Solanum dulcamara L. (P, Q, R).

Using the styrofoam as a reference standard dramatically improves the results using the colour filters (Fig. 24 C). The false-colour image looks very similar to the false-colour picture taken with the grey reference standard (Fig. 24 D).

DISCUSSION In this study a low-cost and easy-to-use multispectral imaging equipment is proposed and the advantages of false colour imaging over UV-photography and spectrophotometry are explained with some examples. Our equipment was proved to be effective to detect new floral color patterns and to be efficiently working in a high elevation mountain region in the Himalayas. The equipment is configured for false colour photography in bee view. Bees use colour cues of flowers to find nectar and pollen sources (von Frisch 1915; Dyer at al. 2014; Orban & Plowright 2014). Experiments with honeybees and FIGURE 20. Colour pictures (left), UV-pictures (middle) and bumblebees have shown that flower visits consist of a series of false-colour pictures (right) of flowers taken under field conditions behavioural reactions mostly based on visual cues. Initially in Lijiang (Yunnan, China). Leontopodium sp. (A, B, C), Pedicularis bees are colour blind in their search for flowers in that they cephalantha Franch. ex Maxim. (D, E, F), Tibetia yunnanensis process only input to the green type of photoreceptors (Giurfa (Franch.) Tsui (G, H, I), Primula poissonii Franch. (J, K, L), Salvia et al. 1996). Only if the visual angle viewing the target flower hylocharis Diels (M, N, O), Paris polyphylla var. yunnanensis surpasses a threshold the bees use colour vision to find flowers (Franch.) Hand.-Mazz (P, Q, R).

June 2018 FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY IN BEE VIEW 113

green ones (Kevan et al. 2001). Rarely the bees’ lower sensitivity in the red range of wavelength has brought into focus (Cure & Wittmann 1990; Chittka & Waser 1997; Lunau et al. 2011; Bergamo et al. 2016). The fact that bees are red blind or less sensitive to red light has caused lot of confusion about how bees might perceive red colours (Hinton 1976; Johnson & Midgley 2001; Rodríguez-Gironés & Santamaría 2004; Martínez-Harms et al. 2010). It was found that some bees have developed sensitivity in the red range of wavelength (Cure & Wittmann 1990; Chittka & Waser 1997) or the red colour of flowers is used to exclude bee as potentially nectar-robbing flower visitors (Lunau et al. 2011; Bergamo et al. 2016), since bees see red colours without additional reflection in the UV as black. Sensory exclusion of bees was also proposed for white, UV-reflecting bird- pollinated flowers, since UV-reflecting white colours are not spontaneously preferred by bees (Lunau et al. 1996; 2011). In the Lousewort Pedicularis gruina a similar phenomenon is demonstrated for the buds of the violet flowers which display the white, UV-reflecting underside of the petals. The Snowdrop Galanthus nivalis represents the common type of white bee-pollinated flowers (Kevan et al. 1996). In the Red Poppy Papaver rhoeas (Papaveraceae) the red flowers in the Eastern Mediterranean are pollinated by red sensitive glaphyrid beetles (Martínez-Harms et al. 2012), whereas the FIGURE 21: Colour pictures (left), UV-pictures (middle) and flowers in the Western part of Europe have evolved reflection false-colour pictures (right) of flowers taken under field conditions in the red range of wavelength are pollinated by bees in Lijiang and Shangri-La (Yunnan, China). Saxifraga signata Engl. (Burkhardt 1997). et Irmsch. (A, B, C), Potentilla griffithii var. velutina Card. (D, E, F), Pedicularis longiflora Rudolph (G, H, I), Pedicularis tricolor Hand.- Floral colour change is known as a semi-honest indicator Mazz. (J, K, L), Trollius yunnanensis (Franch.) Ulbr. (M, N, O). of floral nectar reward (Lunau & Wester 2017). The fact that the flowering phase offering reward is attributed with a more attractive colour (Lunau 1996) is nicely demonstrated with the false-colour picture of Myosotis arvensis. In the Lungwort both the pre-change as well as the post-change flower colour seem to be attractive to bees (Oberrath & Böhning-Gaese 1999) which is supported by false-colour pictures of a related Pulmonaria species. However, false-colour pictures might reveal yet unknown details of floral colour changes (Weiss & Lamont 1997; Ohashi et al. 2015). The bee view might be enhanced if the optical resolution is also considered (Rodríguez-Gironés & Ruiz 2016; Lunau & Verhoeven 2017). One typical pattern is the so-called ultraviolet bull’s eye (Silberglied 1979; Schlangen et al. 2009; Koski & Ashman 2014) of all-yellow flowers displaying a UV-reflecting FIGURE 22. False-colour picture of Myosotis arvensis L. periphery and a UV-absorbing center part either by pollen, merged from photos taken with UV, blue and green filter (A), anthers of pollen-mimicking floral guides (Lunau 2000, merged from photos taken with UV filter and the UV-/IR-Cut filter 2007; Lunau et al. 2017). In this study an all-yellow flower split into colour channels (B), Adobe Lightroom and Photoshop (C), displaying a blue bull’s eye as well as all-yellow flowers and ImageJ and MICA Toolbox (D). displaying a bee-visible colour pattern with different shades of yellow were found. It is known that the centre part of floral advantages of false colour imaging over UV-photography and colour pattern directs the bees’ approaches towards the first spectrophotometry are clearly demonstrated by multiple physical contact and landing site of flowers due to higher examples, especially by the undetected colour pattern of sub- colour saturation and colour contrast against the corolla alpine and alpine flora on the high mountain of eastern (Lunau et al. 1996; Papiorek et al. 2013; Wilmsen et al. Himalayas in China. 2017). In many cases the ultraviolet picture does not indicate Interpreting flowers as seen by bees requires the the higher salience of the flowers’ centre part rather than the consideration of ultraviolet and bee-visible ranges of false-colour picture. Even the absence of UV-reflection wavelength (Kevan 1978, 1979, 1983), although ultraviolet pattern is of significance for flower visitors, e.g. for yellow reflections in general are not more important than blue or bird-pollinated flowers (Papiorek et al. 2013), and for yellow

114 LUNAU ET AL. J Poll Ecol 23(12)

FIGURE 23. Colour pictures (A-D) and false-colour pictures (E-H) of the Pilewort Ficaria verna L. (Ranunculaceae) taken with different camera equipment which was Panasonic GH-1 camera and Pentax 85 mm Ultra Achromatic Takumar lens (A, E), Panasonic GH-1 camera and 105 mm UV- Nikkor lens (B, F), Nikon D40 camera and 18-55 mm Nikkor lens (C,G), and Nikon D40 camera and 105 mm UV-Nikkor lens (D, H).

FIGURE 24: Bellis perennis A: False-colour picture taken with the camera set to auto and using the colour filters. The picture was assembled without any adjustments. B: False-colour picture taken with the camera set to auto. The picture was assembled without any adjustments and by splitting the RGB image. C: Before merging the pictures to the false-colour picture the brightness of the pictures were adjusted so that the styrofoam has the exact same brightness in every colour channel (white balance). D: False-colour image assembled by using the spectralon grey reference standard.

Crocus species (Lunau et al. 2016). A common type of Ma et al. 2015; Zhang et al. 2017; Tao et al. 2018; Yan et al. colour pattern in all-yellow flowers is the combination of an 2018). Unusual flower colour patterns were detected in the orange floral guide with a yellow periphery without any UV- bumblebee buzz-pollinated Pedicularis and indicated a pattern like in the California Poppy Eschscholzia californica highly diverse signal system in this genus. On the mountains (Papaveraceae) and Common Toadflax Linaria vulgaris of southwestern China, Pedicularis species exhibit a (Plantaginaceae). It is still an open question whether and how spectacular level of endemism and floral diversity (> 300 this colour pattern is able to direct flower visitors towards the species; Yang et al. 1998). The different Pedicularis species centre part of the flower (Lunau 1992). If the spectral are pollinated by the same bumblebee species, but the floral reflectance in the ultraviolet and blue ranges of wavelengths mechanisms that ensure pollen deposits and pickups on are negligible the orange colour might appear darker to the different parts of the pollinator’s body ("Pedicularis-type" of bees than the yellow colour and thus appear more bee- mechanical isolation) were well characterized in a few co- subjective saturated, since darker colours appear at least to existing species in China (Wang & Li 2005; Tong & Huang humans more saturated than lighter colours of similar chroma 2016, 2018; Liang et al. 2018). However, the visual and (Lübbe 2010). olfactory signal aspects of this model system have not been explored yet. The false-colour images will be helpful for plant- pollinator interaction studies in these underexplored regions, The flora of China is extremely diverse with more than such as Himalayas. We demonstrated the diversity of floral 30,000 species of angiosperms (Wu et al. 2013). The colours on the high elevation mountains in Lijiang and mountains of southwest China are regarded as one of global Shangri-La, eastern Himalayas. These flowers of the Chinese hotspots of biodiversity with the highest diversity of species flora reveal a large diversity of bee-visible colour pattern which and endemism of all temperate floras (Myers et al. 2000). The has not yet been described to our knowledge despite high floral diversity of high elevation mountains of this region is intensity of studies in visual aspects of pollination biology in strongly shaped by generalist bee species, such as Chinese China (Shi et al. 2009; Eaton et al. 2012; Huang et al. 2015; native honeybee Apis cerana and bumblebees (Ren et al. 2014;

June 2018 FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY IN BEE VIEW 115

Zhao et al. 2016). This region harbours the half of global Ranunculidae, Hamamelididae, Caryophyllidae, Rosidae. species of bumblebees (Williams et al. 2009). The limited Tropische und Subtropische Pflanzenwelt 87:1-193. number of false-colour photos presented in this study indicate Burr B, Rosen D, Barthlott W (1995) Untersuchungen zur that the diverse bee pollination system in Himalayas requires Ultraviolettreflexion von Angiospermenblüten III. Dilleniidae und more researches on their visual signals in the future. Our low- Asteridae s.l.. Tropische und Subtropische Pflanzenwelt 93:1-185. cost and easy-to-use multispectral imaging equipment will be Chen P-J, Awata H, Matsushita A, Yang E-C, Arikawa K (2016) helpful for the future when we explore these understudied Extreme spectral richness in the eye of the Common Bluebottle areas. Butterfly, Graphium sarpedon. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 4:18. In conclusion, this study shows that false colour images of Chittka L, Kevan PG (2005) Flower colour as advertisement. In: flowers in bee view provide similar information as classical Dafni A, Kevan PG, Husband BC (eds) Practical Pollination UV-photos and additional information about the floral Biology. Enviroquest Ltd., Cambridge, ON, Canada, pp 157-196. colour pattern that is not available from UV-photos. The false Chittka L, Menzel R (1992) The evolutionary adaptation of flower colour images of flowers are easy to interpret due to the colors and the insect pollinators' color vision systems. Journal of consistent bathochromic shift for about 100nm during the Comparative Physiology A 171:171-181. reassembling to the false colour image resulting that all kinds Chittka L, Shmida A, Troje N, Menzel R (1994) Ultraviolet as a of bee-visible colour patterns are seen as patterns rather than component of flower reflections, and the colour perception of uniform colours. This also holds for very tiny structure like Hymenoptera. Vision Research 34:1489-1508. pollen grains, anthers, filamental hairs, and veins which is Chittka L, Waser NM (1997). Why red flowers are not invisible for advantageous compared to spectrophotometry. bees. Israel Journal of Plant Sciences 45:169-183. Clark C (1979) Ultraviolet absorption by flowers of the APPENDICES Eschscholzioideae (Papaveraceae). Madrono 26:22-25. Additional supporting information may be found in the online Cure JR, Wittmann D (1990) Callonychium petuniae, a new version of this article: panurgine bee species (Apoidea, Andrenidae) oligolectic on Petunia (Solanaceae). Studies on Neotropical Fauna & Environment APPENDIX I. Estimated costs for a low-budget version and an optimal equipment for false colour photography in bee view. 25:153-156. Daumer, K (1958) Blumenfarben, wie sie die Bienen sehen. REFERENCES Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie 41:49-110. Dyer AG, Boyd-Gerny S, Mcloughlin S, Rosa MGP, Simonov V, Arikawa K (2003) Spectral organization of the eye of a butterfly, Wong BBM (2012) Parallel evolution of angiosperm colour Papilio. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 189:791-800. signals: common evolutionary pressures linked to hymenopteran Arnold SEJ, Faruq S, Savolainen V, McOwan PW, Chittka L (2010) vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences FReD: The Floral Reflectance Database — A web portal for 279:3606-3615. analyses of flower colour. PLoS ONE 5(12): e14287. Dyer AG, Spaethe J, Prack S (2008) Comparative psychophysics of Backhaus W (1991) Color opponent coding in the visual system of bumblebee and honeybee colour discrimination and object the honeybee. Vision Research 31:1381-1397. detection. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 194:617-627. Benitez-Vieyra S, Hempel de Ibarra N, Wertlen AM, Cocucci AA Eaton DAR, Fenster CB, Hereford J, Huang SQ, Ree RH (2012) (2007) How to look like a mallow: evidence of floral mimicry Floral diversity and community structure in Pedicularis between Turneraceae and Malvaceae. Proceedings of the Royal (Orobanchaceae). Ecology 93:182-194. Society B Biological Sciences 274:2239-2248. Eisner T, Silberglied RE, Aneshansley D, Carrel JE, Howland HC Bergamo PJ, Rech AR, Brito VLG, Sazima M (2016) Flower colour (1969) Ultraviolet video-viewing: the television camera as an insect and visitation rates of Costus arabicus support the "bee avoidance" eye. Science 166:1172-1174. hypothesis for red-reflecting hummingbird-pollinated flowers. Garcia JE, Greentree AD, Shrestha M, Dorin A, Dyer AG (2014) Functional Ecology 30:710-720. Flower colours through the lens: Quantitative measurement with Biedinger N, Barthlott W (1993) Untersuchungen zur visible and ultraviolet digital photography. PLoS ONE Ultraviolettreflexion von Angiospermenblüten. I 9(5):e96646. Monocotyledonae. Tropische und Subtropische Pflanzenwelt Giurfa M, Núñez J, Chittka L, Menzel R (1995) Colour preferences 86:1-122. of flower-naive honeybees. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Bowmaker JK, Dartnall HJ (1980) Visual pigments of rods and 177:247-259. cones in a human retina. The Journal of Physiology 298:501-511. Giurfa M, Vorobyev M, Kevan P, Menzel R (1996) Detection of Briscoe AD (2008) Reconstructing the ancestral butterfly eye: focus coloured stimuli by honeybees: minimum visual angles and receptor on the opsins. Journal of Experimental Biology 211:1805-1813. specific contrasts. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 178:699- Brito VLG, Weynans K, Sazima M, Lunau K (2015) Trees as huge 709. flowers and flowers as oversized floral guides: the role of floral Gronquist M, Bezzerides A, Attygalle A, Meinwald J, Eisner M, color change and retention of old flowers in Tibouchina pulchra. Eisner T (2001) Attractive and defensive functions of the Frontiers in Plant Science 6:362. ultraviolet pigments of a flower (Hypericum calycinum). Burkhardt D (1997) Die Welt mit anderen Augen—Wie Insekten Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98:13745- und Vögel die Welt und ihre Farben sehen. Biologie in unserer Zeit 13750. 19:37-46. Guldberg LD, Atsatt PR (1975) Frequency of reflection and Burr B, Barthlott W (1993) Untersuchungen zur absorption of ultraviolet light in flowering plants. The American Ultraviolettreflexion von Angiospermenblüten II. Magnoliidae, Midland Naturalist 93:35-43.

116 LUNAU ET AL. J Poll Ecol 23(12)

Gumbert A (2000) Color choices by bumble bees (Bombus Koski MH, Ashman TL (2014) Dissecting pollinator responses to a terrestris): innate preferences and generalization after learning. ubiquitous ultraviolet floral pattern in the wild. Functional Ecology Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 48:36-43. 28:868-877. He Q, Shen Y, Wang M, Huang M, Yang R, Zhu S, Wang L, Wu Koski MH, Ashman TL (2015a). Floral pigmentation patterns R (2011) Natural variation in petal color in Lycoris longituba provide an example of Gloger’s rule in plants. Nature Plants revealed by anthocyanin components. PLoS ONE 6(8): e22098. 1:14007. Hempel de Ibarra N, Langridge KV, Vorobyev M (2015) More than Koski MH, Ashman TL (2015b) An altitudinal cline in UV floral colour attraction: behavioural functions of flower patterns. Current pattern corresponds with a behavioral change of a generalist Opinion in Insect Science 12:64e70. pollinator assemblage. Ecology 96:3343-3353. Hempel de Ibarra N, Vorobyev M (2009) Flower patterns are Liang H, Ren ZX, Tao ZB, Bernhardt B, Li DZ, Wang H. (2018) adapted for detection by bees. Journal of Comparative Physiology Impact of pre- and post-pollination barriers on pollen transfer and A 195:319-323. reproductive isolation among three sympatric Pedicularis Herrera G, Zagal JC, Diaz M, Fernández MJ Vielma A, Cure M, (Orobanchaceae) species. Plant Biology; DOI:10.1111/plb.12833 Martinez J, Bozinovic F, Palacios AG (2008) Spectral sensitivities Lübbe E (2010) Colours in the Mind - Colour Systems in Reality - of photoreceptors and their role in colour discrimination in the A formula for colour saturation. [Book on Demand]. green-backed firecrown hummingbird (Sephanoides sephaniodes). Lunau K (1992) Innate recognition of flowers by bumblebees – Journal of Comparative Physiology A 194:785-794. orientation of antennae to visual stamen signals. Canadian Journal Hinton HE (1976) Possible significance of the red patches of the of Zoology 70:2139-2144. female crab-, Misumena vatia. Journal of Zoology 180:35- Lunau K (1995) Notes on the colour of pollen. Plant Systematics 39. and Evolution 198:235-252. Huang Y, Li N, Ren ZX, Chen G, Wu ZK, Ma YP (2015) Lunau K (1996) Unidirectionality of floral colour changes. Plant Reproductive biology of Primula beesiana (Primulaceae), an alpine Systematics and Evolution 200:125-140. species endemic to Southwest China. Plant Ecology and Evolution 148:289-296. Lunau K (2000) The ecology and evolution of visual pollen signals. Plant Systematics and Evolution 222:89-111. Johnson SD, Midgley JJ. (2001) Pollination by monkey beetles Lunau K (2007) Stamens and mimic stamens as components of floral (Scarabaeidae: Hopliini): do colour and dark centres of flowers influence alighting behavior? Environmental Entomology 30:861- colour patterns. Botanische Jahrbücher für Systematik, 868. Pflanzengeschichte und Pflanzengeographie 127:13-41. Junker RR, Blüthgen N (2010) Floral scents repel facultative flower Lunau K (2014) Visual ecology of flies with particular reference to visitors, but attract obligate ones. Annals of Botany 105:777-782. colour vision and colour preferences. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 200:497-512. Kevan PG, Chittka L, Dyer AG (2001) Limits to the salience of ultraviolet: Lessons from color vision in bees and birds. Journal of Lunau K, Konzmann S, Bossems J, Harpke D (2016) A matter of Experimental Biology 204:2571-2580. contrast: Yellow flower colour constrains style length in Crocus species. PLoS ONE 11(4):e0154728. Kelber A, Balkenius A, Warrant EJ (2002) Scotopic colour vision in nocturnal hawkmoths. Nature 419:922-925. Lunau K, Konzmann S, Winter L, Kamphausen V, Ren ZX (2017) Pollen and stamen mimicry: the alpine flora as a case study. Kevan PG (1972) Floral colors in the high arctic with reference to -Plant Interactions 11:427-447. insect-flower relations and pollination. Canadian Journal of Botany 50:2289-2316. Lunau K, Maier EJ (1995) Innate colour preferences of flower visitors. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 177:1-19. Kevan PG (1978) Floral coloration, its colorimetric and significance in anthecology. In: Richards AJ (ed.) The pollination of flowers by Lunau K, Papiorek S, Eltz T, Sazima M (2011) Avoidance of Insects. Linnean Society Symposium Series Vol. 6:51-78. achromatic colours by bees provides a private niche for hummingbirds. Journal of Experimental Biology 214:1607-1612. Kevan PG (1979) Vegetation and floral colours revealed by ultraviolet light: interpretational difficulties for functional Lunau K, Verhoeven C (2017) Wie Bienen Blumen sehen - significance. American Journal of Botany 66:749-751. Falschfarbenaufnahmen von Blüten. Biologie in unserer Zeit 47:120-127. Kevan P (1983) Floral colours through the insect eye: what they are Lunau K, Wacht S, Chittka L (1996) Colour choices of naive and what they mean. In: Jones CE, Little RJ (eds.) Handbook of Experimental Pollination Biology. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New bumble bees and their implications for colour perception. Journal York, pp 3-30. of Comparative Physiology A 178:477-489. Kevan PG, Backhaus WG (1998) Color vision: ecology and Lunau K, Wester P (2017) Mimicry and deception in pollination. evolution in making the best of the photic environment. In: In: Becard G (ed.) Advances in Botanical Research, Vol. 82, How Backhaus W, Kliegl R, Werner J (eds.) Color vision: perspectives Plants Communicate with their Biotic Environment; pp 259-279; from different disciplines. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 163-183. Academic Press, Amsterdam. Kevan PG, Chittka L, Dyer AG (2001) Limits to the salience of Ma XK, Shi J, Bänziger H, Sun YN, Guo YY, Liu ZJ, Johnson SD, ultraviolet: lessons from colour vision in bees and birds. Journal of Luo YB (2015) The functional significance of complex floral Experimental Biology 204:2571-2580. colour pattern in a food-deceptive orchid. Functional Ecology 30:721-732. Kevan P G, Giurfa M, Chittka L (1996) Why are there so many and so few white flowers? Trends in Plant Sciences 1:280-284. Martínez-Harms J, Palacios AG, Márquez N, Estay P, Arroyo MTK, Mpodozis J (2010) Can red flowers be conspicuous to bees? Koski MH, Ashman TL (2013) Quantitative variation, heritability, Bombus dahlbomii and South American temperate forest flowers and trait correlations for ultraviolet floral traits in Argentina as a case in point. Journal of Experimental Biology 213:564-571. anserina (Rosaceae): implications for floral evolution. International Journal of Plant Sciences 174:1109-1120. Martínez-Harms J, Vorobyev M, Schorn J, Shmida A, Keasar T, Homberg U, Schmeling F, Menzel R (2012) Evidence of red sensitive photoreceptors in Pygopleurus israelitus (Glaphyridae:

June 2018 FALSE-COLOUR PHOTOGRAPHY IN BEE VIEW 117

Coleoptera) and its implications for beetle pollination in the Silberglied RE (1979) Communication in the ultraviolet. Annual southeast Mediterranean. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Review of Ecology and Systematics 10:373-398. 198:451-463. Stavenga DG, van der Kooi CJ (2016) Coloration of the Chilean Medel R, Botto-Mahan C, Kalin-Arroyo M (2003) Pollinator Bellflower, Nolana paradoxa, interpreted with a scattering and mediated selection on the nectar guide phenotype in the Andean absorbing layer stack model. Planta 243:171-181. monkeyflower, Mimulus luteus. Ecology 84:1721-1732. Stockman A, Macleod D, Johnson N (1993) Spectral sensitivities of Milet-Pinheiro P, Ayasse M, Dötterl S (2015) Visual and olfactory the human cones. Journal of the Optical Society of America floral cues of Campanula (Campanulaceae) and their significance 10:2491-2521. for host recognition by an oligolectic bee pollinator. PLoS ONE Tao ZB, Ren ZX, Bernhardt P, Liang H, Li HD, Zhao YH, Wang 10(6):e0128577. H, Li DZ (2018) Does reproductive isolation reflect the Myers N, Mittermeier RA, Mittermeier CG, da Fonseca GAB, Kent segregation of color forms in the Spiranthes sinensis (Pers.) Ames J (2000) Biodiversity hotspots for conservation priorities. Nature complex (Orchidaceae) in the Chinese Himalayas? Ecology and 403:853-858. Evolution; DOI:10.1002/ece3.4067 Neumeyer C (1981) Chromatic adaptation in the honey bee: Tong ZY, Huang SQ (2016) Pre- and post-pollination interaction Successive color contrast and color constancy. Journal of between six co-flowering Pedicularis species via heterospecific Comparative Physiology 144:543-553. pollen transfer. New Phytologist 211:1452-1461. Oberrath R, Böhning-Gaese K (1999) Floral color change and the Tong ZY, Huang SQ (2018) Safe sites of pollen placement: a attraction of insect pollinators in lungwort (Pulmonaria collina). conflict of interest between plants and bees? Oecologia 186:163- Oecologia 121:383-391. 171. Ohashi K, Makino TT, Arikawa K (2015) Floral colour change in Troscianko J, Stevens M (2015) Picture calibration and analysis the eyes of pollinators: testing possible constraints and correlated toolbox – a free software suite for objectively measuring reflectance, evolution. Functional Ecology 29:1144-1155. colour and pattern. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 6:1320- Orbán LL, Plowright CMS (2014) Getting to the start line: how 1331. bumblebees and honeybees are visually guided towards their first Vasas V, Hanley D, Kevan PG, Chittka L (2017) Multispectral floral contact. Insectes Sociaux 61:325-336. images of flowers reveal the adaptive significance of using long- Papiorek S, Junker RR, Alves-dos-Santos I, Melo GAR, Amaral- wavelength-sensitive receptors for edge detection in bees. Journal Neto LP , Sazima M, Wolowski M, Freitas L, Lunau K (2016) of Comparative Physiology A 203:301-311. Bees, birds and yellow flowers: Pollinator-dependent convergent Von Frisch K (1915) Der Farbensinn und Formensinn der Biene. evolution of UV-patterns. Plant Biology 18:46-55. Zoologische Jahrbücher Abteilung für allgemeine Zoologie und Peitsch D, Fietz A, Hertel H, de Souza J, Ventura DF, Menzel R Physiologie 37:1-187. (1992) The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and Vorobyev M, Kunze J, Gumbert A, Giurfa M, Menzel R (1997) their receptor-based colour vision. Journal of Comparative Flowers through the insect eyes. Israel Journal of Plant Science Physiology A, 170:23-40. 45:93-102. Peterson ML, Miller TJ, Kay KM (2015) An ultraviolet floral Vorobyev M, Marshall J, Osorio D, Hempel de Ibarra N, Menzel R polymorphism associated with life history drives pollinator (1999) Colourful objects through eyes. Colour Research discrimination in Mimulus guttatus. American Journal of Botany and Application 26: 214-217. 102:396-406. Wang H, Li DZ (2005) Pollination biology of four Pedicularis Primack RB (1982) Ultraviolet patterns in flowers, or flowers as species (Scrophulariaceae) in northwestern Yunnan, China. Annals viewed by insects. Arnoldia 42: 139-146. of the Missouri Botanical Garden 92:127-138. Ren ZX, Wang H, Bernhardt P, Li DZ (2014) Insect pollination Weiss MR, Lamont BB (1997) Floral color change and insect and self-incompatibility in edible and/or medicinal crops in pollination: A dynamic relationship. Israel Journal of Plant Science southwestern China, a global hotspot of biodiversity. American 45:185-200. Journal of Botany 101:1700-1710. White TE, Dalrymple RL, Noble DWA, O'Hanlon JC, Zurek DB, Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Ruiz A (2016) toBeeView: A program for Umbers KDL (2015) Reproducible research in the study of simulating the retinal picture of visual scenes on nonhuman eyes. biological coloration. Animal Behaviour 106:51-57. Ecology and Evolution 6:7892-7900. Wikipedia (2. März 2017). CIE 1931 color space. Accessed 9 Rodríguez-Gironés MA, Santamaría L (2004) Why are so many bird March 2017 from Wikipedia: flowers red? PLoS Biology 2(10):e350. en.wikipedia.org/wiki/CIE_1931_color_space Rosen D, Barthlott W (1991) Ökologische Aspekte der Williams S, Dyer A (2007) A photographic simulation of insect Ultraviolett-Reflexion von Blumen in Mitteleuropa, besonders in vision. The Journal of Ophthalmic Photography 29:10-14. der Eifel. Decheniana 144:72-112. Williams P, Tang Y, Yao J, Cameron S (2009) The bumblebees of Schlangen K, Miosic S, Castro A, Freudmann K, Luczkiewicz M, Sichuan (Hymenoptera: Apidae, Bombini). Systematics and Vitzthum F, Schwab W, Gamsjäger S, Musso M, Halbwirth H Biodiversity 7:101-189. (2009) Formation of UV-honey guides in Rudbeckia hirta. Wilmsen S, Gottlieb R, Junker RR, Lunau K (2017) Bumblebees Phytochemistry 70:889-898. require visual pollen stimuli to initiate and multimodal stimuli to Shi J, Luo YB, Bernhardt P, Ran JC, Liu ZJ, Zhou Q (2009) complete a full behavioral sequence in close-range flower Pollination by deceit in Paphiopedilum barbigerum (Orchidaceae): orientation. Ecology and Evolution 7:1384-1393. a staminode exploits the innate colour preferences of hoverflies Winter Y, Lopez J, von Helversen O (2003) Ultraviolet vision in a (Syrphidae). Plant Biology 11:17-28. bat. Nature 425:612-614. Silberglied RE (1976) Visualization and recording of longwave Wu ZY, Raven PH, Hong DY (2013) Flora of China Vol. 1: ultraviolet reflection from natural objects. Functional Photography Introduction. Science Press, Beijing, China and Missouri Botanical 11:20-29; 30-33. Garden Press, St. Louis, USA.

118 LUNAU ET AL. J Poll Ecol 23(12)

Yan J, Wang G, Sui Y, Wang ML, Zhang L (2016) Pollinator Press, Beijing, China and Missouri Botanical Garden Press, St. responses to floral colour change, nectar, and scent promote Louis, USA. reproductive fitness in Quisqualis indica (Combretaceae). Scientific Zhang C, Vereecken NJ, Wang LL, Tian B, Dafni A, Yang YP, Reports 6:24408. Duan YW (2017) Are nectar guide colour changes a reliable signal Yan J, Wang ML, Zhang L (2018) Light induces petal color change to pollinators that enhances reproductive success? Plant Ecology & in Quisqualis indica (Combretaceae). Plant Diversity 40:28-34. Diversity 10:89-96. Yang HB, Holmgren NH, Mill RR (1998) Pedicularis. In: Wu ZY, Raven PH, Hong DY (2013) Flora of China, Vol 18: Science

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.