Recharging Reading Success Getting Struggling Readers Back on Track

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Recharging Reading Success Getting Struggling Readers Back on Track —Graeme Sloan/Education Week Braydan Finnerty, 2nd grade, chooses letter magnets off the board while doing a spelling exercise in front of the rest of the class at Beverly Gardens Elementary in Dayton, Ohio. Recharging Reading Success Getting Struggling Readers Back on Track EDITORS NOTE How Do Kids Learn to Read? What OPINION How can educators optimize literacy the Science Says ..........................................2 education among students who struggle to Connecting Reading & Writing ‘Is a read? In this Spotlight, learn how teachers These Schools Filled Vending High-Leverage Move’ .............................12 are implementing scientific research on Machines With Books. Will It Motivate Reading? .....................................6 Why I Created Book Groups for My reading into curriculum, how schools are Students ........................................................15 helping students foster a love of reading, A Look Inside One Classroom’s and how teachers are using writing to Reading Overhaul ......................................8 How to Make Reading Instruction compliment reading instruction. Much, Much More Efficient ................16 ‘Decodable’ Books: Boring, Useful, or Both?.............................................................. 10 Recharging Reading Success Published on October 2, 2020, in Education Week’s Special Report: Getting Reading Right How Do Kids Learn to Read? What the Science Says By Sarah Schwartz and Sarah D. Sparks ow do children learn to read? For almost a century, re- searchers have argued over the question. Most of the dis- agreement has centered on Hthe very beginning stages of the reading process, when young children are first starting to figure out how to decipher words on a page. One theory is that reading is a natural process, like learning to speak. If teachers and parents sur- round children with good books, this theory goes, kids will pick up reading on their own. Another idea suggests that reading is a series of strategic guesses based on context, and that kids should be taught these guessing strategies. But research has shown that reading is not a natural process, and it’s not a guessing game. Written language is a code. Certain combina- tions of letters predictably represent certain —Video still from “ What the Science Says About How Kids Learn to Read”/Education Week sounds. And for the last few decades, the research has been clear: Teaching young kids how to crack sent spoken sounds, recognize patterns of let- Don’t children learn to read the the code—teaching systematic phonics—is the ter sounds as words, and match those to spoken way they learn to speak? most reliable way to make sure that they learn words whose meanings they know. This differs how to read words. Infants learn to speak by listening to and from Chinese, for example. It uses a tonal spo- Of course, there is more to reading than seeing repeating sounds made by adults and connect- ken language, conveying meaning with small a word on a page and pronouncing it out loud. As ing them to meanings. They don’t consciously differences in stress or pitch. Its writing system such, there is more to teaching reading than just distinguish individual sound units (called pho- is partially logographic—in which written sym- teaching phonics. Reading requires children to nemes) when hearing spoken language. Some bols correspond directly to a word or concept— make meaning out of print. They need to know the research suggests infants learn probabilisti- and also includes words that couple symbols for different sounds in spoken language and be able to cally—for example, hearing the sound “ball” at meaning and symbols for sound. Someone read- connect those sounds to written letters in order to the same time as the sight of a round, bouncy ing Chinese hanzi characters could not “sound decipher words. They need deep background and object over time makes the child associate the out” unfamiliar words character by character. vocabulary knowledge so that they understand the two—while other studies suggest children map words they read. Eventually, they need to be able meaning to a word after experiencing it just once What is systematic, explicit phonics to recognize most words automatically and read or twice. Within the first two years, typically instruction, and why is it important? connected text fluently, attending to grammar, developing toddlers’ brains focus on the most punctuation, and sentence structure. common sounds in their native languages and Connecting printed letters on a page to writ- But knowing how to decode is an essential step connect those sounds to meaning. A child devel- ten sounds isn’t intuitive. While some young chil- in becoming a reader. If children can’t decipher the ops understanding of speech through exposure dren may make those connections themselves, precise words on the page, they’ll never become to language and opportunities to practice the most do not. One set of studies from 1989-90 il- fluent readers or understand the passages they’re “serve and return” patterns of conversation, even lustrates this phenomenon well. reading. without explicit instruction. In these studies, conducted by Brian Byrne That’s why we’ve put together this overview By contrast, children do not naturally develop and Ruth Fielding-Barnsley, researchers taught of the research on early reading, in grades K-2. It reading skill through exposure to text. The way young children between ages 3 and 5 to read whole covers what’s known about how we should teach they learn to connect oral and written language words aloud, like “fat” and “bat.” These children letter-sound patterns, and what we don’t know depends on what kind of language they are learn- didn’t already know their letter names. for sure yet. It touches on what else should be part ing to read. Then, the researchers tested whether the chil- of early reading programs. And it explains why Alphabetic languages, like English or French, dren could transfer their knowledge to reading a we know that most children can’t learn to read use letters to stand for sounds that make up new word. They gave them the word “fun,” and through osmosis or guessing. spoken words. To read an alphabetic language, asked whether the word was “fun” or “bun.” Very Here’s what the evidence shows. children must learn how written letters repre- few of the students could do this successfully. 2 Recharging Reading Success They couldn’t break down the original word into accuracy for young students. phonemes and then transfer their knowledge of A systematic phonics program teaches an those phonemes to a new word. ordered progression of letter-sound correspon- But children could succeed on this task if dences. Teachers don’t only address the letter- they were first given some explicit instructions. sound connections that students stumble over. When children were taught how to recognize Instead, they address all of the combinations me- that certain letters represented certain sounds, thodically, in a sequence, moving on to the next and taught how to segment words to identify once students demonstrate mastery. Teachers those individual letters and sounds, they had explicitly tell students what sounds correspond to much greater success on the original transfer what letter patterns, rather than asking students test. Neuroscience research has since confirmed to figure it out on their own or make guesses. and helped explain these findings. When learning In one series of experiments, Stanford Uni- how to read new words in an unfamiliar made-up versity neuroscientist Bruce McCandliss and language, participants had more long-term suc- his colleagues made up a new written language cess if they were first taught which symbols cor- and taught three-letter words to students either respond to which sounds, than if they tried to re- Lack of exposure and by asking them to focus on letter sounds or on member words as wholes. Brain imaging of these whole words. Later, the students took a reading readers finds that the two teaching strategies tap practice on the part of test of both the words they were taught and new into different neural pathways in the brain. Read- the less-skilled reader words in the made-up language, while an elec- ers taught to connect print to meaning directly troencephalograph monitored their brain activ- could recall words initially more quickly, but delays the development of ity. Those who had focused on letter sounds had less accurately; readers taught to connect print more neural activity on the left side of the brain, to sound and then to meaning read aloud more automaticity and speed at which includes visual and language regions and is quickly and correctly, better recalled the correct the word recognition level. associated with more skilled reading. Those who meanings of words, and transferred their knowl- had been taught to focus on whole words had edge to new words. Slow, capacity-draining more activity on the right side of the brain, which Decades of research has shown that explicit has been characteristically associated with adults phonics instruction benefits early readers, but word-recognition processes and children who struggle with reading. More- particularly those who struggle to read. require cognitive resources over, those who had learned letter sounds were That’s because small strengths or deficits at better able to identify unfamiliar words. the start of reading compound over time. It’s what that should be allocated Early readers benefit from systematic phonics reading expert Keith Stanovich in 1986 dubbed instruction. Among students in grades K-1, pho- the “Matthew Effect in Reading,” after the Bible to comprehension. Thus, nics instruction led to improvements in decod- verse in which the rich get richer and the poor get reading for meaning is ing ability and reading comprehension across the poorer: “The combination of deficient decoding board, according to the National Reading Panel.
Recommended publications
  • Some Issues in Phonics Instruction. Hempenstall, K
    Some Issues in Phonics Instruction. Hempenstall, K. (No date). Some issues in phonics instruction. Education News 26/2/2001. [On-line]. Available: http://www.educationnews.org/some_issues_in_phonics_instructi.htm There are essentially two approaches to teaching phonics that influence what is taught: implicit and explicit phonics instruction. What is the difference? In an explicit (synthetic) program, students will learn the associations between the letters and their sounds. This may comprise showing students the graphemes and teaching them the sounds that correspond to them, as in “This letter you are looking at makes the sound sssss”. Alternatively, some teachers prefer teaching students single sounds first, and then later introducing the visual cue (the grapheme) for the sound, as in “You know the mmmm sound we’ve been practising, well here’s the letter used in writing that tells us to make that sound”. In an explicit program, the processes of blending (What word do these sounds make when we put them together mmm-aaa-nnn?”), and segmenting (“Sound out this word for me”) are also taught. It is of little value knowing what are the building blocks of our language’s structure if one does not know how to put those blocks together appropriately to allow written communication, or to separate them to enable decoding of a letter grouping. After letter-sound correspondence has been taught, phonograms (such as: er, ir, ur, wor, ear, sh, ee, th) are introduced, and more complex words can be introduced into reading activities. In conjunction with this approach "controlled vocabulary" stories may be used - books using only words decodable using the students' current knowledge base.
    [Show full text]
  • Students Who Are Highly Mobile and Reading Instruction
    Reading on the Go! Volume 1: Students Who Are Highly Mobile and Reading Instruction Prepared for the National Center for Homeless Education by Patricia A. Popp, Ph.D. The College of William and Mary December 2004 NCHE Profile The National Center for Homeless Education (NCHE) is a national resource center of research and information enabling communities to successfully address the needs of children and their families who are experiencing homelessness and unaccompanied youth in homeless situations. Funded by the U.S. Department of Education, NCHE provides services to improve educational opportunities and outcomes for homeless children and youth in our nation’s school communities. NCHE is housed at SERVE, a consortium of education organizations associated with the School of Education at the University of North Carolina at Greensboro. The goals of NCHE are the following: • Disseminate important resource and referral information related to the complex issues surrounding the education of children and youth experiencing homelessness • Provide rapid-response referral information • Foster collaboration among various organizations with interests in addressing the needs of children and youth experiencing homelessness • Synthesize and apply existing research and guide the research agenda to expand the knowledge base on the education of homeless children and families, and unaccompanied youth Website: www.serve.org/nche HelpLine: 800-308-2145 Contact: Diana Bowman, Director NCHE at SERVE P.O. Box 5367 Greensboro, NC 27435 Phone: 336-315-7453 or 800-755-3277 Email: [email protected] or [email protected] The content of this publication does not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the U.S. Department of Education, nor does mention of trade names, commercial products, or organizations imply endorsement by the U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Independent Reading Time
    Grades K–8 Independent Independent Reading Time Providing a quiet, relaxed independent reading time several times Reading Time a week for children to read books at their “just-right” reading level is critical to developing successful readers. Handbook The Independent Reading Time Handbook that accompanies Developmental Studies Center’s AfterSchool KidzLibrary offers strategies and tools for implementing an effective independent reading program at your after-school site. This handbook outlines ways to set up your after-school library and how to introduce and run a successful Independent Reading Time (IRT) program. It also specifies the leader’s role, including ways after-school staff can help children select just-right books. ISBN 978-1-57621-905-8 y(7IB5H6*MLTKPS( +;!z!”!z!” Illustration by GarryWilliams Illustration by ASL-IRTHBK Project Name: AS IRT Handbook Cover Round: Final pages Date: 05/11/10 File Name: ASL_IRTHBK_cover Page #: 1 Trim size: 8.375” x 10.875” Colors used: CMYK Printed at: 80% Artist: Garry Williams Editor: Erica Hruby Comments: Independent Reading Time Handbook Project Name: AS IRT Handbook Round: Final pages Date: 05/18/10 File Name: ASL-IRTHBK_interior Page #: i Trim size: 8.375” x 10.875” Colors used: PMS 2685 Printed at: 80% Artist: Joslyn Hidalgo Editor: Erica Hruby Comments: Copyright © 2010 by Developmental Studies Center All rights reserved. Except where otherwise noted, no part of this publication may be reproduced in whole or in part, or stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the written permission of the publisher.
    [Show full text]
  • Independent Reading: What the Research Tells Us
    1James Patterson, Award-winning Novelist and Founder of Read Kiddo Read, The Joy and Power of Reading – A Summary of Research and Expert Opinion Independent Reading: what the research tells us The impact of independent reading for 20 minutes per day Independent Words per year Reading rate % reading time (in millions) 21 minutes / day 1.80 90th 9.6 minutes / day .62 70th 4.6 minutes / day .28 50th Anderson, R. C., P. Wilson, and L. Fielding. Growth in reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly 23: 285–303 www.scholastic.com/worldofpossible Independent Reading: what the research tells us Reading builds a cognitive processing infrastructure that then “massively influences” every aspect of our thinking. (Stanovich, 2003) Children between the ages of 10 and 16 who read for pleasure make more progress not only in vocabulary and spelling but also in mathematics than those who rarely read (Sullivan & Brown, 2013) Omnivorous reading in childhood and adolescence correlates positively with ultimate adult success (Simonton, 1988) Avid teen readers engage in deep intellectual work and psychological exploration through the books they choose to read themselves (Wilhelm & Smith, 2013) www.scholastic.com/worldofpossible Independent Reading: what the research tells us Children Who Read… Succeed! www.scholastic.com/worldofpossible Reader Profiling Early Readers “Children exposed to lots of books during their early childhood will have an easier time learning to read than those who are not.” Dr. Henry Bernstein, Harvard Medical School There’s real opportunity in providing parents with The brain develops faster It’s therefore crucial to books and encouragement than any other time foster literacy during the to read to their children between the ages of zero early stages of life.
    [Show full text]
  • Put Reading First 2006
    Put Reading First Kindergarten Through Grade 3 The Research Building Blocks For Teaching Children to Read ThirdThird Edition The Research Building Blocks for Teaching Children to Read Put Reading First Kindergarten Through Grade 3 Writers: Bonnie B. Armbruster, Ph.D., University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Fran Lehr, M.A., Lehr & Associates, Champaign, Illinois, Jean Osborn, M.Ed., University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Editor: C. Ralph Adler, RMC Research Corporation Designer: Lisa T. Noonis, RMC Research Corporation Contents i Introduction 1 Phonemic Awareness Instruction 11 Phonics Instruction 19 Fluency Instruction 29 Vocabulary Instruction 41 Text Comprehension Instruction This publication was developed by the Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement (CIERA) and was funded by the National Institute for Literacy (NIFL) through the Educational Research and Development Centers Program, PR/Award Number R305R70004, as administered by the Office of Educational Research and Improvement (OERI), U.S. Department of Education. However, the comments or conclusions do not necessarily represent the positions or policies of NIFL, OERI, or the U.S. Department of Education, and you should not assume endorsement by the Federal Government. The National Institute for Literacy The National Institute for Literacy, an agency in the Federal government, is authorized to help strengthen literacy across the lifespan. The Institute works to provide national leadership on literacy issues, including the improvement of reading instruction for children, youth, and adults by sharing information on scientifically based research. Sandra Baxter, Director Lynn Reddy, Deputy Director The Partnership for Reading This document was published by The Partnership for Reading, a collaborative effort of the National Institute for Literacy, the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and the U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Report of the National Reading Panel Hearing
    S. HRG. 106–897 REPORT OF THE NATIONAL READING PANEL HEARING BEFORE A SUBCOMMITTEE OF THE COMMITTEE ON APPROPRIATIONS UNITED STATES SENATE ONE HUNDRED SIXTH CONGRESS SECOND SESSION SPECIAL HEARING APRIL 13, 2000-WASHINGTON, DC Printed for the use of the Committee on Appropriations ( Available via the World Wide Web: http://www.access.gpo.gov/congress/senate U.S. GOVERNMENT PRINTING OFFICE 66–481 cc WASHINGTON : 2001 For sale by the U.S. Government Printing Office Superintendent of Documents, Congressional Sales Office, Washington, DC 20402 COMMITTEE ON APPROPRIATIONS TED STEVENS, Alaska, Chairman THAD COCHRAN, Mississippi ROBERT C. BYRD, West Virginia ARLEN SPECTER, Pennsylvania DANIEL K. INOUYE, Hawaii PETE V. DOMENICI, New Mexico ERNEST F. HOLLINGS, South Carolina CHRISTOPHER S. BOND, Missouri PATRICK J. LEAHY, Vermont SLADE GORTON, Washington FRANK R. LAUTENBERG, New Jersey MITCH MCCONNELL, Kentucky TOM HARKIN, Iowa CONRAD BURNS, Montana BARBARA A. MIKULSKI, Maryland RICHARD C. SHELBY, Alabama HARRY REID, Nevada JUDD GREGG, New Hampshire HERB KOHL, Wisconsin ROBERT F. BENNETT, Utah PATTY MURRAY, Washington BEN NIGHTHORSE CAMPBELL, Colorado BYRON L. DORGAN, North Dakota LARRY CRAIG, Idaho DIANNE FEINSTEIN, California KAY BAILEY HUTCHISON, Texas RICHARD J. DURBIN, Illinois JON KYL, Arizona STEVEN J. CORTESE, Staff Director LISA SUTHERLAND, Deputy Staff Director JAMES H. ENGLISH, Minority Staff Director SUBCOMMITTEE ON DEPARTMENTS OF LABOR, HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES, AND EDUCATION, AND RELATED AGENCIES ARLEN SPECTER, Pennsylvania, Chairman THAD COCHRAN, Mississippi TOM HARKIN, Iowa SLADE GORTON, Washington ERNEST F. HOLLINGS, South Carolina JUDD GREGG, New Hampshire DANIEL K. INOUYE, Hawaii LARRY CRAIG, Idaho HARRY REID, Nevada KAY BAILEY HUTCHISON, Texas HERB KOHL, Wisconsin TED STEVENS, Alaska PATTY MURRAY, Washington JON KYL, Arizona DIANNE FEINSTEIN, California ROBERT C.
    [Show full text]
  • Guided Independent Reading
    TECHNICAL PAPER | NOVEMBER 2012 Guided Independent Reading Accelerated Reader, Accelerated Reader Best Practices, Advanced Technology for Data-Driven Schools, AR, AR Best Practices, ATOS, Renaissance, Renaissance Learning, the Renaissance Learning logo, and STAR Reading are trademarks of Renaissance Learning, Inc., and its subsidiaries, registered, common law, or pending registration in the United States and other countries. © 2012 by Renaissance Learning, Inc. All rights reserved. Printed in the United States of America. This publication is protected by U.S. and international copyright laws. It is unlawful to duplicate or reproduce any copyrighted material without authorization from the copyright holder. For more information, contact: RENAISSANCE LEARNING P.O. Box 8036 Wisconsin Rapids, WI 54495-8036 (800) 338-4204 www.renlearn.com [email protected] 11/12 Contents Introduction ......................................................................................................................................................... 1 Accelerated Reader Best Practices: Factors of Interest ..................................................................................... 3 Research Summary ............................................................................................................................................. 6 Research Implications and Accelerated Reader Best Practices ...................................................................... 12 Conclusion ........................................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • The Readability of Two Grade 4 Natural Sciences Textbooks for South African Schools
    Lucy Sibanda The readability of two Grade 4 natural sciences textbooks for South African schools Abstract This paper, deriving from a larger study, evaluates the readability of two Grade 4 natural sciences textbooks used by learners who speak English as an additional language in two South African schools. The study is set within the context of the reading-related transitional challenges faced by English second language learners when they move from the foundation phase to the intermediate phase. Text readability is critical for educational achievement during this transition. The case study was conducted by means of a qualitative content analysis of factors that are not accommodated in the readability formulae which were used to investigate the textbooks. While the findings from the two instruments were ambivalent for Book 1, with the content analysis showing the book to be largely readable, but the readability calculator indicating it to be beyond the learners’ reading level, both instruments indicated poor readability for Book 2. The study recommends a close consideration of text readability by both authors and teachers. Keywords: readability, readability formulae, reading levels, content analysis, graphics Lucy Sibanda, Rhodes University. Email: [email protected]. South African Journal of Childhood Education | 2014 4(2): 154-175 | ISSN: 2223-7674 |© UJ Sibanda – The readability of two Grade 4 natural sciences textbooks Introduction An understanding of mathematics, science and technology is a prerequisite to meaningful participation in
    [Show full text]
  • NCTE Statement on Independent Reading Revised October 2019
    NCTE Statement on Independent Reading Revised October 2019 OVERVIEW The NCTE Position Statement on Independent Reading is a revision of the previous statement on leisure reading. We believe leisure implies that this reading is solely for entertainment and does not impact long-term reading success. Research supports that independent reading has the most significant impact on student success in reading, but unfortunately it is a practice that is often replaced with other programs and interventions (Lewis & Samuels, 2002). This revision provides a definition of independent reading, explains the benefits of independent reading and its necessity as a protected time in the classroom, shares core values surrounding the instructional practice of independent reading, as well as guidelines and suggested best practices for educational stakeholders (teachers, administrators, and community partners). A list of resources and further research is also provided. DEFINITION OF INDEPENDENT READING Independent reading is a routine, protected instructional practice that occurs across all grade levels. Effective independent reading practices include time for students to read, access to books that represent a wide range of characters and experiences, and support within a reading community that includes teachers and students. Student choice in text is essential because it motivates, engages, and reaches a wide variety of readers. The goal of independent reading as an instructional practice is to build habitual readers with conscious reading identities. STATEMENT Independent reading leads to an increased volume of reading. The more one reads, the better one reads. The more one reads, the more knowledge of words and language one acquires. The more one reads, the more fluent one becomes as a reader.
    [Show full text]
  • The Power of Independent, Self-Selected Reading in The
    2010 middle level essentials The Power of Independent, Self-Selected and ninth grade academy conference Reading in the Middle Grades Andrea J. Stairs & Sara Stairs Burgos If we want our students to grow to appreciate literature, we mutually exclusive and lack correlation (e.g., Tatum, Middle Level Essentials Conference need to give them a say in decisions about the literature they 2006)1. Our main argument in this paper is that, despite An intensive 2-day conference that provides an in- will read. (Atwell, 1998, p. 36) the aforementioned challenges, literacy educators should Learning in depth exploration of Differentiated Instruction, Math, keep independent, self-selected reading at the center of Now, more than ever, teachers are challenged to Science, Literacy, Technology, & Assessment. the middle grades language arts curriculum. We believe negotiate multiple and contradictory demands on their Las Vegas! that a literacy-rich classroom environment grounded time. Across the nation, schools have been required in student-centered pedagogy offers possibilities Ninth Grade Academy Conference to raise their standards for student achievement and April 22–23, 2010 for engaging all learners and encouraging them to make “adequate yearly progress.” This is just a sample An intensive 2-day academy for high school be lifelong readers. After outlining a rationale for Las Vegas, NV of the abundant evidence that the complex work of educators who desire to help ninth grade students independent reading in a reading workshop classroom teaching described two decades ago by Apple (1986) JW Marriott Spa & Resort flourish. Experts from across the country will be environment, we describe how these practices were has intensified.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Orton Gillingham Instructional Techniques During Small Group Instruction
    Rowan University Rowan Digital Works Theses and Dissertations 10-6-2016 The effects of Orton Gillingham instructional techniques during small group instruction Kathleen Tiver Rowan University Follow this and additional works at: https://rdw.rowan.edu/etd Part of the Curriculum and Instruction Commons Recommended Citation Tiver, Kathleen, "The effects of Orton Gillingham instructional techniques during small group instruction" (2016). Theses and Dissertations. 2334. https://rdw.rowan.edu/etd/2334 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Rowan Digital Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Rowan Digital Works. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE EFFECTS OF ORTON GILLINGHAM SUPPLEMENTAL INSTRUCTIONAL TECHNIQUES DURING SMALL GROUP INSTRUCTION IN A FIRST GRADE INCLUSION CLASSROOM by Kathleen G Tiver A Thesis Submitted to the Department of Interdisciplinary and Inclusive Education College of Education In partial fulfillment of the requirement For the degree of Master of Arts in Special Education at Rowan University May 1, 2016 Thesis Chair: S. Jay Kuder, Ed.D. © 2016 Kathleen Tiver Dedications This thesis is dedicated to my daughters Addyson and Alexa, for their understanding while I completed countless hours on research, writing, and completing this study. To Andrew, for your unwavering support and unconditional love during the school year. To my family, for helping with our girls when our schedules needed an extra set of hands. Acknowledgements Much thank you to my Professor, Dr. Jay Kuder for his countless hours of editing, revising, and guidance during this school year. To my colleagues, for working with me during this year and giving me the opportunity to prove a way that will build phonemic awareness for our students.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Readability Levels to Guide Students to Books
    SPECIAL REPORT | JANUARY 2014 Using Readability Levels to Guide Students to Books Readability: What it is and why it is useful For more than 50 years, readability formulas have helped teachers, librarians, and parents match books to students. Readability formulas use objective measurements to analyze text and predict which materials can be comprehended by individual readers. This concept is particularly important in Renaissance Accelerated Reader®, Renaissance’s personalized practice and progress monitoring software that makes the essential student practice component of any reading curriculum more effective. When Accelerated Reader is used according to recommended Accelerated Reader Best Practices for classroom implementation, it has been shown to accelerate reading growth for all students, regardless of ability.1 Research-based Accelerated Reader Best Practices emphasize that students show the most reading improvement if they regularly practice reading within a range of difficulty that is neither too challenging nor too easy. Accelerated Reader uses the ATOS Readability Formula to help teachers guide student reading practice at this optimal level (for more information, see ATOS box, p. 4). Judging the suitability of books for individual students Advances in technology and statistical techniques have led to improvements in the science of readability, but there are still some things that readability formulas cannot do—and will never be able to do. All readability formulas produce an estimate of a book’s difficulty based on selected variables in the text, but none analyze the suitability of a book’s content or literary merit for individual readers. That decision is up to educators and parents, who best know what content is appropriate for each student.
    [Show full text]