Estimation of Average Treatment Effects

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Estimation of Average Treatment Effects Estimation of Average Treatment Effects Honors Thesis Peter Zhang Abstract The estimation of average treatment effects is an important issue in economic evaluations of the impact of policy intervention on job employment and the effect of education and training on income. This honors thesis is concerned with studying different approaches to the estimation of an average treatment effect. Motivated by the works of Khandker, Koolwal, and Samad (2010) and Kreif, Grieve, Radice, and Sekhon (2013), we consider complete-case method, inverse probability weighting and propensity score matching methods using the propensity score, the regression method using two treatment-specific regression models, augmented inverse probability weighting and general- ized method of moments methods using both the propensity score and treatment-specific regression models. These methods yield six different estimates of average treatment effects. Our empirical study is an application of these six estimates to the assessment of the National Supported Work job-training program; our analysis shows a positive impact of this job-training program with higher annual earnings for those participating in the program. We conduct a Monte Carlo simulation study that investigates these six estimates’ performance. Overall, we find that the regression and general- ized method of moments approaches produce least biased and most efficient estimates of an average treatment effect; yet the resulting estimates are most robust against functional form misspecification of the propensity score and treatment-specific regression models. 1. Introduction Causality, i.e., the relationship between cause and effect, is a topic of great significance in eco- nomics and related fields such as medicine, epidemiology, and health science. For example, the impact of policy intervention on job employment, the effect of education and training on income, and the effectiveness of a drug (or whether it is effective at all) on a disease are all questions of causality. One important and commonly used measure of causality is the average treatment effect (ATE) for a binary policy or treatment on a scalar outcome, which is the mean outcome difference between 1 the treatment and control groups. Economic evaluations of policy or health care interventions in many observational studies often require identification and estimation of ATEs, which is challenging because randomized experiments cannot always be implemented. The potential outcome framework, described in Khandker, Koolwal, and Samad (2010), provides a useful way of identifying and evaluating ATEs in observational studies. Under this conceptual framework, the problem of estimating an ATE becomes a missing data problem due to the absence of data on counterfactual outcomes (namely, outcomes from treated units had they not been exposed to the treatment). The presence of missing data in causal inference often leads to potential selection bias and thus complicates the relationship between cause and effect. To account for potential selection bias in treatment (program) participation that might affect evaluation of ATEs, policy makers need to isolate the effect of the policy intervention on outcomes from other observed and unobserved confounding factors affecting the outcomes. This implies that the evaluation of ATEs in observational studies often requires adjustment for differences in baseline covariates because treatment and control groups can be unbalanced on measured or unmeasured covariates. As discussed in Khandker, Koolwal, and Samad (2010), there are a number of different approaches in the literature to handling missing counterfactuals in impact evaluation theory. Rosenbaum and Rubin (1983) introduce the notion of propensity score (PS) defined as the probability of assignment to treatment conditional on covariates. The PS gives rise to two different methods of estimating ATEs, the inverse probability weighting (IPW) method and the propensity score matching (PSM); both of which control the differences between treatment and control groups by adjusting baseline covariates and thus reduce the selection bias. The IPW method weighs each complete outcome by the inverse probability of itself being observed, whereas the PSM method constructs a matching counterfactual or control group that is as similar to the treatment group as possible in terms of observed covariates based on PS. Instead of modeling the probability of assignment on covariates, the regression method estimates the ATE by modeling the outcome on covariates, as discussed in Kreif, Grieve, Radice, and Sekhon (2013). Robins, Rotnitzky, and Zhao (1994) propose a hybrid of the PS and regression methods to estimate ATEs. Their method, called the augmented inverse probability weighting (AIPW) method, is doubly robust (DR) in the sense that it renders consistent estimates of ATEs if either one of the PS or regression models is correctly specified, but not necessarily both. Compared to the PS and regression methods, the AIPW method is less sensitive to the 2 misspecification of the PS or regression model. For other approaches to estimating the ATE in the literature, see, for example, Khandker, Koolwal, and Samad (2010). The generalized method of moments (GMM) method, due to Hansen (1982), is an effective and widely used moments-based approach to parameter estimation in microeconometrics studies. The focus of this thesis is to explore an alternative approach to estimation of ATEs from a GMM perspective by exploiting the PS and regression models. The goal of this thesis is two-fold. The first goal is to apply the GMM-based estimate of ATEs to the program evaluation in econometrics by analyzing a real dataset from a randomized evaluation of a labor market training program, the National Supported Work (NSW) job-training program, originally analyzed by Lalonde (1986) and subsequently by Dehejia and Wahba (1999). It is also of interest to compare the GMM-based estimate of ATEs with other existing ATE estimates described in Khandker, Koolwal, and Samad (2010). Thus, our second goal is to perform a simulation study to examine the merits of the GMM-based and other related methods. This thesis is ordered as follows. Section 2 describes various methodologies of estimating ATEs. The results of the analysis of the NSW job-training data are reported in Section 3. Section 4 presents simulations results comparing various ATE estimates. Finally, the thesis is concluded with discussion in Section 5. 2. Methodology For the ith participant with i = 1,...,n, let Ti represent the treatment assignment taking value 1 if participant i is treated and value 0 if participant i is not treated. Furthermore, let Yi(1) denote the potential outcome under treatment and Yi(0) denote the potential outcome when there is no treatment; then the ATE is given by ATE = E[Yi(1) − Yi(0)]. The potential outcomes Y1(1) and Yi(0) cannot be observed simultaneously for each participant i; instead, we can only observe Yi = TiYi(1)+ (1 − Ti)Yi(0), the observed outcome from participant i. In addition to observe Ti and Yi, we can also observe Xi, a set of other baseline covariates or characteristics of individual i, such as his or her household and local environment. Thus, the observed data is composed of (Ti,Yi,Xi) for i = 1,...,n. Without adjustment for baseline covariates, a complete-case estimator of ATE is 3 simply the mean difference in observed outcomes by treatment status: n n i=1 TiYi i=1(1 − Ti)Yi ATECC = n − n . P i=1 Ti P i=1(1 − Ti) d P P It is widely known that ATECC is biased in observational studies and requires adjustment for differ- ences in baseline covariates.d One important assumption in causal inference is the unconfoundedness or conditional indepen- dence assumption (Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983), which states that conditional on a set of observable covariates Xi that are not affected by treatment, the potential outcomes Yi(1) and Yi(0) are indepen- dent of treatment assignment Ti; symbolically, the unconfoundedness assumption can be expressed as (Yi(1),Yi(0))⊥ Ti|Xi. Under the unconfoundedness assumption, the propensity score, which is the probability of treatment assignment conditional on covariates, is given by p(Xi)= P (Ti =1|Xi)= P (Ti =1|Yi(1),Yi(0),Xi). As in Kreif, Grieve, Radice, and Sekhon (2013), a commonly adopted parametric model for PS is the logistic regression model given by T eη Xi p(Xi, η)= T , 1+ eη Xi where η is a vector parameter and can be estimated by the maximum likelihood estimateη ˆ based on the observed data (Ti,Xi), i =1,...,n. As pointed by Kreif, Grieve, Radice, and Sekhon (2013), the IPW estimate of ATE is obtained below by reweighting the observed outcomes for treatment and control samples using the inverse of the estimated probability of the observed treatment: n n 1 1 ATE = T w Y − (1 − T )w Y , IPW n i i i n i i i Xi=1 Xi=1 d where wi = 1/p(Xi, ηˆ). The validity of this IPW estimate relies on the correct specification of the PS model p(Xi, η). Kreif, Grieve, Radice, and Sekhon (2013) also consider employing two generalized linear models g1(Xi, β1) and g0(Xi, β0) to estimate ATE as given below: n n 1 1 ATE = g (X , βˆ ) − g (X , βˆ ), REG n 1 i 1 n 0 i 0 Xi=1 Xi=1 d 4 where βˆ1 and βˆ0 are the maximum likelihood estimates or the least squares estimates of the parameter vectors β1 and β0. The validity of this regression estimate depends on the correct specification of the two treatment-specific regression models g1(Xi, β1) and g0(Xi, β0). Combining the models for the PS and for the potential outcomes, Kreif, Grieve, Radice, and Sekhon (2013) consider the following AIPW estimate of ATE: n n 1 1 ATE = T w [Y − g (X , βˆ )] − (1 − T )w [Y − g (X , βˆ )] AIPW n i i i 1 i 1 n i i i 0 i 0 Xi=1 Xi=1 d n n 1 1 + g (X , βˆ ) − g (X , βˆ ).
Recommended publications
  • Efficiency of Average Treatment Effect Estimation When the True
    econometrics Article Efficiency of Average Treatment Effect Estimation When the True Propensity Is Parametric Kyoo il Kim Department of Economics, Michigan State University, 486 W. Circle Dr., East Lansing, MI 48824, USA; [email protected]; Tel.: +1-517-353-9008 Received: 8 March 2019; Accepted: 28 May 2019; Published: 31 May 2019 Abstract: It is well known that efficient estimation of average treatment effects can be obtained by the method of inverse propensity score weighting, using the estimated propensity score, even when the true one is known. When the true propensity score is unknown but parametric, it is conjectured from the literature that we still need nonparametric propensity score estimation to achieve the efficiency. We formalize this argument and further identify the source of the efficiency loss arising from parametric estimation of the propensity score. We also provide an intuition of why this overfitting is necessary. Our finding suggests that, even when we know that the true propensity score belongs to a parametric class, we still need to estimate the propensity score by a nonparametric method in applications. Keywords: average treatment effect; efficiency bound; propensity score; sieve MLE JEL Classification: C14; C18; C21 1. Introduction Estimating treatment effects of a binary treatment or a policy has been one of the most important topics in evaluation studies. In estimating treatment effects, a subject’s selection into a treatment may contaminate the estimate, and two approaches are popularly used in the literature to remove the bias due to this sample selection. One is regression-based control function method (see, e.g., Rubin(1973) ; Hahn(1998); and Imbens(2004)) and the other is matching method (see, e.g., Rubin and Thomas(1996); Heckman et al.(1998); and Abadie and Imbens(2002, 2006)).
    [Show full text]
  • Week 10: Causality with Measured Confounding
    Week 10: Causality with Measured Confounding Brandon Stewart1 Princeton November 28 and 30, 2016 1These slides are heavily influenced by Matt Blackwell, Jens Hainmueller, Erin Hartman, Kosuke Imai and Gary King. Stewart (Princeton) Week 10: Measured Confounding November 28 and 30, 2016 1 / 176 Where We've Been and Where We're Going... Last Week I regression diagnostics This Week I Monday: F experimental Ideal F identification with measured confounding I Wednesday: F regression estimation Next Week I identification with unmeasured confounding I instrumental variables Long Run I causality with measured confounding ! unmeasured confounding ! repeated data Questions? Stewart (Princeton) Week 10: Measured Confounding November 28 and 30, 2016 2 / 176 1 The Experimental Ideal 2 Assumption of No Unmeasured Confounding 3 Fun With Censorship 4 Regression Estimators 5 Agnostic Regression 6 Regression and Causality 7 Regression Under Heterogeneous Effects 8 Fun with Visualization, Replication and the NYT 9 Appendix Subclassification Identification under Random Assignment Estimation Under Random Assignment Blocking Stewart (Princeton) Week 10: Measured Confounding November 28 and 30, 2016 3 / 176 Lancet 2001: negative correlation between coronary heart disease mortality and level of vitamin C in bloodstream (controlling for age, gender, blood pressure, diabetes, and smoking) Stewart (Princeton) Week 10: Measured Confounding November 28 and 30, 2016 4 / 176 Lancet 2002: no effect of vitamin C on mortality in controlled placebo trial (controlling for nothing) Stewart (Princeton) Week 10: Measured Confounding November 28 and 30, 2016 4 / 176 Lancet 2003: comparing among individuals with the same age, gender, blood pressure, diabetes, and smoking, those with higher vitamin C levels have lower levels of obesity, lower levels of alcohol consumption, are less likely to grow up in working class, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • STATS 361: Causal Inference
    STATS 361: Causal Inference Stefan Wager Stanford University Spring 2020 Contents 1 Randomized Controlled Trials 2 2 Unconfoundedness and the Propensity Score 9 3 Efficient Treatment Effect Estimation via Augmented IPW 18 4 Estimating Treatment Heterogeneity 27 5 Regression Discontinuity Designs 35 6 Finite Sample Inference in RDDs 43 7 Balancing Estimators 52 8 Methods for Panel Data 61 9 Instrumental Variables Regression 68 10 Local Average Treatment Effects 74 11 Policy Learning 83 12 Evaluating Dynamic Policies 91 13 Structural Equation Modeling 99 14 Adaptive Experiments 107 1 Lecture 1 Randomized Controlled Trials Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) form the foundation of statistical causal inference. When available, evidence drawn from RCTs is often considered gold statistical evidence; and even when RCTs cannot be run for ethical or practical reasons, the quality of observational studies is often assessed in terms of how well the observational study approximates an RCT. Today's lecture is about estimation of average treatment effects in RCTs in terms of the potential outcomes model, and discusses the role of regression adjustments for causal effect estimation. The average treatment effect is iden- tified entirely via randomization (or, by design of the experiment). Regression adjustments may be used to decrease variance, but regression modeling plays no role in defining the average treatment effect. The average treatment effect We define the causal effect of a treatment via potential outcomes. For a binary treatment w 2 f0; 1g, we define potential outcomes Yi(1) and Yi(0) corresponding to the outcome the i-th subject would have experienced had they respectively received the treatment or not.
    [Show full text]
  • Efficient Estimation of Average Treatment Effects Using the Estimated Propensity Score
    Econometrica, Vol. 71, No. 4 (July, 2003), 1161-1189 EFFICIENT ESTIMATION OF AVERAGE TREATMENT EFFECTS USING THE ESTIMATED PROPENSITY SCORE BY KiEisuKEHIRANO, GUIDO W. IMBENS,AND GEERT RIDDER' We are interestedin estimatingthe averageeffect of a binarytreatment on a scalar outcome.If assignmentto the treatmentis exogenousor unconfounded,that is, indepen- dent of the potentialoutcomes given covariates,biases associatedwith simple treatment- controlaverage comparisons can be removedby adjustingfor differencesin the covariates. Rosenbaumand Rubin (1983) show that adjustingsolely for differencesbetween treated and controlunits in the propensityscore removesall biases associatedwith differencesin covariates.Although adjusting for differencesin the propensityscore removesall the bias, this can come at the expenseof efficiency,as shownby Hahn (1998), Heckman,Ichimura, and Todd (1998), and Robins,Mark, and Newey (1992). We show that weightingby the inverseof a nonparametricestimate of the propensityscore, rather than the truepropensity score, leads to an efficientestimate of the averagetreatment effect. We provideintuition for this resultby showingthat this estimatorcan be interpretedas an empiricallikelihood estimatorthat efficientlyincorporates the informationabout the propensityscore. KEYWORDS: Propensity score, treatment effects, semiparametricefficiency, sieve estimator. 1. INTRODUCTION ESTIMATINGTHE AVERAGEEFFECT of a binary treatment or policy on a scalar outcome is a basic goal of manyempirical studies in economics.If assignmentto the
    [Show full text]
  • Nber Working Paper Series Regression Discontinuity
    NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES REGRESSION DISCONTINUITY DESIGNS IN ECONOMICS David S. Lee Thomas Lemieux Working Paper 14723 http://www.nber.org/papers/w14723 NATIONAL BUREAU OF ECONOMIC RESEARCH 1050 Massachusetts Avenue Cambridge, MA 02138 February 2009 We thank David Autor, David Card, John DiNardo, Guido Imbens, and Justin McCrary for suggestions for this article, as well as for numerous illuminating discussions on the various topics we cover in this review. We also thank two anonymous referees for their helpful suggestions and comments, and Mike Geruso, Andrew Marder, and Zhuan Pei for their careful reading of earlier drafts. Diane Alexander, Emily Buchsbaum, Elizabeth Debraggio, Enkeleda Gjeci, Ashley Hodgson, Yan Lau, Pauline Leung, and Xiaotong Niu provided excellent research assistance. The views expressed herein are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Bureau of Economic Research. © 2009 by David S. Lee and Thomas Lemieux. All rights reserved. Short sections of text, not to exceed two paragraphs, may be quoted without explicit permission provided that full credit, including © notice, is given to the source. Regression Discontinuity Designs in Economics David S. Lee and Thomas Lemieux NBER Working Paper No. 14723 February 2009, Revised October 2009 JEL No. C1,H0,I0,J0 ABSTRACT This paper provides an introduction and "user guide" to Regression Discontinuity (RD) designs for empirical researchers. It presents the basic theory behind the research design, details when RD is likely to be valid or invalid given economic incentives, explains why it is considered a "quasi-experimental" design, and summarizes different ways (with their advantages and disadvantages) of estimating RD designs and the limitations of interpreting these estimates.
    [Show full text]
  • Targeted Estimation and Inference for the Sample Average Treatment Effect
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Collection Of Biostatistics Research Archive University of California, Berkeley U.C. Berkeley Division of Biostatistics Working Paper Series Year 2015 Paper 334 Targeted Estimation and Inference for the Sample Average Treatment Effect Laura B. Balzer∗ Maya L. Peterseny Mark J. van der Laanz ∗Division of Biostatistics, University of California, Berkeley - the SEARCH Consortium, lb- [email protected] yDivision of Biostatistics, University of California, Berkeley - the SEARCH Consortium, may- [email protected] zDivision of Biostatistics, University of California, Berkeley - the SEARCH Consortium, [email protected] This working paper is hosted by The Berkeley Electronic Press (bepress) and may not be commer- cially reproduced without the permission of the copyright holder. http://biostats.bepress.com/ucbbiostat/paper334 Copyright c 2015 by the authors. Targeted Estimation and Inference for the Sample Average Treatment Effect Laura B. Balzer, Maya L. Petersen, and Mark J. van der Laan Abstract While the population average treatment effect has been the subject of extensive methods and applied research, less consideration has been given to the sample average treatment effect: the mean difference in the counterfactual outcomes for the study units. The sample parameter is easily interpretable and is arguably the most relevant when the study units are not representative of a greater population or when the exposure’s impact is heterogeneous. Formally, the sample effect is not identifiable from the observed data distribution. Nonetheless, targeted maxi- mum likelihood estimation (TMLE) can provide an asymptotically unbiased and efficient estimate of both the population and sample parameters.
    [Show full text]
  • Average Treatment Effects in the Presence of Unknown
    Average treatment effects in the presence of unknown interference Fredrik Sävje∗ Peter M. Aronowy Michael G. Hudgensz October 25, 2019 Abstract We investigate large-sample properties of treatment effect estimators under unknown interference in randomized experiments. The inferential target is a generalization of the average treatment effect estimand that marginalizes over potential spillover ef- fects. We show that estimators commonly used to estimate treatment effects under no interference are consistent for the generalized estimand for several common exper- imental designs under limited but otherwise arbitrary and unknown interference. The rates of convergence depend on the rate at which the amount of interference grows and the degree to which it aligns with dependencies in treatment assignment. Importantly for practitioners, the results imply that if one erroneously assumes that units do not interfere in a setting with limited, or even moderate, interference, standard estima- tors are nevertheless likely to be close to an average treatment effect if the sample is sufficiently large. Conventional confidence statements may, however, not be accurate. arXiv:1711.06399v4 [math.ST] 23 Oct 2019 We thank Alexander D’Amour, Matthew Blackwell, David Choi, Forrest Crawford, Peng Ding, Naoki Egami, Avi Feller, Owen Francis, Elizabeth Halloran, Lihua Lei, Cyrus Samii, Jasjeet Sekhon and Daniel Spielman for helpful suggestions and discussions. A previous version of this article was circulated under the title “A folk theorem on interference in experiments.” ∗Departments of Political Science and Statistics & Data Science, Yale University. yDepartments of Political Science, Biostatistics and Statistics & Data Science, Yale University. zDepartment of Biostatistics, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill. 1 1 Introduction Investigators of causality routinely assume that subjects under study do not interfere with each other.
    [Show full text]
  • How to Examine External Validity Within an Experiment∗
    How to Examine External Validity Within an Experiment∗ Amanda E. Kowalski August 7, 2019 Abstract A fundamental concern for researchers who analyze and design experiments is that the experimental estimate might not be externally valid for all policies. Researchers often attempt to assess external validity by comparing data from an experiment to external data. In this paper, I discuss approaches from the treatment effects literature that researchers can use to begin the examination of external validity internally, within the data from a single experiment. I focus on presenting the approaches simply using figures. ∗I thank Pauline Mourot, Ljubica Ristovska, Sukanya Sravasti, Rae Staben, and Matthew Tauzer for excellent research assistance. NSF CAREER Award 1350132 provided support. I thank Magne Mogstad, Jeffrey Smith, Edward Vytlacil, and anonymous referees for helpful feedback. 1 1 Introduction The traditional reason that a researcher runs an experiment is to address selection into treatment. For example, a researcher might be worried that individuals with better outcomes regardless of treatment are more likely to select into treatment, so the simple comparison of treated to untreated individuals will reflect a selection effect as well as a treatment effect. By running an experiment, the reasoning goes, a researcher isolates a single treatment effect by eliminating selection. However, there is still room for selection within experiments. In many experiments, some lottery losers receive treatment and some lottery winners forgo treatment (see Heckman et al. (2000)). Throughout this paper, I consider experiments in which both occur, experiments with \two-sided noncompliance." In these experiments, individuals participate in a lottery. Individuals who win the lottery are in the intervention group.
    [Show full text]
  • Inference on the Average Treatment Effect on the Treated from a Bayesian Perspective
    UNIVERSIDADE FEDERAL DO RIO DE JANEIRO INSTITUTO DE MATEMÁTICA DEPARTAMENTO DE MÉTODOS ESTATÍSTICOS Inference on the Average Treatment Effect on the Treated from a Bayesian Perspective Estelina Serrano de Marins Capistrano Rio de Janeiro – RJ 2019 Inference on the Average Treatment Effect on the Treated from a Bayesian Perspective Estelina Serrano de Marins Capistrano Doctoral thesis submitted to the Graduate Program in Statistics of Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro as part of the requirements for the degree of Doctor in Statistics. Advisors: Prof. Alexandra M. Schmidt, Ph.D. Prof. Erica E. M. Moodie, Ph.D. Rio de Janeiro, August 27th of 2019. Inference on the Average Treatment Effect on the Treated from a Bayesian Perspective Estelina Serrano de Marins Capistrano Doctoral thesis submitted to the Graduate Program in Statistics of Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro as part of the requirements for the degree of Doctor in Statistics. Approved by: Prof. Alexandra M. Schmidt, Ph.D. IM - UFRJ (Brazil) / McGill University (Canada) - Advisor Prof. Erica E. M. Moodie, Ph.D. McGill University (Canada) - Advisor Prof. Hedibert F. Lopes, Ph.D. INSPER - SP (Brazil) Prof. Helio dos S. Migon, Ph.D. IM - UFRJ (Brazil) Prof. Mariane B. Alves, Ph.D. IM - UFRJ (Brazil) Rio de Janeiro, August 27th of 2019. CIP - Catalogação na Publicação Capistrano, Estelina Serrano de Marins C243i Inference on the Average Treatment Effect on the Treated from a Bayesian Perspective / Estelina Serrano de Marins Capistrano. -- Rio de Janeiro, 2019. 106 f. Orientador: Alexandra Schmidt. Coorientador: Erica Moodie. Tese (doutorado) - Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Instituto de Matemática, Programa de Pós Graduação em Estatística, 2019.
    [Show full text]
  • Propensity Scores for the Estimation of Average Treatment Effects In
    Propensity scores for the estimation of average treatment effects in observational studies Leonardo Grilli and Carla Rampichini Dipartimento di Statistica "Giuseppe Parenti" Universit di Firenze Training Sessions on Causal Inference Bristol - June 28-29, 2011 Grilli and Rampichini (UNIFI) Propensity scores BRISTOL JUNE 2011 1 / 77 Outline 1 Observational studies and Propensity score 2 Motivating example: effect of participation in a job training program on individuals earnings 3 Regression-based estimation under unconfoundedness 4 Matching 5 Propensity Scores Propensity score matching Propensity Score estimation 6 Matching strategy and ATT estimation Propensity-score matching with STATA Nearest Neighbor Matching Example: PS matching Example: balance checking Caliper and radius matching Overlap checking pscore matching vs regression Grilli and Rampichini (UNIFI) Propensity scores BRISTOL JUNE 2011 2 / 77 Introduction In the evaluation problems, data often do not come from randomized trials but from (non-randomized) observational studies. Rosenbaum and Rubin (1983) proposed propensity score matching as a method to reduce the bias in the estimation of treatment effects with observational data sets. These methods have become increasingly popular in medical trials and in the evaluation of economic policy interventions. Grilli and Rampichini (UNIFI) Propensity scores BRISTOL JUNE 2011 3 / 77 Steps for designing observational studies As suggested by Rubin (2008), we have to design observational studies to approximate randomized trial in order to
    [Show full text]
  • ESTIMATING AVERAGE TREATMENT EFFECTS: INTRODUCTION Jeff Wooldridge Michigan State University BGSE/IZA Course in Microeconometrics July 2009
    ESTIMATING AVERAGE TREATMENT EFFECTS: INTRODUCTION Jeff Wooldridge Michigan State University BGSE/IZA Course in Microeconometrics July 2009 1. Introduction 2. Basic Concepts 1 1. Introduction ∙ What kinds of questions can we answer using a “modern” approach to treatment effect estimation? Here are some examples: What are the effects of a job training program on employment or labor earnings? What are the effects of a school voucher program on student performance? Does a certain medical intervention increase the likelihood of survival? ∙ The main issue in program evaluation concerns the nature of the intervention, or “treatment.” 2 ∙ For example, is the “treatment” randomly assigned? Hardly ever in economics, and problematical even in clinical trials because those chosen to be eligible can and do opt out. But there is a push in some fields, for example, development economics, to use more experiments. ∙ With retrospective or observational data, a reasonable possibility is to assume that treatment is effectively randomly assigned conditional on observable covariates. (“Unconfoundedness” or “ignorability” of treatment or “selection on observables.” Sometimes called “exogenous treatment.”) 3 ∙ Or, does assignment depend fundamentally on unobservables, where the dependence cannot be broken by controlling for observables? (“Confounded” assignment or “selection on unobservables” or “endogenous treatment”) ∙ Often there is a component of self-selection in program evaluation. 4 ∙ Broadly speaking, approaches to treament effect estimation fall into one of three situations: (1) Assume unconfoundedness of treatment, and then worry about how to exploit it in estimation; (2) Allow self-selection on unobservables but exploit an exogenous instrumental variable; (3) Exploit a “regression discontinuity” design, where the treatment is determined (or its probability) as a discontinuous function of observed variable.
    [Show full text]
  • Estimating Individual Treatment Effects with Time-Varying Confounders
    Estimating Individual Treatment Effects with Time-Varying Confounders Ruoqi Liu∗, Changchang Yin∗, Ping Zhang∗ ∗The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, USA 43210 Email: fliu.7324, yin.731, [email protected] Abstract—Estimating the individual treatment effect (ITE) from observational data is meaningful and practical in health- Zt-1 Zt ... ZT-1 care. Existing work mainly relies on the strong ignorability assumption that no hidden confounders exist, which may lead At At+1 AT YT+t to bias in estimating causal effects. Some studies considering the ... hidden confounders are designed for static environment and not easily adaptable to a dynamic setting. In fact, most observational Xt Xt+1 ... XT data (e.g., electronic medical records) is naturally dynamic and consists of sequential information. In this paper, we propose Deep Sequential Weighting (DSW) for estimating ITE with time-varying t t+1 T Time confounders. Specifically, DSW infers the hidden confounders by incorporating the current treatment assignments and historical Fig. 1. The illustration of causal graphs for causal estimation from dy- information using a deep recurrent weighting neural network. namic observational data. During observed window T , we have a trajectory T The learned representations of hidden confounders combined fXt;At;Zt−1gt=1 [ fYT +τ g, where Xt denotes the current observed with current observed data are leveraged for potential outcome covariates, At denotes the treatment assignments, Zt denotes the hidden and treatment predictions. We compute the time-varying in- confounders and YT +τ denotes the potential outcomes. At any time stamp t, the treatment assignments At are affected by both observed covariates verse probabilities of treatment for re-weighting the population.
    [Show full text]