Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1075-7

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Thinking about the past and in daily life: an experience sampling study of individual differences in

Roger E. Beaty1 · Paul Seli2 · Daniel L. Schacter3

Received: 18 January 2018 / Accepted: 10 August 2018 / Published online: 20 August 2018 © Springer-Verlag GmbH , part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract Remembering the past and imagining the future are hallmarks of mental time travel. We provide evidence that such expe- riences are influenced by individual differences in temporal and affective biases in cognitive style, particularly brooding rumination (a negative past-oriented bias) and (a positive future-oriented bias). Participants completed a 7-day, cellphone-based experience-sampling study of temporal orientation and mental imagery. Multilevel models showed that individual differences in brooding rumination predicted less vivid and positive past- and future-oriented thoughts, even after controlling for depressed mood. People high in brooding rumination were also more likely to report thinking about a past experience when probed at random during the day. Conversely, optimists were more likely to report more vivid and positive future-oriented, but not past-oriented thoughts, although they did not report thinking more or less often about the past and future. The results suggest that temporal and affective biases in cognitive style influence how people think about the past and future in daily life.

Introduction to form mental representations that support our ability to simulate both past and future events (e.g., Schacter & Addis, Mentally revisiting the past and projecting the future are 2007; Schacter, Benoit, & Szpunar, 2017). defining features of mental time travel. Remembering past Numerous studies have documented striking cognitive experiences is undoubtedly important for normal functioning and neural similarities between remembered past experience as it allows us to learn from our experiences and to develop and imagined future experiences (for reviews, see Szpunar, a temporally stable sense of self (i.e., autonoetic conscious- 2010; Schacter et al., 2012). For example, studies using ness; Tulving, 1985). Likewise, imagining the future serves functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have shown several adaptive functions (Schacter, 2012). For instance, it remarkably similar patterns of brain activation when people allows us to plan future events and to engage in important recall an event from their past and when they imagine an decision-making processes that influence our future lives event in their future (e.g., Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2007; (e.g., Baird, Smallwood, & Schooler, 2011; D’Argembeau, Szpunar, Watson, & McDermott, 2007; Schacter, Addis, Renaud, & Van der Linden, 2011; Stawarczyk, Cassol, & & Buckner, 2007; for a recent meta-analysis, see Benoit & D’Argembeau, 2013; Stawarczyk & D’Argembeau, 2015). A Schacter, 2015). Other experimental work has documented notable theme in the literature on mental time travel is that overlap among the underlying cognitive processes and phe- future thinking, such as episodic memory, is a reconstructive nomenological characteristics associated with imagining process that draws upon prior knowledge and experiences the future and remembering the past (e.g., D’Argembeau & Mathy, 2011; McDermott, Wooldridge, Rice, Berg, & Szpu- * Roger E. Beaty nar, 2016; Schacter & Madore, 2016; Szpunar & McDer- [email protected] mott, 2008). Several recent studies have examined past- and future- 1 Department of , Pennsylvania State University, oriented in the context of mind-wandering, or 140 Moore Building, University Park, PA 16801, USA thoughts that arise independently of external stimulation 2 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke (for a review, see Seli et al., 2018). Such work has used University, Durham, USA experience-sampling methods to probe temporal orienta- 3 Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, tion and other phenomenological characteristics during USA

Vol.:(0123456789)1 3 806 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 laboratory tasks and in daily life by periodically interrupting people suffering from disorders (Brown et al., 2014; participants with thought probes (Kane et al., 2007; Poerio, McNally, Litz, Prassas, Shin, & Weathers, 1994; Wu, Szpu- Totterdell, & Miles, 2013; Song & Wang, 2012; Spronken, nar, Godvich, Schacter, & Hofmann, 2015; for review, see Holland, Figner & Dijksterhuis, 2016). Spronken et al. Miloyan, Bulley, & Suddendorf, 2016). (2016) found that future-oriented thoughts are rated as more Research with nonclinical populations has also shown positive than past-oriented thoughts in the lab and in daily considerable variability in the extent to which people can life, consistent with the optimism bias of future-oriented construct detailed mental representations about the future cognition (Weinstein, 1980). Moreover, Poerio et al. (2013) (e.g., D’Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2006). Moreover, assessed mood before and after mind-wandering episodes in individual variation in executive functioning is predictive daily life and found that sad mood often preceded mind-wan- of the quantity and quality of episodic detail in future sim- dering (contrary to prior work suggesting that sad mood is a ulations (Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2008; D’Argembeau, consequence of mind-wandering; Killingsworth & Gilbert, Ortoeleva, Jumentier, & Van der Linden, 2010; Hill & 2010), and that sad mood prior to mind-wandering episodes Emery, 2013), pointing to the potential importance of cog- was more frequently past-oriented. nitive flexibility. Indeed, a recent functional brain imaging Contemporary mind-wandering research emphasizes the study found that activity within neural systems engaged distinction between spontaneous (unintentional) and delib- during more flexible constructions of future scenarios (i.e., erate (intentional) mind-wandering (Seli, Risko, Smilek, incongruent with past events) correlated with individual dif- & Schacter, 2016). In terms of temporal orientation, delib- ferences in divergent creative thinking, or the ability to gen- erate future-oriented thoughts may involve strategic and erate a range of possible solutions to open-ended problems deliberate planning of an upcoming event (e.g., thinking (Roberts et al., 2017). Taken together, this growing body of about what items to buy at the grocery store). Spontaneous research suggests that individual differences in cognitive and future thoughts, on the other hand, occur without conscious affective processes influence how people remember the past initiation and may arise during an ongoing task (i.e., task- and project the future. unrelated thoughts), despite one’s best to focus Although future thinking relies on past experience attention on the task at hand (Seli, Risko, & Smilek, 2016). (Schacter & Addis, 2007), projecting into the future also According to Christoff et al., the “default state” of mind- requires a partial break from the past to successfully con- wandering is spontaneous—freely moving from one topic struct new simulations of what one has yet to experience. to another—but various emotional and cognitive factors can This process may, therefore, be disrupted by an inability impact the content and variability of thoughts via constraints to move beyond past experience—in other words, getting (Christoff, Irving, Fox, Spreng, & Andrews-Hanna, 2016). “stuck” in a recursive loop of past-oriented thought (e.g., Negative emotion, for example, can constrain the sponta- ruminating) should yield greater challenges in shifting atten- neous variability of thoughts by inducing a past-oriented, tion towards the future. On the other hand, a tendency to perseverative cognitive style. focus on future-related experiences might impact the con- Despite considerable experimental work on past- and structive process by biasing attention toward the future. future-oriented cognition, relatively less is known about In the research, we examined the role of cognitive how and why people differ in these abilities. Research with styles characterized by past- and future-oriented thought in clinical populations, however, suggests that an over-general mental time travel. Specifically, we explored whether brood- memory bias—describing the past and future in broad, cat- ing rumination, a tendency to perseverate on negative past egorical terms—contributes to deficits in both remember- experiences, is related to decreases in people’s ability to ing and imagining (e.g., Brown et al., 2014; D’Argembeau, vividly and positively imagine the future. We also explored Rafford, & Van der Linden, 2008; Williams, Ellis, Tyers, & whether optimism, a tendency to expect positive future Healy, 1996). When recalling specific life events, depressed events, is related to enhanced imagery for the future. Criti- patients tend to offer a summary of related past experiences cally, although rumination and optimism have been linked that are comprised of minimal episodic detail (Watkins & to past- and future-oriented cognition, respectively, in a lab Teasdale, 2001; but see Kvavilashvili & Schlagman, 2011). context, it remains unclear whether these traits similarly Past work has found that this overgeneral memory bias correspond to past- and future-oriented thoughts in daily extends to rumination, a tendency to perseverate on nega- life, which are more likely to be spontaneous than lab-based tive past-oriented information, which has been attributed to assessments of past and future thinking. deficits in executive control during retrieval and to atten- Using an experience-sampling design, we examined tional capture to negatively valenced mnemonic informa- temporal orientation and mental imagery by calling peo- tion (Williams, Barnhofer, Crane, Herman, Raes, Watkins, ple on their cellphones at random times throughout the day & Dalgleish, 2007). Studies have likewise reported reduced for 1 week and asking if they were thinking about the past, specificity of episodic memories and future simulations in present, or future; we also asked them to rate the vividness,

1 3 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 807 valence, and temporal distance of their thoughts. This receive survey calls (e.g., 10 a.m.–10 p.m.). An interactive approach allowed us to explore how temporal, cognitive, and voice response (IVR) system administered the automated sur- affective biases in thinking style impact people’s momentary veys (Burgin, Silvia, Eddington, & Kwapil, 2013). The sys- conscious experiences of the past, present, and future. Previ- tem (Telesage, 2009) generated eight survey calls per day, at ous research suggests that autobiographical and future think- quasi-random times, within each participant’s 12-h window of ing deficits are related to an over-general memory bias (e.g., availability. Participants responded to survey items using the Dickson & Bates 2005; Kremers, Spinhoven, Van Der Does, keypad of their phones. & van Dyck, 2006; MacLeod, Rose, & Williams, 1993; Wil- The survey began by asking participants if they were think- liams et al., 1996); however, it remains unclear whether this ing about the past, present, or future. The “present” option effect is driven by negative mood or perseverative thinking, was endorsed if participants were not immediately concerned as negative mood and rumination are highly correlated con- with an aspect of their past or future. The “past” and “future” structs (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). To address this issue, we options branched to items regarding vividness (“How vivid also measured depressive symptoms. is this thought?”), positive valence (“How positive is this We hypothesized that people high in brooding rumina- thought?”), and temporal distance (e.g., if future, then “Is this tion would not only report more past-oriented thought in thought about the near or distant future?”). daily life but also show reductions in the vividness of future- A seven-point scale was used for the vividness and valence oriented thought—a phenomenological dimension related to items (1 = not at all, 7 = very much), and a dichotomous scale the specificity with which people recall the past and imag- was used for the temporal distance item (1 = recent past, ine the future (Sheldon & El-Asmar, 2017). Conversely, we 2 = distant past; 1 = near future, 2 = distant future). The three expected that people high in optimism would report more phenomenological items (temporal distance, positive valence, vivid future-related thoughts in daily life. Previous labora- and vividness) afforded a detailed assessment of when mental tory research suggests that optimism is characterized by the episodes had occurred in time (temporal distance), as well as ability to generate vivid and positive mental imagery for the extent to which episodes were characterized by positive the future (Blackwell et al., 2013). It is unknown, however, emotion (valence) and contextually rich (vividness) mental whether people high in optimism spontaneously generate imagery. positive and vivid future-oriented thoughts outside of the lab, and whether they show a similar profile for past-ori- Lab questionnaires ented thoughts. The present research thus offers a first look at the extent to which optimists experience a bias towards We administered the abbreviated Ruminative Response Scale the future in everyday life. We hypothesized that people high (RRS), a 10-item measure of cognitive style that excludes in optimism would report more frequent episodes of future- items confounded with other depressive symptoms (Treynor, oriented thought, and that such episodes would be character- Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003). The short RSS has two ized by increased vividness and positive valence. subscales—brooding and self-reflection—each measured with five items. The brooding subscale assesses how often people engage in negative self-focused thoughts (e.g., “I think about a Method recent situation, wishing it had gone better”; “Why do I always react this way?”), whereas the reflection subscale assesses Participants neutral pondering about the self (e.g., “I write down what I am thinking and analyze it”; “go someplace alone to think Seventy-nine students from the University of North Carolina about your feelings”). Participants also completed the depres- at Greensboro participated in the study (mean age = 20.06, sion subscale of the anxiety stress scales (DASS; SD = 2.54; 61 women, 18 men). Students received credit Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995), a seven-item scale that assesses toward a course option or up to $20 for their participation. depressive symptoms (e.g., dysphoria; “I felt downhearted and Partial compensation was awarded during the first phase blue”). Finally, we administered the revised life orientation test of the study and, if students completed at least 60% of the (LOT-R; Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994), a 10-item scale phone surveys, they were again compensated. of dispositional optimism (e.g., “I’m always optimistic about my future”). Procedure

The first phase of the study involved a tutorial of the phone survey system and a series of computerized questionnaires. Students filled out consent forms, and then provided their cell phone numbers and a convenient 12-h period of time to

1 3 808 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816

Results did not reach conventional statistical significance (t = 1.87, p = 0.06). Thus, people spent most of their days engaged Model specification in present-oriented or atemporal thought, but they also spent a considerable amount of time engaged in past- and The data were analyzed using multilevel structural equa- future-oriented thought. tion modeling (SEM) with maximum likelihood estimation For past-oriented thought, participants reported a rela- in Mplus 7. We modeled items from each questionnaire as tively low degree of positive valence (M = 2.19; SD = 1.81; indicators of latent variables; for example, “brooding” was using a seven-point scale). The vividness of past-oriented specified as a latent variable, indicated by the five items of thought was moderate (M = 3.07; SD = 2.34), suggesting an its subscale. The same procedure was applied to the other overall neutral level of valence and imagery of past-oriented measures (self-reflection, depression, and optimism). For imagery within the sample. Regarding temporal distance, model identification, the latent variables’ variances were participants reported thinking more often about events in the fixed to one (Kline, 2011). recent past (77%) than in the distant past (23%). A multilevel approach allowed us to accommodate the For future-oriented thought, participants also reported a hierarchical structure of the nested survey data. Responses moderate degree of positive valence (M = 3.05; SD = 2.28), to the phone surveys were group-mean centered at the which was significantly greater than the mean level of past- within-person level (i.e., Level 1), and responses to the lab oriented thought reported above (i.e., M = 2.19; t = 5.25, questionnaires were grand-mean centered at the between- p < .001). Interestingly, the vividness of future-oriented person level (i.e., Level 2; Enders & Tofighi, 2007). We imagery was quite high (M = 4.69; SD = 2.05) and signifi- report logistic regression coefficients for analyses with cat- cantly greater than the mean level of vividness for past-ori- egorical variables (e.g., the likelihood of thinking about ented thought (i.e., M = 3.07; t = 6.17, p < 0.001). For tempo- the past, present, or future) and unstandardized regression ral distance, participants reported thinking more often about coefficients for continuous variables (e.g., the vividness of events in the near future (78%) than in the distant future future-oriented thoughts). (22%). Thus, like past-oriented thoughts, future-oriented thoughts were mostly restricted to events close in time, sug- gesting a relatively narrow window of mental time travel in Descriptive statistics daily life.

Participants completed a total of 2483 phone surveys. The Rumination and past-oriented thought survey completion rate (57%) was comparable to other cellphone-based experience sampling studies (e.g., Beaty Our first multilevel analysis examined the effects of brooding et al., 2013; Burgin et al., 2013), and consistent with the and self-reflection on the likelihood of thinking about the required level of compliance for receiving additional com- past in daily life. Although they were strongly correlated pensation at the end of the study (i.e., 60%). Before report- (r = 0.75), brooding, but not self-reflection, significantly ing the individual differences analyses, we first explore predicted the likelihood of thinking about the past in daily baseline levels of temporal orientation within the entire life (b = − 0.68, p = 0.04): consistent with our hypothesis, sample. This descriptive analysis provides a closer look at people high in brooding were more likely to report thinking how often people think about the past, present, and future about the past when probed by the survey system at random in daily life, in addition to the extent to which past- and times of the day. future-oriented thoughts are characterized by vivid mental We then added the depression variable to a model with imagery, positive emotional content, and greater temporal brooding and self-reflection predicting the likelihood of distance. thinking about the past (see Fig. 1; Table 1). Brooding Overall, the sample was more likely to be thinking remained a robust predictor of past-oriented thought (brood- about the present moment when called at random by the ing b = − 0.89, p = 0.02), suggesting that, when controlling phone survey system: participants reported thinking about for depressed mood, a perseverative cognitive style is related the present more than two-thirds of their waking hours to an increased frequency of past-oriented thoughts. (68%). It is important to note that the “present” category Next, we examined the extent to which rumination and likely includes both clearly defined present thoughts and depressed mood were related to the phenomenology of past- a range of other thoughts with no discernable temporal oriented thoughts. Vividness, valence, and temporal distance orientation. Future-oriented thought was reported on were modeled as multivariate outcomes, predicted by brood- 19% of the survey calls, and past-oriented thought was ing and self-reflection. Brooding negatively predicted the reported on 13% of the calls—a nominal difference that vividness of past-oriented thought (b = − 0.90, p = 0.01)— as levels of brooding increased, people recalled less vivid

1 3 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 809

We then examined the effects of rumination and depressed mood on the likelihood of thinking about the pre- sent. Brooding, self-reflection, and depression were modeled as predictors of the likelihood of thinking about the present. No significant effects emerged, suggesting that rumination and depressed mood were unrelated to a present-oriented focus in daily life.

Rumination and future-oriented thought

Our next set of analyses examined the frequency and phe- nomenology of future thinking as a function of brooding and self-reflection. A model with brooding, self-reflection, and depressed mood predicting the likelihood of engaging in future thought revealed no significant effects (ps > 0.30), suggesting that subclinical levels of rumination and depressed mood are largely unrelated to how often people Fig. 1 The probability of thinking about the past as a function of imagine the future in daily life. n x brooding. Note: = 79. The brooding variable on the -axis was Next, brooding and self-reflection were modeled as pre- derived from summing and z-transforming the five items of the brooding subscale. The full model included brooding, self-reflection, dictors of the vividness, valence, and temporal distance of and depression, so the above graph represents the probability of future-oriented thoughts. Brooding negatively predicted thinking about the past as a function of brooding, adjusted for levels imagery (b = − 0.53, p = 0.02) and it showed a marginal z of self-reflection and depressed mood. -transformed observed vari- effect on valence (b = − 0.54, p = 0.13); no effects of tempo- ables were used instead of latent variables for the sake of illustration ral distance emerged. Thus, people high in brooding rumina- tion generated less vivid mental images of the future. Table 1 Unstandardized effects Model Past: vivid Past: positive Past: distance of the RRS, DASS-D, and LOT-R on past-oriented thought b p b p b p

1. RRS Brooding − 0.90 0.01 − 0.09 0.67 − 0.22 0.48 Self-reflection 0.49 0.18 0.16 0.50 0.48 0.16 2. DASS-D DASS-D − 0.15 0.53 0.04 0.77 0.31 0.16 3. RRS and DASS-D Brooding − 0.96 0.01 − 0.09 0.69 − 0.25 0.43 Self-reflection 0.38 0.35 0.14 0.59 0.33 0.38 DASD-D 0.23 0.53 0.01 0.94 0.24 0.46 4. LOT-R LOT-R 0.27 0.29 − 0.13 0.44 − 0.11 0.67

n = 79 RRS Ruminative Response scale, DASS-D Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-Depression, LOT-R life orien- tation test-revised thoughts about their personal pasts. Importantly, the effect of We then assessed the role of depressed mood in future- brooding remained significant when the depression variable oriented thought. Because rumination is a common symp- was added to the model (see Table 1); brooding, self-reflec- tom of depression (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991) and future tion, and depression were unrelated to valence and temporal thinking deficits have been related to dysphoria in labora- distance. Taken together, although people high in brooding tory studies (e.g., Holmes, Lang, Moulds, & Steele, 2008; rumination were more likely to be thinking about the past in Williams et al., 1996), we could not yet rule out an influ- daily life, their experiences for past-oriented episodes were ence of depressed mood in future thought. Modeled as the less vivid.

1 3 810 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816

Table 2 Unstandardized effects Model Future: vivid Future: positive Future: distance of the RRS, DASS-D, and LOT-R on future-oriented b p b p b p thought 1. RRS Brooding − 0.53 0.02 − 0.32 0.18 0.05 0.89 Self-reflection 0.33 0.17 0.09 0.70 0.12 0.75 2. DASSD DASSD − 0.11 0.53 − 0.41 0.02 − 0.02 0.87 3. RRS and DASSD Brooding − 0.70 0.03 − 0.22 0.49 0.11 0.69 Self-reflection 0.37 0.26 0.27 0.40 0.17 0.58 DASSD 0.10 0.71 − 0.44 0.13 − 0.20 0.43 4. LOT-R LOT-R 0.45 0.01 0.47 0.01 0.12 0.49

n = 79 RRS Ruminative Response Scale, DASSD Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-Depression, LOT-R life orienta- tion test-revised lone predictor of imagery vividness and valence, depres- Optimism and future-oriented thought sive symptoms negatively predicted valence (b = − 0.41, p = 0.02), but not vividness or temporal distance (see We then examined whether optimists were more likely to Table 2). In the full model, however, with brooding, self- report thinking about the future in daily life. The effect of reflection, and depressive symptoms predicting vividness, optimism on the likelihood of engaging in future-oriented valence, and temporal distance, only brooding negatively thought, however, was not significant (p = 0.60), indicating predicted vividness (b = − 0.70, p = 0.03); no effects of that optimism is not characterized by thinking more or less depressive symptoms emerged.1 about the future. Finally, we examined the phenomenology of future- Optimism and past-oriented thought oriented thoughts as a function of optimism. Vividness, valence, and temporal orientation were modeled as multi- We next tested whether optimism, a trait associated with a variate outcomes, predicted by the latent optimism variable. positive future outlook, was related to enhanced imagery for Optimism predicted both vividness (b = 0.55, p = 0.01) and past and future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Optimism was valence (b = 0.54, p = 0.01), but not temporal distance (see modeled as a predictor of the likelihood of engaging in past- Table 2). Thus, although optimists did not spend more time oriented thought. The effect of optimism was not significant thinking about the future, their future-oriented thoughts were (p = 0.58), suggesting that people high in optimism were not more likely to contain vivid and positive mental imagery. more likely to be thinking about the past in daily life. Next, we explored whether optimism was related to the phenom- enology of past-oriented thoughts. Vividness, valence, and Discussion temporal distance of past-oriented thoughts were modeled as multivariate outcomes. Optimism showed nonsignificant The present research suggests that temporal and affective effects on all dependent variables (see Table 1). Thus, people biases in cognitive style influence mental time travel in eve- high in optimism were not more likely to be thinking of the ryday life. Using an experience sampling design, we found past, nor were their past-oriented thoughts more likely to that brooding rumination (a negative past-oriented bias) be characterized by greater vividness and positive valence. and optimism (a positive future-oriented bias) predicted a distinct pattern of decreases and increases in past- and future-oriented mental imagery. Not only did people high in brooding rumination report less vivid imagery for the past and future, they also spent more time dwelling in past-ori-

1 ented thought. In contrast, people high in optimism reported We also assessed the role of . Past research suggests that enhanced imagery for the future, but they did not spend more women ruminate more than men (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991), and our sample was approximately two-thirds female; however, gender did not or less time in the past or the future. Taken together, these decrease the effect of brooding on vividness. results suggest that individual differences in cognitive style

1 3 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 811 influence how people think about the past and future in daily people high in brooding reported thinking about the past life. more often, their thoughts were actually characterized by Overall, participants reported more present or atemporal less mental imagery for both the past and the future. thoughts (68%) than past (13%) and future (19%) thoughts. This observation points to a possible dissociation between It is important to note that the prevalence of future-oriented the frequency and phenomenology of mental imagery in thought reported in the current study was greater than pre- people high in brooding rumination. One explanation for vious experience sampling studies on temporal orientation this pattern of effects is that brooders’ thought content may and mental imagery (e.g., 12%; Klinger & Cox, 1987), comprise verbal rather than visual–spatial imagery; for potentially due to methodological differences across stud- example, they may experience more frequent episodes of ies, such as thought sampling procedure. Regarding tem- perseverative inner speech related to negative past events. poral distance, participants reported thinking more about Future work should further explore the past-oriented thought events in the recent past and near future, which suggests a content associated with high levels of brooding rumination, relatively narrow window of mental time travel in daily life. and determine the extent to which this cognitive style is Moreover, the phenomenological quality of past and future characterized by deficits in verbal or visual–spatial imagery thoughts was notably different across temporal dimensions: in daily life. participants reported a greater degree of positive valence for Regarding changes in future thinking reported by people future-oriented thoughts, pointing to an enhanced conscious high in brooding, one plausible explanation is that a perse- experience of the future in daily life. verative, past-oriented thinking style disrupts future thinking Notably, we found that vividness ratings were higher via cognitive inflexibility. In other words, a fixation on past for future- than past-oriented thoughts. Previous work on experiences may prevent attention from being reoriented episodic memory and future simulation has reported higher toward the future. According to the impaired disengage- vividness ratings for memory compared to simulation (e.g., ment hypothesis (Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan, & De D’Argembeau & van der Linden, 2004, 2006). However, Raedt, 2011), a chronic perseverative focus on negative self- because this study did not assess thought content, we cannot referential events can lead to an overall difficulty in with- know whether thought reports about the past were related drawing attention from negative thoughts. In this context, it to actual episodic memories, simulations of imagined past could be that brooders become “stuck” in a recursive loop events (e.g., counterfactual simulations; Schacter, Benoit, De of past-oriented thought, thereby preventing attention from Brigard, & Szpunar, 2015), or other forms of self-generated disengaging from the past and reorienting toward the future. thought. Moreover, our thought probes did not assess inten- On the other hand, brooding was not associated with a global tionality, and past work has shown that vividness can vary as reduction of future-oriented thought in daily (the effect a function of intentionality, with involuntary past and future was specific to vividness) so the extent to which future- thoughts rated as similarly vivid (Berntsen & Jacobsen, thinking deficits in rumination are a function of impaired 2008). Subsequent work should consider thought content past engagement requires further investigation. Moreover, and intentionality to determine how these variables relate brooding rumination is associated with an abstract cogni- to the vividness and valence of past and future thoughts in tive style (Watkins, 2008)—or a tendency to think about the daily life. past and future experiences with minimal concrete detail— which may in part explain the link between rumination and Mental time travel and cognitive style decreased vividness of past and future thoughts in the cur- rent study. Our results extend previous research on deficits in past- and However, if brooders indeed experience difficulty disen- future-oriented thinking. Specifically, we found that brood- gaging from past experience, they may rely more on recast- ing rumination was related to reductions in past- and future- ing those experiences, rather than constructing novel ones, oriented mental imagery, even when controlling for levels of when imagining possible future events, which might further depressed mood. This finding builds on past work by sug- impact the novelty of future-oriented thought. People high gesting that future thinking decreases stem from a persevera- in brooding may, therefore, oversample from past experi- tive, past-oriented cognitive style (i.e., rumination), rather ence when imagining the future because their attention is than a depressed mood per se. Although depressed par- chronically biased towards past-oriented thought. Indeed, ticipants reported less positively valenced future thoughts, recasting past experience when imagining novel future epi- this effect was no longer significant once levels of rumina- sodes is a symptom of cognitive impairment in neuropsy- tion were taken into account. Furthermore, people high in chological disorders such as semantic dementia (Irish & brooding did not report significantly less positive valence Piguet, 2013). An interesting question for future work to in their thoughts about the future. They were, however, less consider is whether impaired cognitive flexibility affects the likely to report vivid imagery for the future. Thus, although novelty of future-oriented thought, or the extent to which

1 3 812 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 future thought content deviates from past experience. In this life. In hindsight, however, it may be reasonable to assume context, the ability to construct novel future thoughts can be that optimists would not report spending more time in the construed as a type of creative thought process. future. Optimism is a positive psychological trait charac- Recent behavioral and neuroimaging research points to terized by the tendency to expect positive future outcomes a tight coupling in the cognitive and neural mechanisms (Scheier et al. 1994)—not necessarily a tendency to perse- involved in episodic future thinking and creative cognition verate on, or think more often about, such outcomes. Indeed, (Addis, Pan, Musicaro, & Schacter, 2016; Beaty & Schacter a high degree of future-oriented thought may be indicative 2018; Roberts et al., 2017; Schacter & Madore, 2016). of psychological disorder (e.g., worry or anxiety). Unlike Research on creative thought suggests that executive control the past-oriented focus of brooders, then, optimists may contributes to people’s ability to generate novel, useful, and engage in a more adaptive cognitive style that allows atten- uncommon ideas (Beaty, Benedek, Silvia, & Schacter, 2016; tion to freely shift among past, present, and future-oriented Gilhooly, Fioratou, Anthony, & Wynn, 2007). The ability thoughts in daily life. to freely direct attention thus may influence how much It is important to note that worry is strongly correlated people tend to recast past experiences when constructing with rumination in clinical populations: people who tend to novel thoughts about the future. Subsequent research should ruminate about past events also tend to worry about future explore the extent to which cognitive flexibility and creative events, pointing to a general tendency toward repetitive thought contribute to the novelty of episodic future thinking. thinking in people with clinical conditions (Watkins, 2008). Another fruitful direction for future research is to extend Because we did not assess worry in the current study, the the existing literature on individual differences in mental extent to which the observed effects of rumination on past- time travel, including related research on time perspective, and future-oriented thought in daily life reflects a global bias which has established that people vary in their biases toward to engage in negative, repetitive thought is unclear. Future past- and future-oriented thinking (Zimbardo & Boyd, work might, therefore, assess common and unique effects of 2015). For example, D’Argembeau et al. (2010) reported rumination and worry on past- and future-oriented thought a positive correlation between future orientation and the in daily life using a bi-factor approach, which can assess amount sensory details reported when participants imagined global or high-order effects of a latent construct (e.g., nega- future events; the authors also found that self-consciousness tive/repetitive thinking) and specific or lower-order effects (a factor linked to self-reflection in rumination) correlated indicating that construct (e.g., rumination and worry). positively with subjective reports of experiencing future- Optimism is also associated with an ability to cope with oriented events. An interesting direction for future research stressful life events (Brissette, Scheier, & Carver, 2002). would be to examine whether temporally relevant cogni- To what extent do optimists rely on their ability to vividly tive styles such as rumination and optimism predict aspects imagine the future in the face of stressors in daily life? One of past- and future-oriented thought, beyond what can be possibility is that people high in optimism are successful explained by variation in time perspective and other estab- in managing stressful events because they can more easily lished biases in cognitive style. disengage from the present and mentally project positive Another goal of the present work was to determine and vivid future experiences. This capacity may provide an whether optimism is related to enhanced future thinking in adaptive, self-regulatory buffer, whereby further negative daily life. We found that people high in optimism reported effect is prevented by the ability to disengage from adverse an active imagination for the future. To our knowledge, physical and psychological conditions and mentally simu- this study is the first to provide evidence that optimism is late positive future experiences. Subsequent research should characterized by greater vividness and positive valence for explore whether optimists show enhanced future-oriented future-oriented thought in everyday life, therefore, validat- mental imagery when confronted with stressful events in ing a widely used self-report measure of optimism (i.e., the daily life, and determine the extent to which enhanced future LOT-R; Scheier et al., 1994). Past research has shown that thinking aids in self-regulation. optimists report more vivid mental imagery than pessimists Our results may also have implications for debates about when imagining plausible future episodes (Blackwell et al., the role of temporal factors in studies of remembering the 2013). Our study extends this work by demonstrating that past and imagining the future. As discussed by Schacter optimists report enhanced vividness ratings for future-ori- et al. (2012; see also Addis, Pan, Vu, Laiser, and Schacter, ented thoughts, but not for past. Regarding temporal dis- 2009), references to “past events” and “future events” in tance, people high in optimism were not more likely to be these studies are often confounded with the distinction thinking about the near or distant future, which suggests that between “remembering” and “imagining”. Remembered their thought content showed variability across time. events, of course, necessarily refer to the past. We also found that, contrary to our hypothesis, optimists However, cognitive or neural characteristics attributed did not show a bias toward future-oriented thought in daily to “future events” could potentially also be attributed to

1 3 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 813

“imagined events”, regardless of whether such events refer On average, people responded to about 57% of the surveys to the future, the past, or the present. For example, studies over the course of the week. Although consistent with past of atemporal scene construction (e.g., Hassabis, Kumaran, work using IVR systems (Burgin et al., 2013; 58%), other & Maguire, 2007) reveal many of the same cognitive and experience-sampling methods that do not require partici- neural features documented for remembered past and imag- pants to respond to phone calls (e.g., personal digital assis- ined future experiences (for review, see Mullally & Maguire, tants, PDA; app-based methods) may be preferred in future 2014), even though no mental time travel is involved. Thus, studies. Indeed, Burgin and colleagues found that survey it is not always clear whether studies of the relation between completion rates were higher in a PDA condition compared remembering the past and imagining the future specifically to an IVR condition, although the daily-life ratings provided address the relation between past and future, or whether in this study were comparable (Burgin et al., 2013). We sug- they address the relation between memory and imagination, gest future daily-life experience-sampling studies consider regardless of the involvement of mental time travel. employing one of the many app-based methods currently With respect to our study, we cannot know for certain available (e.g., MetricWire; http://www.metri cwire .com) whether the differences documented here between brooding which may be more conducive to higher survey completion ruminators and optimists are entirely accounted for by dif- rates. ferences in temporal orientation (i.e., past vs. future); it is conceivable that the effects we observed are attributable to differences between memory (past) and imagination (future) that would not be observed if participants were imagining Summary and future directions (as opposed to remembering) past events. Because we did not obtain the content of the past and future events reported The present research examined the role of cognitive style by participants, we do not know whether past events reported in everyday mental time travel, which is more likely to be by participants were imagined, as opposed to remembered. spontaneous than the kind of cue-elicited mental time travel This issue may merit exploration in future research. None- typically studied in the lab. We found that although peo- theless, given that the individual difference variable we ple high in brooding rumination spent more time engaged examined was defined in terms of temporal orientation, we in past-oriented thought, they experienced less vivid men- think that our data most likely do reveal differences that are tal imagery for their past- and future-oriented thoughts. In specifically related to mental time travel. addition, we found that people high in optimism were not Another important caveat of the study is that the thought more or less likely to be thinking about the past or future, probes did not distinguish between spontaneous and delib- but they reported enhanced mental imagery and positive erate cognition. This distinction is particularly relevant for valence for future-oriented thoughts. Our results suggest the topic of the special issue in Psychology Research, and that certain temporal biases in cognitive style affect the it has been increasingly emphasized in the mind-wandering frequency and phenomenology of past- and future-oriented literature. Although some work has examined intentional- cognition. Future work could extend this correlational study ity and spontaneity of past- and future-oriented thoughts with experimental manipulations that induce the temporal in a laboratory context (Seli, Ralph, Konishi, Smilek, & direction of spontaneous cognition (cf., Cole, Staugaard, & Schacter, 2017), to our knowledge, no research has explored Bernsten, 2016). Moreover, because we did not ask people to these dimensions in daily life. Because mind-wandering var- report what they were thinking about, our conclusions con- ies across lab and daily-life contexts (Kane et al., 2017), an cerning the content of such thoughts are necessarily limited. interesting direction for future research would be to compare Subsequent research should explore additional subjective temporality and spontaneity in lab and life. In our study, we and objective criteria by exploring past- and future-oriented suspect that many thought probes captured task-unrelated cognition in individuals with cognitive, affective, and tem- or daydreams, while others may have captured poral biases, and further examine future-thinking deficits in some deliberate aspects of planning. Future work could clinical populations. assess whether intentionality and temporality interact with cognitive style: it is possible that rumination is associated Research funding P.S. was supported by a Social Science and Humani- ties Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) Banting Post-Doctoral Fel- with more spontaneous and negative past-oriented thought, lowship. D.L.S received research Grants from the National Institute of whereas optimism is related to more deliberate future-ori- R01 MH060941. ented thought. We encourage researchers to examine tempo- rality and spontaneity in the context of individual differences Compliance with ethical standards in cognitive style. A final methodological limitation of the current work Ethical approval All procedures performed in studies involving human worth noting concerns the response rate of daily-life surveys. participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the insti-

1 3 814 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 tutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki phones for experience sampling research. Social Science Com- Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. puter Review, 31, 244–251. Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews- Informed consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a participants included in the study. dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17, 718–731. Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S. R., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involun- tary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 44, 376–389. D’Argembeau, A., & Mathy, A. (2011). Tracking the construction of References episodic future thoughts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 258–271. Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Musicaro, R., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Diver- D’Argembeau, A., Ortoleva, C., Jumentier, S., & Van der Linden, M. gent thinking and constructing episodic simulations. Memory, 24, (2010). Component processes underlying future thinking. Memory 89–97. & Cognition, 38, 809–819. Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M., Laiser, N., & Schacter, D. L. (2009). D’Argembeau, A., Rafford, S., & Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remem- Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: Dis- bering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. Journal tinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imaging and of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 247–251. remembering. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2222–2238. D’Argembeau, A., Renauld, O., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Fre- Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering quency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural in daily life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 96–103. substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsy- D’Argembeau, A., Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Collette, F., Van der chologia, 45, 1363–1377. Linden, M., Feyers, D., et al. (2010). The neural basis of personal Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related goal processing when envisioning future events. Journal of Cogni- changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychologi- tive Neuroscience, 22, 1701–1713. cal Science, 19, 33–41. D’Argembeau, A., & van der Linden, M. (2004). Phenomenal char- Baird, B., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the future: acteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wander- and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal ing. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1604–1611. distance. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 844–858. Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Crea- D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual differences tive cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid Sciences, 20, 87–95. visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness Beaty, R. E., Burgin, C. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Kwapil, T. R., Hodges, and Cognition, 15, 342–350. D. A., & Silvia, P. J. (2013). Music to the inner ears: Exploring De Brigard, F., Szpunar, K. K., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Coming to individual differences in musical imagery. Consciousness and grips with the past: Effect of repeated simulation on the perceived Cognition, 22, 1163–1173. plausibility of episodic counterfactual thoughts. Psychological Beaty, R. E., & Schacter, D. L. (2018). Episodic memory and cognitive Science, 24, 1329–1334. control. Contributions to creative idea production. In R. Jung & O. Dickson, J. M., & Bates, G. W. (2005). Influence of repression on auto- Vartanian (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of biographical memories and expectations of the future. Australian creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press. Journal of Psychology, 57, 20–27. Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, Psychological Methods, 12, 121–138. and the Arts, 6, 309–319. Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Diver- Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2013). Metaphorically speaking: Cognitive gent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating abilities and the production of figurative language. Memory and novel uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, Cognition, 41, 255–267. 98, 611–625. Benoit, R. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the core network Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Using imagina- supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation tion to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. Journal likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450–457. of Neuroscience, 27, 14365–14374. Berntsen, D., & Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (spontaneous) Hill, P. F., & Emery, L. J. (2013). Episodic future thought: Contri- mental time travel into the past and future. Consciousness and butions from working memory. Consciousness & Cognition, 22, Cognition, 17, 1093–1104. 677–683. Blackwell, S. E., Rius-Ottenheim, N., Schulte-van Maaren, Y. W. M., Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., Moulds, M. L., & Steele, A. M. (2008). Carlier, I. V. E., Middelkoop, V. C., Zitman, F. G., et al. (2013). Prospective and positive mental imagery deficits in dysphoria. Optimism and mental imagery: A possible cognitive marker to Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(8), 976–981. promote well-being? Psychiatry Research, 206, 56–61. Irish, M., & Piguet, O. (2013). The pivotal role of semantic memory Brissette, I., Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (2002). The role of opti- in remembering the past and imagining the future. Frontiers in mism in social network development, coping, and psychological Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 27. adjustment during a life transition. Journal of Personality and Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., Social Psychology, 82, 102–111. & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: Brown, A. D., Addis, D. R., Romano, T. A., Marmar, C. R., Bryant, An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive R. A., Hirst, W., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). Episodic and semantic control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18, 614–621. components of autobiographical memories and imagined future Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation events in post-traumatic stress disorder. Memory, 22, 595–604. modeling (3rd edn.). New York: Guilford. Burgin, C. J., Silvia, P. J., Eddington, K. M., & Kwapil, T. R. (2013). Klinger, E., & Cox, W. M. (1987). Dimensions of thought flow in eve- Palm or cell? Comparing personal digital assistants and cell ryday life. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 7, 105–128.

1 3 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816 815

Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an and self-esteem): a re- of the Life Orientation Test. unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063–1078. Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Seli, P., Kane, M., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D., Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science Schooler, J., & Smilek, D. (2018). Mind-wandering as a natural perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical kind: a family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, psychology review, 31, 138–145. 22, 479–490. Kremers, I. P., Spinhoven, P., Van Der Does, A. J., & van Dyck, R. Seli, P., Ralph, B. C., Konishi, M., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Social problem solving, autobiographical memory and (2017). What did you have in mind? Examining the content of the future specificity in outpatients with borderline personality intentional and unintentional types of mind wandering. Con- disorder. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 13, 131–137. sciousness and Cognition, 51, 149–156. Kvavilashvili, L., & Schlagman, S. (2011). Involuntary autobiographi- Seli, P., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2016). On the necessity of distin- cal memories in dysphoric mood: A laboratory study. Memory, guishing between unintentional and intentional mind wandering. 19, 331–345. Psychological Science, 27, 685–691. Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of nega- Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind- tive emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive Sci- scales (DASS) with the Beck depression and anxiety inventories. ences, 20, 605–617. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335–343. Sheldon, S., & El-Asmar, N. (2017). The cognitive tools that support MacLeod, A. K., Rose, G., & Williams, J. M. (1993). Components of mentally constructing event and scene representations. Memory, hopelessness about the future in parasuicide. Cognitive Therapy 26, 858–868. and Research, 17, 441–455. Silvia, P. J., Kwapil, T. R., Eddington, K. M., & Brown, L. H. (2013). McDermott, K. B., Wooldridge, C. L., Rice, H. J., Berg, J. J., & Szpu- Missed beeps and missing data: dispositional and situational nar, K. K. (2016). Visual perspective in remembering and episodic predictors of non-response in experience sampling research. future thought. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychol- Social Science Computer Review, 31, 471–481. ogy, 69, 243–253. Song, X., & Wang, X. (2012). Mind wandering in Chinese daily McNally, R. J., Litz, B. T., Prassas, A., Shin, L. M., & Weathers, F. lives—an experience sampling study. PloS One, 7, e44423. W. (1994). Emotional priming of autobiographical memory in Spronken, M., Holland, R. W., Figner, B., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2016). post-traumatic stress disorder. Cognition & Emotion, 8, 351–367. Temporal focus, temporal distance, and mind-wandering Miloyan, B., Bulley, A., & Suddendorf, T. (2016). Episodic foresight valence: results from an experience sampling and an experi- and anxiety: Proximate and ultimate perspectives. British Journal mental study. Consciousness and Cognition, 41, 104–118. of Clinical Psychology, 55, 4–22. Stawarczyk, D., Cassol, H., & D’Argembeau, A. (2013). Phenom- Mullally, S. L., & Maguire, E. A. (2014). Memory, imagination, and enology of future-oriented mind-wandering episodes. Frontiers predicting the future: A. common brain mechanism? The Neuro- in Psychology, 4, 425. scientist, 20, 220–234. Stawarczyk, D., & D’Argembeau, A. (2015). Neural correlates of Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects personal goal processing during episodic future thinking and on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psy- mind-wandering: An ALE meta-analysis. Human Brain Map- chology, 100, 569–582. ping, 36, 2928–2947. Poerio, G. L., Totterdell, P., & Miles, E. (2013). Mind-wandering and Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic future thought: an emerging con- negative mood: Does one thing really lead to another? Conscious- cept. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 142–162. ness and Cognition, 22, 1412–1421. Szpunar, K. K., & McDermott, K. B. (2008). Episodic future thought Roberts, R. P., Wiebels, K., Sumner, R. L., van Mulukom, V., Grady, and its relation to remembering: evidence from ratings of sub- C. L., Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2017). An fMRI investiga- jective experience. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 330–334. tion of the relationship between future imagination and cognitive Szpunar, K. K., Watson, J. M., & McDermott, K. B. (2007). Neural flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 95, 156–172. substrates of envisioning the future. Proceedings of the National Schacter, D. L. (2012). Adaptive constructive processes and the future Academy of Sciences, 104, 642–647. of memory. American Psychologist, 67, 603–613. Telesage. (2009). SmartQ (version 5.2.48) [computer software]. Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). On the constructive simula- Chapel Hill: Telesage. tion of past and future events. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumina- 30, 331–332. tion reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remember- and Research, 27, 247–259. ing the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychol- Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 657–661. ogy, 26, 1–12. Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin, V. C., Spreng, R. Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral N., & Szpunar, K. K. (2012). The future of memory: remember- memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic think- ing, imagining, and the brain. Neuron, 76, 677–694. ing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 353–357. Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., De Brigard, F., & Szpunar, K. K. (2015). Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive Episodic future thinking and episodic counterfactual thinking: thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206. intersections between memory and decisions. Neurobiology of Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life Learning and Memory, 17, 114–121. events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., & Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic 806–820. future thinking: mechanisms and functions. Current Opinion in Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Herman, D., Raes, F., Behavioral Sciences, 17, 41–50. Watkins, E., & Dalgleish, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory Schacter, D. L., & Madore, K. P. (2016). Remembering the past and specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133, imagining the future: identifying and enhancing the contribution 122–148. of episodic memory. Memory Studies, 9, 245–255. Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., & Healy, H. (1996). The Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguish- specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the ing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, future. Memory & Cognition, 24, 116–125.

1 3 816 Psychological Research (2019) 83:805–816

Wu, J., Szpunar, K. K., Godovich, S., Schacter, D. L., & Hofmann, S. Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2015). Putting time in perspective: G. (2015). Episodic future thinking in generalized anxiety disor- A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. In M. Stolarski, der. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 36, 1–8. N. Fieulaine, & W. van Beek (Eds.), Time perspective theory: Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cog- Review, research and application. Cham: Springer. nitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 136–143.

1 3