Current Zoology 59 (4): 569–578, 2013

The evolution of sex

Ally R. HARARI*, Hadass STEINITZ Department of Entomology, The Volcani Center, Bet Dagan, Israel

Abstract The role of female sex pheromones in natural selection, particularly as a means for species recognition to avoid the generation of hybrid offspring with low fitness, has been widely explored and is generally accepted by scholars. However, the significance of sex pheromones in shaping () and in competition over breeding resources (social se- lection) has been largely ignored. The effect of sexual selection on sex pheromones as a sexually dimorphic signaling trait has been discounted because the amount of released by is typically minute, while the role of sex pheromones in competition over breeding resources (other than mates) has not yet been considered. As a result of natural selection, variation in sex pheromones among females is expected to be low, and males are not expected to choose their mates among phero- mone-releasing conspecific females. Sexual selection, on the other hand, should drive the increase in pheromone variance among females, and males are expected to choose females based on this variation. Moreover, social selection resulting from more general social interactions, for example competition among females for breeding sites and food, should also promote variance among fe- male sex pheromones. Here, we review the current evidence for each of the three selection processes acting on sex pheromones of female as an advertising trait. We suggest that the three selection types are not mutually exclusive but rather act together to promote different fitness components in diverse ecological situations [Current Zoology 59 (4): 569–578, 2013]. Keywords Sex pheromone, Natural selection, Sexual selection, Social selection, Competition, Mate choice

Pheromones are chemical signals produced by an or- when are used to attract mates, provide information ganism that can elicit, even in small quantities, a be- about the species and the gender of the signaler havioral or physiological response in another individual (Svensson, 1996; Johansson and Jones, 2007). However, of the same species (Wyatt, 2003). Pheromones may be the following questions about the nature of pheromones used for a variety of purposes. For example, phero- are still somewhat ambiguous. Are sex pheromones mones may be used to alert conspecifics of danger, as shaped by natural selection to prevent interspecific information for orientation and foraging (Holldobler et and the resulting production of hybrid offspring al., 2001), to aggregate in attempt of resource exploita- (Paterson, 1985; Löfstedt et al., 1991; Baker, 2002; Mas tion (Borden, 1989) and as a social or recognition cue and Jallon, 2005)? Did pheromones evolve via sexual for kin or familiar individuals (Penn and Potts, 1999; selection aimed at facilitating the competition for mates Brennan and Kendrick, 2006). Pheromones are also (Martin and Lopez, 2006; Harari et al., 2011)? Have sex used to attract or repel mates (for review, see Johansson pheromones evolved through social selection in the in- and Jones, 2007), to mark territories (for review, see traspecific competition over resources other than mates, Mason and Parker, 2010), and to advertise the status or such as direct competition for food and nest sites condition of the signaler (Olsson et al., 2003; Martin (Goekce et al., 2007; Harari et al., 2011; Tobias et al., and Lopez, 2006; Harari et al., 2011). In general, 2012)? Is it possible that sex pheromones are affected pheromones are communication tools used by many by all three kinds of selection (natural, sexual, and so- taxa to convey information among individuals of the cial), depending on the species and the environment? same species (Karlson and Luscher, 1959; Wyatt, 2003). The debate on the distinctions between the different Insects, in particular, have mastered the olfaction chan- selection mechanisms began with publication of The nel as a means for communication using volatile, task- Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex by specific substances. The properties of the pheromone Darwin (1871) and Darwinism by Wallace (1890). Ever allow for diverse information to be bounded to the re- since, the debate has not lost its passion (Endler, 1986; ceiver detecting and processing capabilities. Grafen, 1987; Andersson, 1994; Clutton-Brock, 2004; Researchers generally agree that sex pheromones, Lessells et al., 2006; Roughgarden et al., 2006; Carranza,

Received Apr. 8, 2013; accepted July 8, 2013. ∗ Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] © 2013 Current Zoology 570 Current Zoology Vol. 59 No. 4

2009; Clutton-Brock, 2009; Shuker, 2010). Darwin 1871; Andersson, 1994; Shuker, 2010). Competition for himself addressed these difficulties by distinguishing ecological resources other than mates is attributed to between the effects of sexual selection from natural se- social selection (Tobias et al., 2012), and obtaining a lection (Darwin, 1871, page 257; and see Box 1). mate of the right species (Dobzhansky, 1937), is attribu- The evidence that females also have a say in adver- ted to natural selection. These three types of selection tising traits and in competition for mates has further are not mutually exclusive. On the contrary, they may complicated the view of the classic Darwinian mecha- all contribute to the shaping of female pheromones in nisms operating on sexual selection, namely male-male relation to the various ecological contexts. In all selec- competition and female choice (Darwin, 1871). As tion types, the fitness coin by which the success of the pointed out by Clutton-Brock (2009), females may be individuals is determined is the number or quality of subject to selection similar to that operating on males offspring they leave (Box 1, Table 1). under various ecological conditions, such as a female- 1 Female Sex Pheromones and biased operational sex ratio that generally promotes reversing sex roles either fully or partly (Vincent et al., Natural Selection 1992; Vepsalainen and Savolainen, 1995; Butchart, Pheromones may have evolved by natural selection 2000). when no benefit is expected by choosing a particular Besides its central role in reviving the field of sexual conspecific mate (i.e., absence of mate choice). selection, the natural/sexual/social selection debate is Dobzhansky (1937) postulated that natural selection acts important for understanding how repeated patterns in against hybrids through adaptive behavioral changes evolution arose in light of the variable selection pres- and sexual traits. As such, traits that avoid interspecific sures. The evolution of female ornaments is one bio- mismatched are shaped by natural selection logical pattern that is currently at the crux of the discus- because males and females of the same species have a sion, although similar traits in males are generally ac- mutual interest in nullifying the risk of producing no or cepted and are attributed to sexual selection (for review, infertile hybrid offspring (Paterson, 1985; Löfstedt et al., see Clutton-Brock, 2007, Clutton-Brock, 2009). 1991; Baker, 2002; Mas and Jallon, 2005). Accordingly, Here, we discuss the possible selection types operat- sex pheromones are expected to be under strong stabi- ing on female pheromones as a sexually dimorphic lizing selection to prevent changes in the exact signal secondary trait. In doing so, we adopt the suggestions of because slight deviations from the species code may Shuker (2010) and Tobias et al. (2012) to categorize the lead to interspecific mating. operating selection pressure based on the type of re- Evidence for natural selection operating on species- sources the organisms are competing for. Competition specific sex pheromones is scarce but does exist. As a over mates is attributed to sexual selection (Darwin, product of natural selection, differences in phero-

Box 1 Definitions

Darwin (1871) distinguished between two selection types that promote the evolution of phenotypic traits: (1) natural selection, which acts on characters that involve the "struggle for existence", and (2) sexual selection, which acts on characters in relation to competition for mates. West-Eberhard (1983) introduced a third type of selection (3), social selection, which acts on traits that promote the competition among con- specifics over resources that are not directly associated with mates. According to these definitions, sexual selection acts in the context of mate choice and competition over mates, social selection contributes to the evolution of traits in the context of competition for resources other than mates, and natural selection affects characters that involve solitary individuals or individuals in the solitary phase of their life cycle and traits that are not directly affected by competition with conspecifics. In the context of evolution of female sex pheromones, natural selection shapes the pheromone for species recognition (it stabilizes selection), sexual selection promotes differences in pheromone blend (mainly in the amount and ratio of components) among conspecific females, and social selection affects female perception (auto-detection) of the pheromone and behavior that increases reproductive success in the presence of competing females. The selection types are not mutually exclusive and may act simultaneously, reflecting pheromone plasticity in response to ecological constraints, resulting in pheromone plasticity in relation to ecological constraints. For example, the female sex pheromone of Heliothis viresence may change when the moth is sympatric with a closely related species (Groot et al., 2005), thereby responding to the pressures of natural selec- tion. At the same time, the pheromone is costly to produce (Foster and Johnson, 2010) and thus can serve as an honest signal of female size and age, thereby responding to sexual selection.

HARARI AR, STEINITZ H: The evolution of female sex pheromones 571

Table 1 Examples of selection type, the affected trait, and the fitness gained Selection Species Character Fitness component Reference type Natural Bark beetles Pheromone plasticity Avoidance of hybrids Symonds and Elgar, 2004 Natural Hemileuca eglanterina (Lepidoptera) Pheromone plasticity Avoidance of hybrids Mcelfresh and Millar, 2001 Natural Lymantria monacha (Lepidoptera) Pheromone plasticity Avoidance of hybrids Gries et al., 2001 Natural Heliothis virescens (Lepidoptera) Pheromone plasticity Avoidance of hybrids Groot et al., 2005 Natural Drosophila serrate (Diptera) Pheromone plasticity Avoidance of hybrids Higgie et al., 2000 Sexual Lobesia botrana (Lepidoptera) Pheromone characteristics Honest advertisement Harari et al., 2011 Sexual Neoleucinodes elegantalis (Lepidoptera) Pheromone characteristics Honest advertisement Jaffe et al., 2007 Sexual Mamestra brassicae (Lepidoptera) Pheromone characteristics Honest advertisement Noldus and Potting, 1990 Sexual Heliothis zea (Lepidoptera) Pheromone characteristics Honest advertisement Raina et al., 1986 Palaniswamy and Sea- Sexual (Lepidoptera) Calling behavior Competition for mates brook, 1985 Sexual Adoxophyes Sp (Lepidoptera) Calling behavior Competition for mates Noguchi and Tamaki, 1985 Sexual Homona magnanima (Lepidoptera) Calling behavior Competition for mates Noguchi and Tamaki, 1985 Palanaswamy and Sea- Social Choristoneura fumiferana (Lepidoptera) Egg laying Competition for resources brook, 1978 Social Choristoneura rosaceana (Lepidoptera) Egg laying Competition for resources Goekce et al., 2007 Social Lobesia botrana (Lepidoptera) Egg laying Competition for resources Harari et al., 2011 mone characteristics in related sympatric species, such sent (in Bohemia, the Czech Republic) (Gries et al., as those sharing geographical zones or the same host 2001). In their influential studies, Groot and her col- species, are expected. In bark beetles, the most pro- leagues (2005; 2006; 2007; 2009) found temporal varia- nounced differences in the pheromone blends have been tion in the pheromone blend of Heliothis virescens and found among the most closely related beetles in each H. subflexa, suggesting that the pheromone composi- genus (Symonds and Elgar, 2004). Similarly, species tion may be altered based on the female experience from species-rich moth families tend to have more concerning the risk of mating mistakes in sympatric components in their female-released pheromone blend populations and the risk of sexual harassment by Heli- than do species from species-poor families, allowing for coverpa zea males. a larger number of unique blends (Byers, 2006). It is An additional mechanism that may have evolved to useful to consider species that occur in allopatric and reduce interspecific matings is the development of spe- sympatric populations with close relatives. For example, cific chemical receptors that have higher affinity to females of the sheep moths Hemileuca eglanterina and components in the pheromone of a sympatric sister spe- H. nuttalli in allopatry produce the same active com- cies. Adding these components to the otherwise typical pound in their sex pheromone blend (acetate ester). species-specific sex pheromone often reduces male at- When these species are sympatric, H. eglanterina fe- traction to the female (Löfstedt, 1990; Löfstedt et al., males do not produce this component, and H. eg- 1990). lanterina males actively avoid females, which retain this Phelan and Baker (1987) supported the view that sex component in their pheromone blend (Mcelfresh and pheromones have an adaptive role in preventing mating Millar, 2001). Another example is provided by the fruit mistakes. They demonstrated that pheromone-releasing fly Drosophila birchii and D. serrata (Higgie et al., organs in males are more common in closely related 2000). The sex pheromone blend (cuticular hydrocar- species, sharing the same host, than in species in distinct bons) of D. serrata is plastic and differs when in sym- populations. Tortricid moths, for example, lack phero- patry or allopatry with D. birchii. The pheromone blend mone-emitting organs, and only a small portion of this of allopatric D. serrata was altered after breeding for moth family shows host overlap. nine generations in the vicinity of D. birchii. When functioning to prevent mistakes in mating, sex For the forest defoliator nun moth Lymantria pheromones are expected to be under stabilizing selec- monacha, the ratio of the female sex pheromone com- tion (Löfstedt, 1993; Linn and Roelofs, 1995; Butlin and ponents and time of activity are different when its close Trickett, 1997; Phelan, 1997; Zhu et al., 1997; Shaw and relative L. fumida is present (in Honshu, Japan) vs. ab- Parsons, 2002). Indeed, females of the European corn

572 Current Zoology Vol. 59 No. 4 borer moth Ostrinia nubilalis produce two types of sex according to their part in mate choice with respect to pheromone blends that differ only in the ratio of the two species recognition, mate recognition, and mate assess- major components (Linn et al., 1997). As a result, males ment. The first two categories, choice of the correct of one population are less attractive to females in the species and mating status, do not take part in the com- other population (Liu and Haynes, 1994; Linn et al., petition for mates per se, and we have discussed them in 1997; Zhu et al., 1997). Restricted male response to a the current review as part of natural selection (see change in a female pheromone blend was also demon- above). Regarding pheromone-based mate assessment, strated in other species (Löfstedt, 1990; Linn and Table 2 in Johansson and Jones (2007) lists 21 species Roelofs, 1995; Zhu et al., 1997). These findings, how- in which males advertise their quality (fluctuating ever, are in contrast with other studies that demonstrate asymmetry, major histocompatibility complex, immuno- individual variation in female sex pheromones in a competence, competitive ability, aggressiveness, domi- population, both in the quantity and the ratio of compo- nance, and attractiveness) but only two species in which nents (Collins and Cardé, 1985; Witzgall and Frérot, females signal their condition (age and size) through 1989; Löfstedt, 1990; Jaffe et al., 2007; Harari et al., sex-specific pheromones: the copepod Tigriopus ja- 2011). Furthermore, contrary to the prediction evoked ponicus (Ting et al., 2000), and the snake Thamnophis by natural selection, males often respond to a wide array sirtalis (Lemaster and Mason, 2002). of the sex-specific pheromone blends (Löfstedt, 1990; Johansson and Jones (2007) attribute the dearth of Linn and Roelofs, 1995; Roelofs et al., 2002). studies demonstrating the use of sex pheromones as The latter findings, which counter the view that sex advertising traits by female moths to the minute amount pheromones are highly conserved, have led to the in- of sex pheromone typically released (El-Sayed, 2010) ference that changes in pheromone characteristics and its low cost of production (Cardé and Baker, 1984; (within an individual life time and at the population Kokko and Wong, 2007), thus, "…seems an unlikely level) can influence mate choice. Hence, the concept of result if they were subject to mate choice" (Johansson sex pheromones as a secondary trait that is shaped by and Jones, 2007, page 257). Johansson and Jones agree sexual selection has been increasingly considered with the general view that females signal to males when (Johansson and Jones, 2007). the cost of advertising is low, while males signal to fe- 2 Female Sex Pheromones and Sexual males when the cost of advertising is high (Greenfield, 1981; Svensson, 1996; Landolt and Phillips, 1997; Selection Johansson and Jones, 2007). According to this line of According to the broad definition of sexual selection reasoning, female sex pheromones cannot serve as sec- (Andersson, 1994; Shuker, 2010; Tobias et al., 2012), a ondary, extravagant traits that reveal female quality be- sexually selected trait is one that influences intraspecific cause a substantial cost of the trait or of its maintenance competition for mates. This includes competition for the is a prerequisite of honest advertising (Zahavi, 1977; number or quality of mates and for resources that di- Kotiaho, 2000, 2001, 2002). rectly influence the probability of mating. Although the In general, the elaborate traits of females have at- role of chemical signals in sexual selection was first tracted considerably less research attention than those of suggested by Darwin (1871) (Box 1), sex pheromones males (Amundsen, 2000), although the females of many were not often recognized as traits that affecting compe- species do have such traits (Andersson, 1994; Clutton- tition for mates. In his book Sexual Selection, Andersson Brock, 2004; Kraaijeveld et al., 2007). The origin of this (1994) cited less than a handful studies on phero- research bias lays at the feet of Darwin, who suggested mone-mediated mate choice. Similarly, in their review that female secondary traits are derived from the showy of the various ecological, behavioral, and biochemical secondary traits of males (Darwin, 1871; Lande, 1980). aspects of hydrocarbons in insects, Howard and Blom- In the last decade, accumulating research has demon- quist (2005) did not consider the role of hydrocarbons in strated that, like the ornaments of males, those of fe- sexual selection. males may have been selected by female-female compe- More recently, Johansson and Jones (2007) have ad- tition over mates and by male choice of females, i.e., by dressed the challenge to review the rapidly increasing sexual selection (see examples in Clutton-Brock, 2007, number of studies concerning the role of sexual selec- 2009; Rosvall, 2011; Tobias et al., 2012). tion in shaping sex pheromones (Johansson and Jones, Competition among females for mates is expected 2007, Page 265). They categorized sex pheromones when male quality varies and when the operational sex

HARARI AR, STEINITZ H: The evolution of female sex pheromones 573 ratio is female-biased. Evidence from non-pheromone (as suggested by Atema 1986 and demonstrated by Jaffe signals is plentiful (see Rosvall, 2011). In the case of the et al. 2007 and Harari et al. 2011). That pheromones deep-snouted pipefish Syngnathus typhle, for example, advertise female quality is often demonstrated by quan- females compete for mates, displaying their ornamenta- tifying the cost of advertising (Foster and Johnson, 2011; tion to establish a dominance hierarchy, and males con- Harari et al., 2011). Quantity and ratio of components in sider the female hierarchy when choosing their mates the female moth sex pheromone may reveal female size (Berglund and Rosenqvist, 2001). Similarly, female age (Jaffe et al., 2007; Harari et al., 2011) and age (Miller in the pinyon jay Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus is corre- and Roelofs, 1980; Raina et al., 1986; Noldus and Pot- lated with malar and head coloration, which are used to ting, 1990). Cost of pheromone production was behav- assess dominance in female competition for mates iorally demonstrated, i.e., mortality rates were higher (Johnson, 1988). and fecundity was lower for females that released Accepting that sex pheromones function as secon- pheromones than for those that did not (Foster, 2009; dary traits subject to sexual selection (Darwin, 1871; Harari et al., 2011). There is also evidence that females Andersson, 1994; Shuker, 2010), one can make three investing in resistance to insecticides produce reduced predictions. First, females vary in the trait under selec- amounts of pheromones (Campanhola et al., 1991; tion (i.e., the emitted pheromone). Second, females Delisle and Vincent, 2002), indicating a high metabolic compete for mates, advertising their quality via the sex cost of pheromone production (Foster, 2005, 2009; pheromone. Third, males are influenced by the message Foster and Johnson, 2010). conveyed through the pheromone. Concerning the first Female advertising and male mate choice are pre- prediction, individual variation in pheromone characteri- dicted when mating is costly for males (Zahavi, 1977; stics (i.e., the amount and ratio of components) has been Kokko and Monaghan, 2001; Byrne and Rice, 2006), documented for several insect species (e.g., for bark which is often associated with female phenotypic varia- beetles: Birgersson et al., 1988; and for moths: Miller tion (Harari et al., 1999; Kvarnemo and Simmons, 1999; and Roelofs, 1980; Haynes et al., 1984; Safonkin and Jaffe et al., 2007; Harari et al., 2011) and female-biased Bykov, 2006). The variations may include the overall sex ratios (Parker, 1983; Owens and Thompson, 1994; amount of the pheromone and/or a different ratio of the Johnstone et al., 1996). Mating cost for males may in- components in the pheromone blend (Collins and Cardé, clude energetic courtship displays (Segoli et al., 2006), 1985; Witzgall and Frérot, 1989; Löfstedt, 1990; presenting the females with nutritive nuptial gifts Svensson et al., 1997; Jaffe et al., 2007; Harari et al., (Thornhill, 1980; Dewsbury, 1982), intense intrasexual 2011). competition for mates (Bonduriansky and Brooks, 1999; Concerning the second prediction, there is evidence Fromhage and Schneider, 2005), and in the case of all that female moths use female sex pheromones to com- moth species, limited sperm supply (Friedlander et al., pete for mates. For example, females increased calling 2005; Teng and Zhang, 2009). behavior when they sensed pheromones released by In species in which females have multiple mates, conspecific females (Palaniswamy and Seabrook, 1985; sperm competition may affect the reproductive output Stelinski et al., 2006). These females may be attempting of the males. If the pheromone signals the female re- to attract early mate-searching males. Females of other productive state, males may allocate resources, increas- moth species delay calling when exposed to conspecific ing their reproductive potential accordingly. Males of pheromone (Noguchi and Tamaki, 1985; Weissling and the white widow spider, Latrodectus pallidus, for exam- Knight, 1996). These and other examples suggest that ple, performed a significantly shorter display when ap- female calling alters the calling of other conspecific proaching mated females because the risk of cannibali- females and that such changes may affect future repro- sm is considerably reduced (Harari et al., 2009). Addi- ductive success (Goekce et al., 2007; Yang et al., 2009). tionally, the amount of sperm that male moths transfer As is the case for other extravagant traits (Zahavi, to females is correlated with female mating experience 1977), female sex pheromones are expected to honestly and age (Wedell and Cook, 1999; Teng and Zhang, reveal the signaler’s physiological state through rapid 2009). changes in the quality and quantity of their pheromone As opposed to males of most other taxa, male moths (Aliniazee and Stafford, 1971; Webster and Cardé, 1982; stop producing fertile sperm in the pupal stage (Fried- Liu and Haynes, 1994). Thus, the signal should allow lander et al., 2005). As a consequence, sperm reserves males to discriminate among females for reproduction are diminished after each , and the potential

574 Current Zoology Vol. 59 No. 4 number of females that can be inseminated is limited schistus microps, for example, exhibit antagonistic be- (Callahan and Cascio, 1963; Friedlander et al., 2005; havior at sites with a shortage of nests (Borg et al., Teng and Zhang, 2009). In addition, females suffer re- 2002), and only winners of aggressive interactions be- duced reproductive success after mating with experi- tween individuals of the parasitoid Pachycrepoideus enced, polygynous males (Torres-Vila and Jennions, vindemmiae gain access to oviposition sites (Goubault 2005; Lauwers and Van Dyck, 2006; Marcotte et al., et al., 2007). 2006) and thus may discriminate against them. Indeed, Like morphological and behavioural traits, sex males prefer larger, more fecund females (Torres-Vila et pheromones may play a role in intrasexual competition al., 2002), as indicated by the features of the pheromone over breeding resources. Females can detect their air- emitted by the female (Jaffe et al., 2007; Foster and borne species specific pheromone (Palanaswamy and Johnson, 2011; Harari et al., 2011). Seabrook, 1978; Noguchi and Tamaki, 1985; Goekce 3 Female Sex Pheromones and Social et al., 2007). There is evidence that the release of pheromone by females affects the oviposition of con- Selection specific females. Exposure of the female to the sex The currently understood definitions of natural selec- pheromone of its own species stimulates oviposition for tion and sexual selection (Darwin, 1871; Andersson, some moth species, e.g., Choristoneura fumiferana; 1994; Shuker, 2010) leave some behaviors in a grey area. (Palanaswamy and Seabrook, 1978) but deters oviposi- Males and females may differ in their struggle for in- tion by other moth species (Palanaswamy and Seabrook, creased reproductive success. In general, males compete 1978; Goekce et al., 2007; Harari et al., 2011). In both for access to females, while females compete for access cases, mated females compete for oviposition sites. The to resources (Rubenstein, 2012; Tobias et al., 2012). airborne pheromone released by the female provides Although both males and females often use ornaments information about the signaler’s physical condition to achieve higher fecundity, male extravagant traits are (Jaffe et al., 2007; Foster, 2009; Harari et al., 2011) and used to obtain mates directly (e.g., through male-male thus provides information about the forthcoming inten- fighting) or indirectly (e.g., by defending territory), sity of offspring competition for food. This behavior is while females traits are often used to obtain resources similar to female competition at time of pregnancy or other than mates (e.g., food patches and nest sites) that maternal care (Wolff and Peterson, 1998; Rodel et al., are needed for offspring welfare after competition over 2008). mates has ended successfully (Carranza, 2009; Clutton- 4 Summary Brock, 2009). Tobias et al. (2012) followed West- Eberhard (1983) in suggesting that intraspecific compe- Substantial evidence indicates that sex pheromones tition for resources other than mates represents a diffe- in general and female-produced sex pheromones in par- rent form of selection that they termed social selection. ticular function as secondary (signaling) traits. The na- In this context, signaling traits (e.g., ornaments, weap- ture of the selection acting on the female trait should be ons, elaborate displays) may be involved in direct or defined in terms of the resource being competed for. indirect competition for resources other than mates or The definition of the operative selection pressure is not used in non-sexual contests (Moore et al., 1997; Wolf et simply semantic but is relevant to the trait’s potential al., 1999; Mcglothlin et al., 2010; Rosvall, 2011) and adaptive (or non-adaptive) significance. As demon- yet still provide relevant information about the signaler. strated in this review, the female sex pheromone is an One such example of a socially selected trait is the adaptive trait that is moulded by three selection pres- horn of the female dung beetle. The female dung beetle sures: natural, sexual, and social selection. Natural se- uses its horn in intrasexual competition for breeding lection acts on species recognition and reduces the resources (Watson and Simmons, 2010) which affects probability of mismatched matings. Sexual selection the number and quality of surviving offspring. Another acts through female intraspecific competition for mates example may include competition by tropical streak- and male choice of females (although mutual choice is backed orioles for foraging territory during breeding also possible because females still heavily invest in the and non-breeding seasons (Murphy et al., 2009). Fe- offspring). In social selection, females use the trait to males may also benefit from aggressive behavior toward compete against conspecific females for ecological re- conspecific females when breeding opportunities are sources. The contribution of social selection to evolu- costly to find. Females of the common goby Pomato- tion of the female sex pheromone as a signaling trait is

HARARI AR, STEINITZ H: The evolution of female sex pheromones 575 difficult to assess because the evolution of pheromones over attractive females. Behav. Ecol. 12(4): 402–406. has been traditionally attributed to either natural selec- Birgersson G, Schlyter F, Bergstrom G, Lofqvist J, 1988. indivi- tion or sexual selection. However, recent papers demon- dual variation in aggregation pheromone content of the bark beetle Ips typographus. J. Chem. Ecol. 14(9): 1737–1761. strate that female moths use sex pheromones to adver- Bonduriansky R, Brooks RJ, 1999. Why do male antler flies Pro- tise their reproductive potential, and that males use this topiophila litigata fight? The role of male combat in the struc- information when choosing females as mates. Thus, it is ture of mating aggregations on moose antlers. Ethol. Ecol. not unreasonable to hypothesize that female sex phero- Evol. 11(3): 287–301. mones provide information about the intensity of the Borden JH, 1989. Semiochemicals and bark beetle populations: impending competition among offspring for essential Exploitation of Natural phenomena by pest management strategists. Ecography 12(4): 501. resources. There is growing evidence that females can Borg AA, Forsgren E, Magnhagen C, 2002. Plastic sex-roles in detect airborne, species-specific pheromone produced the common goby: The effect of nest availability. Oikos 98(1): by conspecific females and use the information to com- 105. pete for resources or take flight in a search for resources Brennan PA, Kendrick KM, 2006. Mammalian social odours: elsewhere. Attraction and individual recognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 361(1476): 2061–2078. It is interesting and perhaps initially perplexing that Butchart SHM, 2000. Population structure and breeding system of the exact same trait, the female sex pheromone, may be the sex-role reversed, polyandrous bronze-winged jacana subjected to three different selection pressures. Moreo- Metopidius indicus. Ibis. 142(1): 93–102. ver, the selection pressures are expected to have differ- Butlin RK, Trickett AJ, 1997. Can population genetic simulations ent effects on pheromone variance, i.e., natural selection help to interpret pheromone evolution? In: Cardé RT, Minks should reduce variance (stabilizing effect) but sexual AK ed. Insect Pheromone Research: New Directions. New York: Chapman and Hall, 548–562. and social selection should enhance variance. Given the Byers JA, 2006. Pheromone component patterns of moth evolu- importance of the trait in preventing the production of tion revealed by computer analysis of the pherolist. J. Anim. unfit hybrid offspring and in mediating sexual and so- Ecol. 75(2): 399–407. cial communication, plasticity in the female sex phero- Byrne PG, Rice WR, 2006. Evidence for adaptive male mate mone presumably enables its adjustment in response to choice in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 273(1589): 917–922. various environmental and competitive constraints. An Callahan PS, Cascio T, 1963. Histology of the reproductive tracts excellent example of such plasticity is provided by and transmission of sperm in the corn earworni Heliothis zea. Heliothis viresence (see Box 1). Females of this moth Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 56(4): 535–556. species can change their sex pheromone to reduce the Campanhola C, McCutchen BF, Baehrecke EH, Plapp FW, 1991. risk of incorrect mating (Groot et al., 2005) and can also Biological constraints associated with resistance to pyrethroids in the tobacco budworm (Lepidoptera, Noctuidae). J. Econ. apparently use their sex pheromone as an honest signal Entomol. 84(5): 1404–1411. of female size and age (Foster and Johnson, 2010). Cardé RT, Baker TC, 1984. Sexual communication with phero- Whether such plasticity in the female sex pheromone is mones. In: Bell WJ, Cardé RT ed. of In- common among moths and other species warrants addi- sects. London: Chapman & Hall, 355–383. tional research. Carranza J, 2009. Defining sexual selection as sex-dependent selection. Anim. Behav. 77(3): 749–751. References Clutton-Brock T, 2007. Sexual selection in males and females. Science 318(5858): 1885–1882. Aliniazee MT, Stafford EM, 1971. Evidence of a sex pheromone Clutton-Brock T, 2009. Sexual selection in females. Anim. Behav. in the omnivorous leaf roller Platynota stultana (Lepidoptera: 77(1): 3–11. Tortricidae): Laboratory and field testing of male attraction to Clutton-Brock TH, 2004. What is sexual selection? In: Kappeler virgin females. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 64(6): 1330–1335. PM, Schaik CV ed. Sexual Selection in Primates: New and Amundsen T, 2000. Why are female ornamented? Trends in Comparative Perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Ecology & Evolution 15(4): 149–155. Press, 24–36. Andersson M, 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton: Princeton Uni- Collins RD, Cardé RT, 1985. Variation in and heritability of as- versity Press. pects of pheromone production in the pink bollworm moth Atema J, 1986. Review of sexual selection and chemical commu- Pectinophora gossypiella (Lepidoptera: Gelechiidae). Ann. nication in the lobster Homarus americanus. Can. J. Fish. Entomol. Soc. Am. 78(2): 229–234. Aqua. Sci. 43(11): 2283–2390. Darwin C, 1871. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation Baker TC, 2002. Mechanism for saltational shifts in pheromone to Sex. London: John Murray. communication systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 99(21): Delisle J, Vincent C, 2002. Modified pheromone communication 13368–13370. associated with insecticidal resistance in the obliquebanded Berglund A, Rosenqvist G, 2001. Male pipefish prefer dominant leafroller Choristoneura rosaceana (Lepidoptera : Tortricidae).

576 Current Zoology Vol. 59 No. 4

Chemoecology 12(1): 47–51. 2009. Geographic and temporal variation in moth chemical Dewsbury DA, 1982. Ejaculate cost and male choice. Am. Nat. communication. Evolution 63(8): 1987–2003. 119(5): 601–610. Harari AR, Handler AM, Landolt PJ, 1999. Size-assortative mat- Dobzhansky TG, 1937. Genetics and the Origin of Species. New ing, male choice and female choice in the Curculionid beetle York: Columbia University Press. Diaprepes abbreviatus. Anim. Behav. 58(6): 1191–1200. El-Sayed A, 2010.The Pherobase: Database of Insect Pheromones Harari AR, Zahavi T, Thiery D, 2011. Fitness cost of pheromone and Semiochemicals. 2005. www.pherobase.com. Last Date production in signaling female moths. Evolution 65(6): 1572– accessed December 10, 2010. 1582. Endler JA, 1986. Natural Selection in the Wild. Princeton: Harari AR, Ziv M, Lubin Y, 2009. Conflict or cooperation in the Princeton University Press. courtship display of the white widow spider Latrodectus pal- Foster S, 2005. Lipid analysis of the sex pheromone gland of the lidus. J. Arachnol. 37(3): 254–260. moth Heliothis virescens. Arch. Insect Biochem. Physiol. 59(2): Haynes KF, Gaston LK, Pope MM, Baker TC, 1984. Potential for 80–90. evolution of resistance to pheromones: Inter-individual and in- Foster S, 2009. Sugar feeding via trehalose haemolymph con- ter-populational variation in chemical communication system centration affects sex pheromone production in mated Helio- of pink bollworm moth. J. Chem. Ecol. 10(11): 1551–1565. this virescens Moths. J. Exp. Biol. 212(17): 2789–2794. Higgie M, Chenoweth S, Blows MW, 2000. Natural selection and Foster SP, Johnson CP, 2010. Feeding and hemolymph trehalose the reinforcement of mate recognition. Science 290(5491): concentration influence sex pheromone production in virgin 519–521. Heliothis virescens Moths. J. Insect Physiol. 56(11): 1617–1623. Holldobler B, Morgan ED, Oldham NJ, Liebig J, 2001. Recruit- Foster SP, Johnson CP, 2011. Signal honesty through differential ment pheromone in the harvester ant genus Pogonomyrmex. J. quantity in the female-produced sex pheromone of the moth Insect Physiol. 47(4–5): 369–374. Heliothis virescens. J. Chem. Ecol. 37(7): 717–723. Howard RW, Blomquist GJ, 2005. Ecological, behavioral, and Friedlander M, Seth RK, Reynolds SE, 2005. Eupyrene and biochemical aspects of insect hydrocarbons. Annu. Rev. En- apyrene sperm: Dichotomous spermatogenesis in Lepidoptera. tomol. 50: 371–393. In: Simpson SJ ed. Advances in Insect Physiology, Vol 32. San Jaffe K, Miras B, Cabrera A, 2007. Mate selection in the moth Diego: Elsevier Academic Press Inc, 206–308. Neoleucinodes elegantalis: Evidence for a supernormal Fromhage L, Schneider JM, 2005 .Virgin doves and mated hawks: chemical stimulus in sexual attraction. Anim. Behav. 73: 727– Contest behaviour in a spider. Anim. Behav. 70(5): 1099–1104. 734. Goekce A, Stelinski LL, Gut LJ, Whalon ME, 2007. Comparative Johansson BG, Jones TM, 2007. The role of chemical communi- behavioral and eag responses of female obliquebanded and cation in mate choice. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 82(2): redbanded leafroller moths (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) to their 265–289. sex pheromone components. Eur. J. Entomol. 104(2): 187–194. Johnson K, 1988. Sexual selection in pinyon jays II: Male choice Goubault M, Cortesero AM, Poinsot D, Wajnberg E, Boivin G, and female-female competition. Anim. Behav. 36(4): 1048– 2007. Does host value influence female aggressiveness, con- 1053. test outcome and fitness gain in parasitoids? Ethology 113(4): Johnstone RA, Reynolds JD, Deutsch JC, 1996. Mutual mate 334–343. choice and sex differences in choosiness. Evolution 50(4): Grafen A, 1987. Measuring sexual selection: Why bother. In: 1382–1391. Bradbury JW, Andersson MB, Heisler L ed. Sexual Selection: Karlson P, Luscher M, 1959. Pheromones: A new term for a class Testing the Alternatives. Report of the Dahlem Workshop on of biologically active substances. Nature 183(4653): 55–56. Sexual Selection: Testing the Alternatives, Berlin 1986, August Kokko H, Monaghan P, 2001. Predicting the direction of sexual 31–September 5. Chichester; New York: Wiley, 221–233. selection. Ecol. Lett. 4(2): 159–165. Greenfield MD, 1981. Moth sex pheromones: An evolutionary Kokko H, Wong BBM, 2007. What determines sex roles in mate perspective. Fla. Entomol. 64(1): 4–17. searching? Evolution 61(5): 1162–1175. Gries G, Schaefer P, Gries R, Liška J, Gotoh T, 2001. Reproduc- Kotiaho JS, 2000. Testing the assumptions of conditional handi- tive character displacement in Lymantria monacha from cap theory: Costs and condition dependence of a sexually se- Northern Japan? J. Chem. Ecol. 27(6): 1163–1176. lected trait. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48(3): 188–194. Groot A, Gemeno C, Brownie C, Gould F, Schal C, 2005. Male Kotiaho JS, 2001. Costs of sexual traits: A mismatch between and female antennal responses in Heliothis virescens and H. theoretical considerations and empirical evidence. Biol. Rev. subflexa to conspecific and heterospecific sex pheromone 76(03): 365–376. compounds . Environ. Entomol. 34(2): 256–263. Kotiaho JS, 2002. Sexual selection and condition dependence of Groot A, Santangelo R, Ricci E, Brownie C, Gould F et al., 2007. courtship display in three species of horned dung beetles. Be- Differential attraction of Heliothis subflexa males to synthetic hav. Ecol. 13(6): 791–799. pheromone lures in Eastern US and Western Mexico. J. Chem. Kraaijeveld K, Kraaijeveld-Smit FJL, Komdeur J, 2007. The evo- Ecol. 33(2): 353–368. lution of mutual ornamentation. Anim. Behav. 74(4): 657–677. Groot AT, Horovitz JL, Hamilton J, Santangelo RG, Schal C et al., Kvarnemo C, Simmons LW, 1999. Variance in female quality, 2006. Experimental evidence for interspecific directional se- operational sex ratio and male mate choice in a bushcricket. lection on moth pheromone communication. Proc. Natl. Acad. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 45(3–4): 245–252. Sci. USA. 103(15): 5858–5863. Lande R, 1980. Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection, and adapta- Groot AT, Inglis O, Bowdridge S, Santangelo RG, Blanco C et al., tion in polygenic characters. Evolution. 34(2): 292–305.

HARARI AR, STEINITZ H: The evolution of female sex pheromones 577

Landolt PJ, Phillips TW, 1997. Host plant influences on sex phenotypes and the evolutionary process. III. Social evolution. pheromone behavior of phytophagous insects. Annu. Rev. En- Evolution 64(9): 2558–2574. tomol. 42: 371–391. Miller JR, Roelofs WL, 1980. Individual variation in sex Lauwers K, Van Dyck H, 2006. The cost of mating with a pheromone component ratios in two populations of the red- non-virgin male in a monandrous butterfly: Experimental evi- banded leafroller moth Argyrotaenia velutinana. Environ. dence from the speckled wood Pararge aegeria. Behav. Ecol. Entomol. 9(3): 359–363. Sociobiol. 60(1): 69–76. Moore AJ, Brodie ED, Wolf JB, 1997. Interacting phenotypes and Lemaster MP, Mason RT, 2002. Variation in a female sexual at- the evolutionary process .1. Direct and indirect genetic effects tractiveness pheromone controls male mate choice in garter of social interactions. Evolution 51(5): 1352–1362. snakes. J. Chem. Ecol. 28(6): 1269–1285. Murphy TG, Hernandez-Mucino D, Osorio-Beristain M, Mont- Lessells CKM, Bennett ATD, Birkhead TR, Colegrave N, Dall gomerie R, Omland KE, 2009. Carotenoid-based status sig- SRX et al., 2006. Debating sexual selection and mating strate- naling by females in the tropical streak-backed oriole. Behav. gies. Science 312(5774): 689–690. Ecol. 20(5): 1000–1006. Linn CE, Roelofs WL, 1995. Pheromone communication in moths Noguchi H, Tamaki Y, 1985. Conspecific female sex pheromone and its role in the speciation process. In: Lambert DM, Spencer delays calling behavior of Adoxophyes Sp. and Homona mag- HG ed. Speciation and the Recognition Concept: Theory and nanima (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae). Jap. J. Appl. Entomol. Zool. Application. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 263– 29(2): 113–118. 300. Noldus L, Potting RPJ, 1990. Calling behaviour of Mamestra Linn CE, Young MS, Gendle M, Glover TJ, Roelofs WL, 1997. brassicae: Effect of age and photoperiod. Entomol. Exp. Appl. Sex pheromone blend discrimination in two races and hybrids 56(1): 23–30. of the european corn borer moth Ostrinia nubilalis. Physiol. Olsson M, Madsen T, Nordby J, Wapstra E, Ujvari B et al., 2003. Entomol. 22(3): 212–223. Major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in sand liz- Liu YB, Haynes KF, 1994. Temporal and temperature-induced ards. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 270 (Suppl 2): S254–S256. changes in emission rates and blend ratios of sex pheromone Owens IPF, Thompson DBA, 1994. Sex differences, sex ratios components in Trichoplusia ni. J. Insect Physiol. 40(4): 341– and sex roles. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 258(1352): 93–99. 346. Palanaswamy P, Seabrook WD, 1978. Behavioral responses of the Löfstedt C, 1990. Population variation and genetic control of female eastern spruce budworm Choristoneura fumiferana pheromone communication systems in moths. Entomol. Exp. (Lepidoptera, Tortricidae) to the sex pheromone of her own Appl. 54(3): 199–218. species. J. Chem. Ecol. 4(6): 649–655. Löfstedt C, 1993. Moth pheromone genetics and evolution. Philos. Palaniswamy P, Seabrook W, 1985. The alteration of calling be- Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B. 340(1292): 167–177. haviour by female Choristoneura fumiferana when exposed to Löfstedt C, Hansson BS, Dijkerman HJ, Herrebout WM, 1990. synthetic sex pheromone. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 37(1): 13–16. Behavioural and electrophysiological activity of unsaturated Parker GA, 1983. Mate quality and mating decisions. In: Bateson analogues of the pheromone tetradecyl acetate in the small P ed. Mate Choice. New York: Cambridge University Press, ermine moth Yponomeuta rorellus. Physiol. Entomol. 15(1): 141–166. 47–54. Paterson HEH, 1985. The recognition concept of species. In: Vrba Löfstedt C, Herrebout W, Menken SJ, 1991. Sex pheromones and ES ed. Species and Speciation. Pretoria: Transvaal Museum, their potential role in the evolution of reproductive isolation in 21–29. small ermine moths (Yponomeutidae). Chemoecology 2(1): Penn DJ, Potts WK, 1999. The evolution of mating preferences 20–28. and major histocompatibility complex genes. Am. Nat. 153(2): Marcotte M, Delisle J, McNeil JN, 2006. Impact of male mating 145–164. history on the postmating resumption of sexual receptivity and Phelan PL, 1997. Evolution of mate-signaling in moths: Phyloge- lifetime reproductive success in Choristoneura rosaceana Fe- netic considerations and predictions from the asymmetric males. Physiol. Entomol. 31(3): 227–233. tracking hypothesis. In: Choe JC, Crespi RJ ed. Mating Sys- Martin J, Lopez P, 2006. Interpopulational differences in chemical tems in Insects and Arachnids. Cambridge Cambridge Univer- composition and chemosensory recognition of femoral gland sity Press, 240–256. secretions of male Lizards Podarcis hispanica: Implications Phelan PL, Baker TC, 1987. Evolution of male pheromones in for sexual isolation in a species complex. Chemoecology 16(1): moths: Reproductive isolation through sexual selection. Sci- 31–38. ence 235(4785): 205–207. Mas F, Jallon JM, 2005. Sexual isolation and cuticular hydrocar- Raina AK, Klun JA, Stadelbacher EA, 1986. Diel periodicity and bon differences between Drosophila santomea and Drosophila effect of age and mating on female sex pheromone titer in yakuba. J. Chem. Ecol. 31(11): 2747–2752. Heliothis zea (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Mason R, Parker MR, 2010. Social behavior and pheromonal Am. 79(1): 128–131. communication in reptiles. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 196(10): 729– Rispoli VF, Wilson AB, 2008. Sexual size dimorphism predicts the 749. frequency of multiple mating in the sex-role reversed pipefish McElfresh JS, Millar JG, 2001. Geographic variation in the Syngnathus typhle. J. Evol. Biol. 21: 30–38. pheromone system of the saturniid moth Hemileuca eg- Rodel HG, Starkloff A, Bautista A, Friedrich AC, von Holst D, lanterina. Ecology 82(12): 3505–3518. 2008. Infanticide and maternal offspring defence in European McGlothlin JW, Moore AJ, Wolf JB, Brodie ED, 2010. Interacting rabbits under natural breeding conditions. Ethology 114(1):

578 Current Zoology Vol. 59 No. 4

22–31. 367(1600): 2274–2293. Roelofs WL, 1995. Chemistry of sex attraction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Torres-Vila LM, Jennions MD, 2005. Male mating history and Sci. USA. 92(1): 44–49. female fecundity in the Lepidoptera: Do male virgins make Roelofs WL, Liu WT, Hao GX, Jiao HM, Rooney AP et al., 2002. better partners? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 57(4): 318–326. Evolution of moth sex pheromones via ancestral genes. Proc. Torres-Vila LM, Rodriguez-Molina MC, Stockel J, 2002. Delayed Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 99(21): 13621–13626. mating reduces reproductive output of female European grape- Rosvall KA, 2011. Intrasexual competition in females: Evidence vine moth Lobesia botrana (Lepidoptera : Tortricidae). Bull. for sexual selection? Behav. Ecol. 22(6): 1131–1140. Entomol. Res. 92(3): 241–249. Roughgarden J, Oishi M, Akçay E, 2006. Reproductive social Vepsalainen K, Savolainen R, 1995. Operational sex ratios and behavior: Cooperative games to replace sexual selection. Sci- mating conflict between the sexes in the water strider Gerris ence 311(5763): 965–969. lacustris. Am. Nat. 146(6): 869–880. Rubenstein DR, 2012. Sexual and social competition: Broadening Vincent A, Ahnesjo I, Berglund A, Rosenqvist G, 1992. Pipefishes perspectives by defining female roles introduction. Philos. and seahorses: Are they all sex-role reversed? TREE 7: 237– Trans. R. Soc. B. 367(1600): 2248–2252. 241. Safonkin AF, Bykov VI, 2006. Variation in sex pheromone com- Wallace AR, 1890. Darwinism: An Exposition of the Theory of position in large fruit-tree tortrix Archips podana Scop. (Lepi- Natural Selection, with Some of Its Applications. London, doptera: Tortricidae). Biol. Bull. 33(5): 479–482. New York: Macmillan. Segoli M, Harari AR, Lubin Y, 2006. Limited mating opportuni- Watson NL, Simmons LW, 2010. Reproductive competition pro- ties and male monogamy: A field study of white widow spiders motes the evolution of female weaponry. Proc. Roy. Soc. B. Latrodectus pallidus (Theridiidae). Anim. Behav. 72(3): 635– 277(1690): 2035–2040. 642. Webster RP, Cardé RT, 1982. Relationships among pheromone Shaw KL, Parsons YM, 2002. Divergence of mate recognition titre, calling and age in the omnivorous leafroller moth behavior and its consequences for genetic architectures of Platynota stultana. J. Insect Physiol. 28(11): 925–933. speciation. Am. Nat. 159(S3): S61–S75. Wedell N, Cook PA, 1999. Butterflies tailor their ejaculate in Shuker DM, 2010. Sexual selection: Endless forms or tangled response to sperm competition risk and intensity. Proc. R. Soc. bank? Anim. Behav. 79(3): E11–E17. Lond. B. 266(1423): 1033–1039. Stelinski L, Il'Ichev A, Gut L, 2006. Antennal and behavioral Weissling TJ, Knight AL, 1996. Oviposition and calling behavior responses of virgin and mated oriental fruit moth (Lepidoptera : of codling moth (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) in the presence of Tortricidae) females to their sex pheromone. Ann. Entomol. codlemone. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 89(1): 142–147. Soc. Am. 99(5): 898–904. West-Eberhard MJ, 1983. Sexual selection, social competition, Svensson M, 1996. Sexual selection in moths: The role of chemi- and speciation. Q. Rev. Biol. 58(2): 155–183. cal communication. Biol. Rev. 71(1): 113–135. Witzgall P, Frérot B, 1989. Pheromone emission by individual Svensson MGE, Bengtsson M, Lofqvist J, 1997. Individual females of carnation tortrix Cacoecimorpha pronubana. J. variation and repeatability of sex pheromone emission of Chem. Ecol. 15(2): 707–717. female turnip moths Agrotis segetum. J. Chem. Ecol. 23(7): Wolf JB, Brodie ED, Moore AJ, 1999. Interacting phenotypes and 1833–1850. the evolutionary process. II. Selection resulting from social in- Symonds MRE, Elgar MA, 2004. Species overlap, speciation and teractions. Am. Nat. 153(3): 254–266. the evolution of aggregation pheromones in bark beetles. Ecol. Wolff JO, Peterson JA, 1998. An offspring-defense hypothesis for Lett. 7(3): 202–212. territoriality in female mammals. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 10(3): Teng ZQ, Zhang QW, 2009. Determinants of male ejaculate in- 227–239. vestment in the cotton bollworm Helicoverpa armigera: Mat- Wyatt TD, 2003. Pheromones and Animal Behaviour: Communi- ing history, female body size and male age. Physiol. Entomol. cation by Smell and Taste. Cambridge: Cambridge University 34(4): 338–344. Press. Thornhill R, 1980. Mate choice in Hylobittacus apicalis (Insecta: Yang MW, Dong SL, Chen L, 2009. Electrophysiological and Mecoptera) and its relation to some models of female choice. behavioral responses of female beet armyworm Spodoptera Evolution 34(3): 519–538. exigua (Hubner) to the conspecific female sex pheromone. J. Ting JH, Kelly LS, Snell TW, 2000. Identification of sex, age and Insect Behav. 22(2): 153–164. species-specific proteins on the surface of the Harpacticoid Zahavi A, 1977. The cost of honesty (further remarks on the copepod Tigriopus japonicus. Marine Biol. 137(1): 31–37. Handicap Principle). J. Theor. Biol. 67(3): 603–605. Tobias JA, Montgomerie R, Lyon BE, 2012. The evolution of Zhu JW, Chastain BB, Spohn BG, Haynes KF, 1997. Assortative female ornaments and weaponry: Social selection, sexual se- mating in two pheromone strains of the cabbage looper moth lection and ecological competition. Phil.Trans. Roy. Soc. B. Trichoplusia ni. J. Insect Behav. 10(6): 805–817.