Biological Rhythm Research, 2014 Vol. 45, No. 1, 27–36, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2013.787759

The effect of Ramadan intermittent fasting on dynamic postural control in judo athletes Nafaa Souissia,e*, Amira Zouitaa, Hamdi Chtouroub,c, Hbiba Ferchichid, Catherine Dzirid, Salma Abedelmalekc and Nizar Souissib,c,d aBiomechanics Laboratory, National Institute of Orthopedics M.T. Kassab, Tunis, Tunisia; bResearch Laboratory, Sport Performance Optimisation, National Centre of Medicine and Sciences in Sport (CNMSS), Tunis, Tunisia; cResearch Unit (EM2S), High Institute of Sport and Physical Education, Sfax University, Sfax, Tunisia; dService of Medicine Physique- Fonctionnel Readaptation, Institut Kassab of Orthopedie, Manouba, Tunisia; eHigh Institute of Sport and Physical Education, Ksar-Saïd, Manouba University, Manouba, Tunisia

The aim of this study was to evaluate Ramadan intermittent fasting (RIF) effects on dynamic postural control in male judo athletes. In a randomized order, 11 male judo- kas (22.5 ± 2.8 years, 173.8 ± 6.9 cm, and 70.3 ± 4.4 kg; mean ± SD) were asked to perform four protocols of dynamic postural control, i.e., sit to stand test (STS), step up and over test (SUO), step quick turn test (SQT), and forward lunge test (FL), one week before RIF (BRF), during the second week (SWR) of RIF, and three weeks after RIF (AR). The results of the present study showed that during the dynamic bal- ance tests, the movement time, lift-up index, and impact index during the STS test were significantly higher during SWR in comparison with BRF and AR (p < 0.05). However, those measured during the SUO test appeared independent of RIF. The turn time and turn degree during the SQT test and the distance, the impact index, the contact time, and the force impulse increased significantly from BRF and AR to the SWR (p < 0.05). However, no significant difference was observed in all tests (i.e. STS, SUO, SQT, and FL) between BRF and AR. In conclusion, the present study suggested that RIF may negatively affect the postural control of judo athletes. Keywords: dynamic balance; Ramadan intermittent fasting; Judoists

Introduction Ramadan intermittent fasting (RIF) is an Islamic practice, observed by over one billion people worldwide (Aloui et al. 2013; Hammouda et al. 2013). Each year during the lunar month of RIF, Muslims abstain from eating, drinking, smoking, or having sexual relationship during the daytime, between sunrise and sunset (Chtourou et al. 2012). This leads to alterations in feeding habits, sleep duration, pattern, and architecture (Roky et al. 2003; Waterhouse 2010). Well-documented effects of RIF include changes in circadian rhythms (Reilly & Edwards 2007; Roky et al. 2012), physiological (Roky et al. 2004), metabolic, and immune function (Leiper et al. 2008; Chennaoui et al. 2009; Abedelmalek et al. 2011), as well as reductions in daytime hydration, blood glucose, and body temperature (Leiper et al. 2003) physical and cognitive performances are also affected (Chtourou et al. 2011; Hamouda et al. 2012). As major sporting calendars do not take religious observances into account, many athletes continue to train or compete while fasting (Chaouachi et al. 2012).

*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Ó 2013 Taylor & Francis 28 N. Souissi et al.

Partial sleep deprivation can be expected to occur in RIF. Indeed, some studies have demonstrated that during this month, sleep duration decreased and sleep timing shifted (BaHammam 2005, 2006). Recently, Waterhouse (2010) showed that the RIF is associ- ated with change in sleep habits and increased sleepiness. More recently, Rocky et al. (2012) showed that accumulated sleep loss has negative impacts on cognitive function, mood, daytime sleepiness, and performance. In this context, BaHammam (2005, 2006) and Roky et al. (2003) showed an increase in the sleep latency and proportion of non- rapid eye movement sleep, a modification of sleep architecture, and a reduction in the total sleep time and slow-wave sleep. Likewise, during this month, the normal sleep- wake cycle was disrupted by the RIF (Souissi et al. 2008) due to rising earlier and/or retiring later, which can cause partial sleep deprivation (Waterhouse 2010). Thus, sleep loss results in drop in the level of performance during tasks requiring sensorimotor coordination, cognitive processes, and physical performance (Souissi et al. 2008; Reilly & Waterhouse 2009; HajSalem et al. 2013; Jarraya et al. 2013; Abedel- malek et al. 2012). Moreover, sleep loss may affect the balancing ability of the athlete (Bougard & Davenne 2011). More recently, Smith et al. (2012) suggested that extended wake may increase the risk of a fall or other consequences of impaired postural control. In addition, the rhythm of postural control might be influenced by either vigilance and/ or body temperature (Avni et al. 2006; Forsman et al. 2007). Several studies have investigated the effects of sleep deprivation on postural control using center of pressure, center of pressure area, and center of pressure velocity as stabilometric indexes (Uimonen et al. 1994; Caldwell et al. 2003; Gribble & Hertel 2004). The literature indicates that postural control efficiency decreases during the night, while postural sways can be correlated to sleepiness (Liu et al. 2001; Nakano et al. 2001). Bougard and Davenne (2011) showed that one night of partial sleep deprivation reduced the ability to maintain balance (i.e. during the stork stand test of the dominant leg) when tests were performed at 18:00 h. The majority of the studies described previously focused on analyzing the effects of circadian rhythm or sleep deprivation on postural control. To our knowledge, no study has investigated the effect of RIF on dynamic postural control in judokas elites. In this con- text, we recently found that static postural control is affected by RIF (Souissi et al. 2013). It is therefore critical to investigate the effect of RIF on postural control using dynamic tests. Paillard et al. (2002) showed that no differences in static balance are evident in judoists performing at the regional level and judoists performing at the national and international levels. Moreover, Perrin et al. (2002) focused on the differ- ences between judo and dance activities concerning balance control abilities. They found that with eyes open, both judoists and dancers showed better static and dynamic stance than a control group not involved in any sport activity; with eyes closed, only judoists have a significant better stance. This indirectly demonstrated that both activities stimulate different proprioceptive canals. Thus, the ability to maintain equilibrium represents a significant component of suc- cess during a judo match. In light of these observations, it is critical to examine the relation between RIF and dynamic postural control.

Materials and methods Participants Eleven judo athletes (22.5 ± 2.8 years, 173.8 ± 6.9 cm, and 70.3 ± 4.4 kg) volunteered to participate in this study. They were given a thorough explanation of the protocol before Biological Rhythm Research 29 signing an informed consent form. The participants were also selected based on their chronotype using a sleep questionnaire (Horne & Östberg 1976). They had an interme- diate chronotype (i.e. sleep duration between 22:30 ± 1:00 and 07:00 ± 1:00 h). Their mean period of practising judo was 11 ± 6.2 years. The participants’ technical levels were first kyu (brown belt) or first dan (black belt). They competed in 73 kg weight category. Based on results of a self-reported questionnaire, no subject had been treated with any drug that is known to affect immune function, had experienced acute illness from infection during the prior three months, or had smoked tobacco regularly. All judokas participated in official judo competitions during this year and trained 4–5 times per week.

Experimental design In randomized design, participants participated in three experimental conditions: one week before RIF (BRF), during the second week of RIF (SWR), and three weeks after RIF (AR). During each session, the judokas were asked to complete four dynamic tests: the sit to stand test (STS), the step quick turn test (SQT), the step up and over test (SUO), and the forward lunge test (FL) (i.e. 3 trials with 15-s rest interval between rep- etitions). To avoid the effect of time-of-day on postural control (Gribble et al. 2007), testing was conducted at the same time-of-day between 08:00 and 09:00 h. Before the test session, all participants came to the laboratory to become fully familiarized with the procedure and tests involved so as to minimize the learning defects during the experiment. Before each session, body weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg using a Tanita digital scale (Tanita, Tokyo, Japan). Instructions concerning sleep, diet, and physical activity were given to the participants prior to experimentation. Before the month of RIF, participants were synchronized with a nocturnal rest from 23:00 ± 1 to 07:00 h. During the month of RIF, the participants had to go to sleep before 01:30 h and to wake at 07:00 h after a night of uninterrupted sleep. All participants kept the same hours of sleep during the three weeks of the experiment. Spontaneous body movement was assessed continuously by wrist actigraphy (Actiwatch Sleep & Activity software, version 5.32; Cambridge Neurotechnology, Cambridge, UK). Total sleep duration decreased significantly from BRF (07:30 ± 00:30 h) to SWR (05:41 ± 00:20 h). The average (Mean ± SD) sleeping time of the participants was 01:54 ± 00:25 h less during SWR compared with BRF and AR, during which the partici- pants had refrained from eating or drinking during the daytime. All meals were eaten at a standard time within the participant’s usual schedules and RIF customs. There were also dietary restrictions prohibiting any food or drink that could enhance wakefulness, or agents such as alcohol. The participants were required to record their food intakes in a diary over a span of three days for each week of physical testing.

Posturographic assessments The measurement of postural stability was done using NeuroCom®Balance Master® (NeuroCom Internationnal; Software Version 7). Protocols were selected from a broad spectrum, and each parameter was measured as a three-trials average (Mean ± SD) score during the four dynamic tests (i.e. STS, SQT, SUO, and FL). 30 N. Souissi et al.

Dynamic balance testing The STS test During the STS, the subject was asked to sit on a height-adjustable seat, the level of which was set at knee height and located on a force platform, followed by extension of the body to an erect standing position. Participants were asked to position their feet parallel on the force platform and forming a 90° angle with the shank and to keep their trunk vertical and their arms folded across their chest. Feet positions were traced on the plate to ensure exact repositioning after each trial. The measured parameters are weight transfer time, rising index (force exerted to rise), and sway velocity during the rising phase.

The SQT test During the SQT, the subject took two steps forward and quickly pivoted 180° on the test leg and returned to the starting position at the head of the force plate. Turn time, or the time it took the subject to complete only the turn and turn sway, the postural stabil- ity during the turn time were recorded.

The SUO test During the SUO, the subject stood behind a 30-cm high box placed laterally across the force plate. The subject stepped onto the box with the designated leg and lifted the opposite leg over to the other side of the box and back onto the force plate. The lift-up index, movement time, and impact index were measured. The lift-up index was defined as the maximum concentric lifting force (percent body weight) exerted by the leading leg. Movement time was defined as the number of seconds between the initial weight shift of the non-stepping leg and the contact of the same leg with the force platform. In theory, an unstable lower extremity should be less able to “control” the descent of the opposite leg and result in higher lift-up and impact indexes (Chu & Held 1999).

The FL test Participants performed the FL by taking a stride forward and flexing the hip and knee of the forward leg up to 90° and then returning upright to the starting position. The test was performed on the left and then the right leg. The distance, impact index, contact time, and force impulse of the lunging extremity were recorded. The distance that the center of gravity moved forward was expressed as a percentage of the subject’s height. The impact index was the maximum vertical force exerted onto the force plate during landing and was expressed as a percentage of body weight. Contact time was the dura- tion of the lunging leg in contact with the force plate. Force impulse was a measure of the total work performed by the lunging leg during the landing and thrust phases of movement (percent body weight  seconds force was exerted).

Statistical analyses Statistical tests were processed using STATISTICA software (StatSoft, France). Data were reported as mean ± SD. The Shapiro-Wilk W-test of normality revealed that the data were normally distributed. Once the assumption of normality was confirmed, parametric tests were performed. All parameters (i.e. movement time, lift up index, impact index, turn time, turn degree, distance, contact time, and force impulse) were Biological Rhythm Research 31 analyzed using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). When appropriate, significant differences among means were tested using the least significant difference post hoc test. The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Results Dynamic balance testing Data of the movement time, lift-up index, and impact index during the STS, SQT, SUO, and FL tests and during the two test sessions (i.e. BRF, SWR, and AR) are pre- sented in Tables 1, 2, 3, and 4 respectively. Concerning STS test, ANOVA showed that the movement time (p < 0.05), lift-up index (p < 0.05), and impact index (p < 0.01) were significantly higher during SWR in comparison with BRF and AR. Likewise, the data of the turn time and turn degree on the right and left foot during the SQT showed that these parameters increased signifi- cantly from BRF and AR to SWR (p < 0.05). Concerning FL, statistical analysis showed that distance, impact index, contact time, and force impulse of the left foot were significantly higher during SWR in comparison with BRF and AR (p < 0.05). However, no significant difference was observed for the distance and impact index of the right foot between BRF and SWR. Moreover, no sig- nificant difference was observed for the movement time, lift-up index, and impact index on the right and left foots during the SUO test between BRF, SWR, and AR. Likewise, for all parameters, no significant differences were observed between BRF and AR.

Discussion The main purpose of this study was to evaluate the influence of RIF on postural control during dynamic balance tests in judo athletes. The results showed that RIF seems to influence measures of postural control of judokas during the four dynamic tests (i.e. STS, SQT, SUO, and FL). To our knowledge, the effects of RIF on postural control during dynamic tests have not been investigated. In a recent study we showed a significant effect of RIF on the

Table 1. Movement time, lift-up index, and impact index during the STS test measured during BRF, SWR, and AR (mean ± SD; n = 11).

BRF SWR AR

Weight transfer time (s) 0.38 ± 0.26 0.58 ± 0.28⁄ 0.36 ± 0.35 Rising index (% of body weight) 34.67 ± 10.3 41.79 ± 7.55⁄ 32.67 ± 7.1 Sway velocity (deg/s) 2.7 ± 0.95 3.91 ± 0.56⁄⁄ 2.2 ± 0.88

⁄. ⁄⁄: significant difference with SWR at the levels of p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, respectively.

Table 2. Turn time and turn degree on the right and the left foot during the SQT test measured during BRF, SWR, and AR (mean ± SD; n = 11).

Left foot Right foot BRF SWR AR BRF SWR AR

Turn time (s) 0.64 ± 0.19 0.81 ± 0.34⁄ 0.59 ± 0.23 0.68 ± 0.2 0.97 ± 0.33⁄ 0.66 ± 0.12 Turn sway (deg) 18.68 ± 4.52 22.5 ± 3.47⁄ 17. 33 ± 4.21 18.83 ± 4.44 22.99 ± 4.16⁄ 18.56 ± 4.35

⁄: significant difference between before and during RIF at the level of p < 0.05. 32 N. Souissi et al.

Table 3. movement time, lift-up index, and impact index on the right and the left foot during the SUO test measured during BRF, SWR, and AR (mean ± SD; n = 11).

Left foot Right foot BRF SWR AR BRF SWR AR

Movement 1.42 ± 0.25 1.49 ± 0.24 1.43 ± 0.19 1.44 ± 0.2 1.48 ± 0.21 1.41 ± 0.28 time (s) Lift up index 48.15 ± 7.53 51.67 ± 9 49.16 ± 6.38 55.7 ± 10.27 55.93 ± 8.3 54.21 ± 6.43 (% of body weight) Impact index 67.47 ± 10.08 69.83 ± 11.26 68.46 ± 9. 6 64.37 ± 12.01 67.9 ± 15.19 68.53 ± 10.2 (% of body weight) static postural control tests (Souissi et al. 2013). It is, therefore, difficult to compare our results referring to the literature. In addition, the mechanism responsible for such an effect is difficult to understand at present. However, it seems possible to propose some hypotheses. As Muslims rise earlier and/or retire late during RIF, the normal sleep-wake cycle is disturbed, and this can cause a partial sleep deprivation (Roky et al. 2003; BaHammam 2005, BaHammam et al. 2010; Waterhouse 2010). In this context, numer- ous studies have reported impaired postural control, following sleep deprivation (Liu et al. 2001; Nakano et al. 2001; Patel et al. 2007). Gomez et al. (2008) showed that 36 h of sleep deprivation could negatively affect the postural control. To date, the results of the scientific literature concerning the effect of sleep deprivation on balance abilities recorded in the morning (between 08:00 and 09:00 h), as in the present study, are controversial. In fact, although some studies highlighted a deleterious effect (Nakano et al. 2001; Avni et al. 2006; Patel et al. 2008), others found no effect (Uimonen et al. 1994; Gribble & Hertel 2004; Patel et al. 2008; Bougard & Davenne 2011; Bougard et al. 2011) or observed a decrease in postural efficiency only under particularly demanding conditions (Bougard & Davenne 2011; Bougard et al. 2011). Our results show no significant differences in all parameters. These findings are in accordance with Chennaoui et al. (2009) who showed that all the physiological and metabolic parameters return to their initial state after three weeks of RIF. Various assumptions have been made to explain sleep deprivation effects on postural control. In this context, previous studies suggested that the increase in postural sway, following sleep deprivation, is due to the decrease in vestibular system efficiency (Kohen-Raz et al. 1996; Avni et al. 2006; Morad et al. 2007) and/or the integration of the various sensory inputs (Teasdale & Simoneau 2001). In fact, maintaining a stable base of support during a balance control task (i.e. static and dynamic) requires the inte- gration of sensory input to create the appropriate motor responses needed to make limb and trunk corrections (Gribble et al. 2007). It has been shown that psychomotor performances, such as critical flicker fusion (Ali & Amir 1989), subjective alertness (Lagarde et al. 1996), and memory (Hakkou et al. 1988, 1994), were reduced during RIF. These RIF-related changes might, in part, explain the findings of the present study. In fact, the task of minimizing one’s base of support require a heightened level of attention (Gribble et al. 2007). Recently, Dolu et al. (2007) showed that attention levels decreased during RIF. In this context, previous studies showed that alertness is impaired by caloric restriction or sleep length limitation Table 4. distance, impact index, contact time, and force impulse on the right and the left foot during the FL test during BRF and SWR (mean ± SD; n = 11).

Left foot Right foot BRF SWR AR BRF SWR AR ⁄ Distance (% of body height) 50.24 ± 3.39 51.64 ± 3.81 50.43 ± 1.17 51.39 ± 4.29 52.3 ± 4.86 51.41 ± 4.28 ⁄⁄ Impact index (% of body weight) 33.03 ± 6.27 37.95 ± 7.75 34.3 ± 6.32 35.12 ± 5.44 35.73 ± 9.59 34.21 ± 6.43 ⁄ Contact time (s) 0.94 ± 0.13 1.07 ± 0.23 0.92 ± 0.6 0.89 ± 0.1 1.02 ± 0.17⁄ 0.83 ± 0.13 ⁄ ⁄

Force impulse (% of body weight/s) 106.91 ± 2.8 117.67 ± 23.16 108.87 ± 3.4 101.52 ± 8.32 113.79 ± 15.9 102.72 ± 9.41 Research Rhythm Biological

⁄ ⁄⁄ . : significant difference between before and during RIF at the levels of p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, respectively. 33 34 N. Souissi et al.

(Pilcher & Huffcutt 1996; Dinges et al. 1997). Since there is a diurnal variation in alertness as measured by subjective rating scale (Monk 1994), by immediate memory test (Folkard & Monk 1980), logical reasoning test (Folkard et al. 1983), and manual dexterity test, the decrease in alertness in the morning during RIF could be related to two factors: (1) the delay in rising time (i.e. sleepiness, especially in the morning) and (2) the abstinence of breakfast with its usual coffee drink content (Smith et al. 1994; Benton & Parker 1998; Roky et al. 2000, 2003). Our study was limited to a small group of athletes; furthermore, the participants were well-trained males, and it remains unclear whether the results can be applied to sedentary or patient participants. In conclusion, the present study suggested that RIF may negatively affect the pos- tural control of judo athletes. In fact, dynamic equilibrium was worthy during SWR, in comparison with BRF. This effect might, in part, result in a clearly decreased perfor- mance of judoka during competitions scheduled in RIF. This is an important point for coaches who plan training programs, and it may benefit athletes because it implies that all training sessions must be programed throughout the day taking into consideration the effect of RIF on judoka’s postural control.

Acknowledgments The authors declare no conflict of interest. This study was supported by the Ministry of Higher Teaching and Scientific Research, Tunisia. We thank all participants for participating in this study.

References Abedelmalek S, Chtourou H, Aloui A, Aouichaoui C, Souissi N, Tabka Z. Forthcoming 2012. Effect of time of day and partial sleep deprivation on plasma concentrations of IL-6 during a short-term maximal performance. Eur J Appl Physiol. 113:241–248. Abedelmalek S, Souissi N, Takayuki A, Hadouk S, Tabka Z. 2011. Effect of acute maximal exer- cise on circulating levels of Interleukin-12 during Ramadan fasting. Asian J Sports Med. 2:154–160. Ali M, Amir T. 1989. Efects of fasting on visual flicker fusion. Percept Mot Skills. 69:627–631. Aloui A, Chtourou H, Hammouda O, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, Souissi N. 2013. The challenge of rapid weight loss prior to competition for Muslim combat sport athletes during Ramadan. Biol Rhythm Res. [Internet]. Available from: http://DOI:10.1080/09291016.2013.780698. Avni N, Avni I, Barenboim E, Azaria B, Zadok D, Kohen-Raz R, Morad Y. 2006. Brief posturo- graphic test as an indicator of fatigue. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 60:340–346. BaHammam A. 2005. Assessment of sleep patterns, daytime sleepiness, and chronotype during Ramadan in fasting and non-fasting individuals. Saudi Med J. 26:616–622. BaHammam A. 2006. Does Ramadan fasting affect sleep? Int J Clin Pract. 60:1631–1637. BaHammam A, Alrajeh M, Albabtain M, Bahammam S, Sharif M. 2010. Circadian pattern of sleep, energy expenditure, and body temperature of young healthy men during the intermittent fasting of Ramadan. Appetite. 54:426–429. Benton D, Parker P. 1998. Breakfast, blood glucose and cognition. Am J Clin Nutr. 67:772S–778S. Bougard C, Davenne D. 2011. Effects of sleep deprivation and time-of-day on selected physical abilities in off-road motorcycle riders. Eur J Appl Physiol. 1121:59–67. Bougard C, Lepelley M, Davenne D. 2011. The influences of time-of-day and sleep deprivation on postural control. Exp Brain Res. 2091:109–115. Caldwell A, Prazinko B, Caldwell J. 2003. Body posture affects electroencephalographic activity and psychomotor vigilance task performance in sleep-deprived participants. Clin Neurophysi- ol. 1141:23–31. Chaouachi A, Leiper J, Chtourou H, Aziz A, Chamari K. 2012. The effects of Ramadan intermit- tent fasting on athletic performance: recommendations for the maintenance of physical fitness. J Sports Sci. 30:53–73. Biological Rhythm Research 35

Chennaoui M, Desgorces F, Drogou C, Boudjemaa B, Tomaszewski A, Depiesse F, Gomez-Mer- ino D. 2009. Effects of Ramadan fasting on physical performance and metabolic, hormonal, and inflammatory parameters in middle-distance runners. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 34: 587–594. Chtourou H, Hammouda O, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, Souissi N. 2012. The effect of time-of-day and Ramadan fasting on anaerobic performances. Int J Sports Med. 33:142–147. Chtourou H, Hammouda O, Souissi H, Chamari K, Chaouachi A, Souissi N. 2011. The effect of Ramadan fasting on physical performances, mood state and perceived exertion in young foot- ballers. Asian J Sports Med. 2:177–185. Chu D, Held J. 1999. Objective assessment of the acl-injured patient: weight bearing, steeping up/down and lunging. Neurocom Int. 1:1–11. Dinges D, Pack F, Williams K, Gillen K, Powell J, Ott G, Aptowicz C, Pack A. 1997. Cumula- tive sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep. 20:267–277. Dolu N, Yüksek A, Sizer A, Alay M. 2007. Arousal and continuous attention during Ramadan intermittent fasting. J Basic Clin Physiol Pharmacol. 184:315–322. Folkard S, Monk T. 1980. Circadian rhythms in human memory. Br J Psychol. 71:295–307. Folkard S, Wever R, Wildgruber C. 1983. Multi-oscillatory control of circadian rhythms in human performance. Nature. 305:223–226. Forsman P, Haeggström E, Wallin A, Toppila E, Pyykkö I. 2007. Daytime changes in postural stabil- ity and repeatability of posturographic measurements. J Occup Environ Med. 496:591–596. Gomez S, Patel M, Berg S, Magnusson M, Johansson R, Fransson P. 2008. Effects of propriocep- tive vibratory stimulation on body movement at 24 and 36 h of sleep deprivation. Clin Neuro- physiol. 119:617–625. Gribble A, Hertel J. 2004. Changes in postural control during a 48-h sleep deprivation period. Percept Mot Skills. 99:1035–1045. Gribble PA, Tucker WS, White PA. 2007. Time-of-day influences on static and dynamic postural control. J Athl Training. 42:35–41. HajSalem M, Chtourou H, Aloui A, Hammouda O, Souissi N. 2013. Effects of partial sleep deprivation at the end of the night on anaerobic performances in judokas. Biol Rhythm Res. [Internet]. Available from: http://DOI:10.1080/09291016.2012.756282. Hakkou F, Iraki L, Tazi A. 1994. Ramadan, chronobiology and health. Chronobiol Int. 11:340– 342. Hakkou F, Wast D, Jaouen C. 1988. Does Ramadan impair vigilance and memory. Psychophar- macology. 1:96–213. Hammouda O, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chahed H, Bellimem H, Chamari K, Souissi N. 2013. Time-of-day effects on biochemical responses to soccer-specific endurance in elite Tunisian football players. J Sports Sci. [Internet]. Available from: http://DOI:10.1080/02640414. 2012.757345. Hamouda O, Chtourou H, Farjallah MA, Davenne D, Souissi N. 2012. The effect of Ramadan fasting on the diurnal variations in aerobic and anaerobic performances in Tunisian youth soc- cer players. Biol Rhythms Res. 432:177–190. Horne JA, Ostberg O. 1976. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness–evening- ness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 42:97–110. Jarraya M, Jarraya S, Chtourou C, Souissi N, Chamari K. 2013. The effect of partial sleep depri- vation on the reaction time and the attentional capacities of the . Biol Rhythm Res. [Internet]. Available from: http://DOI:10.1080/09291016.2012.721589. Kohen-Raz R, Himmelfarb M, Tzur S, Kohen-Raz A, Shub Y. 1996. An initial evaluation of work fatigue and circadian changes as assessed by multiplate posturography. Percept Mot Skills. 82:547–557. Lagarde D, Batejat D, Boussif M, Pradella S, Girault S, Huppe M. 1996. Ramadan et vigilance. Med Aeronaut Spat. 35:175–182. Leiper JB, Molla AM, Molla AM. 2003. Effects on health of fluid restriction during fasting in Ramadan. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2:30–38. Leiper JB, Watson P, Evans G, Dvorak J. 2008. Intensity of a training session during Ramadan in fasting and non-fasting Tunisian youth football players. J Sports Sci. 3:S71–S79. Liu Y, Higuchi S, Motohashi Y. 2001. Cahnges in postural sways during a period of sustained wakefulness in male adults. Occup Med. 8:490–495. 36 N. Souissi et al.

Monk T. 1994. Circadian rhythms in subjective activation, mood and performance efficiency. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. Philadel- phia (PA): W.B. Saunders; p. 321–330. Morad Y, Azaria B, Avni I, Barkana Y, Zadok D, Kohen-Raz R, Barenboim E. 2007. Posturogra- phy as an indicator of fatigue due to sleep deprivation. Aviat Space Environ Med. 78:859– 863. Nakano T, Araki K, Michimori A, Inbe H, Hagiwara H, Koyama E. 2001. Nineteen-hour varia- tion of postural sway, alertness and rectal temperature during sleep deprivation. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 55:277–278. Paillard T, Costes-salon C, Lafont C, Dupui P. 2002. Are there differences in postural regulation according to the level of competition in judoists? Br J Sports Med. 36:304–305. DOI: 10.1136/ bjsm.36.4.304. Patel M, Gomez S, Berg S, Almbladh P, Lindblad J, Petersen H, Magnusson M, Johansson R, Fransson PA. 2007. Effects of 24-h and 36-h sleep deprivation on human postural control and adaptation. Exp Brain Res. 1852:165–173. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-007-1143-5. Patel M, Gomez S, Berg S, Almbladh P, Lindblad J, Petersen H, Magnusson M, Johansson R, Fransson PA. 2008. Effects of 24-h and 36-h sleep deprivation on human postural control and adaptation. Exp Brain Res. 185:165–173. Perrin P, Deviterne D, Hugel F, Perrot C. 2002. Judo, better than dance, develops sensorimotors adaptabilities involved in balance control. Gait Posture. 15:187–194. DOI: 10.1016/S0966- 63620100149-7. Pilcher J, Huffcutt A. 1996. Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: a meta-analysis. Sleep. 19:318–326. Reilly T, Edwards B. 2007. Altered sleep-wake cycles and physical performance in athletes. Phys- iol Behav. 90:274–284. Reilly T, Waterhouse J. 2009. Sports performance: is there evidence that the body clock plays a role? Eur J Appl Physiol. 106:321–332. Roky R, Iraki L, HajKhlifa R, Lakhdar Ghazal N, Hakkou F. 2000. Daytime alertness, mood, psychomotor performances, and oral temperature during Ramadan intermittent fasting. Ann Nutr Metab. 44:101–107. Roky R, Chapotot F, Benchekroun M, Benaji B, Hakkou F, Elkhalifi H, Buguet A. 2003. Daytime sleepiness during Ramadan intermittent fasting: polysomnographic and quantitative waking EEG study. J Sleep Res. 122:95–101. Roky R, Herrera CP, Ahmed Q. 2012. Sleep in athletes and the effects of Ramadan. J Sports Sci. 30:75–84. Roky R, Houti I, Moussamih S, Qotbi S, Aadil N. 2004. Physiological and chronobiological changes during Ramadan intermittent fasting. Ann Nutr Metab. 48:296–303. Smith A, Kendrick A, Maben A, Salmon J. 1994. Effects of breakfast and caffeine on cognitive performance, mood and cardiovascular functioning. Appetite. 22:39–55. Smith S, Cheng T, Kerr G. 2012. The effect of extended wake on postural control in young adults. Exp Brain Res. 221:329–335. Souissi N, Souissi M, Souissi H, Chamari K, Tabka Z, Dogui M, Davenne D. 2008. Effect of time of day and partial sleep deprivation on short-term, high-power output. Chronobiol Int. 25:1062–1076. Souissi N, Chtourou H, Zouita A, Dziri C, Souissi N. 2012. Effects of Ramadan intermittent fast- ing on postural control in judo athletes. Biol Rhythm Res. 44:237–244. Teasdale N, Simoneau M. 2001. Attentional demands for postural control: the effects of aging and sensory reintegration. Gait Posture. 14:203–210. Uimonen S, Laitakari K, Bloigu R, Sorri M. 1994. The repeatability of posturographic measure- ments and the effects of sleep deprivation. J Vestib Res. 4:29–36. Waterhouse J. 2010. Effects of Ramadan on physical performance: chronobiological consider- ations. Br J Sports Med. 44:509–515. Copyright of Biological Rhythm Research is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.