Color Preferences of Frugivorous Birds in Relation to the Colors of Fleshy Fruits’

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Color Preferences of Frugivorous Birds in Relation to the Colors of Fleshy Fruits’ THE CONDOR -- 1990 A JOURNAL OF AVIAN BIOLOGY SW17 Volume 92 Number 3 The Condor92545-555 0 The CooperOrnithological Society 1990 COLOR PREFERENCES OF FRUGIVOROUS BIRDS IN RELATION TO THE COLORS OF FLESHY FRUITS’ MARY F. WILLSON,* DANIEL A. GRAFF,AND CHRISTOPHERJ. WHELAN~ Department of Ecology,Ethology, and Evolution, Universityof Illinois, SherfordVivarium, 606 E. Healey Street, Champaign, IL 61820 Abstract. Aviary experimentsdemonstrated that threeNorth Americanfmgivorous mi- grantbird speciesoften exhibit colorpreferences, when factors such as taste,nutrition, and accessibilityto food sourcesare equal. Individual birds differed in initial color preferences, transitivity, and temporal stability of color preferences.There was little tendency of these birds to favor red and black, which are the most common colors of fruits of bird-dispersed plants, and a weak tendency to reject yellow. We suggestthat avian color preferencesmay not provide strong selection favoring the evolution of the common fruit colors, and that the frequency distribution of fruit hues is best explained in other ways. Key words: Color preferences;fmgivory; fruit color; Gray Catbird; Swainsons’ Thrush; Hermit Thrush;Dumetella carolinensis: Catharus ustulatus; C. guttatus. INTRODUCTION proposed,and several factors may have contrib- Fleshy fruits consumed by avian frugivores are uted to this distribution (Willson and Whelan, often colored red or black, sometimes blue, pur- in press). To begin to assessthe importance of ple, pink, or white, but rarely orange, yellow, different factors that may help account for the green, or brown (Janson 1983, Willson et al. observed distribution of fruit colors, we examine 1989). Fruit colors are commonly considered to the simple hypothesis that the frequency distri- increase the conspicuousnessof a ripe fruit crop bution of colors reflects the color preferencesof and/or attract the birds that eat fruits and dis- avian frugivores. Under this hypothesis, one perse the enclosed seeds (Darwin 1859, Ridley would expect that red and black would be pre- 1930, van der Pijl 1982). However, there is little ferred most frequently and consistently, followed documentation of the effects of fruit colors on by blue and white, and then by yellow, green, avian foraging decisions (McPherson 1988) or and brown. on variation in color preferenceseither within or Here we examine the following questionsabout among fiugivorous bird species. the attractivenessof different colors, as measured An explanation of the frequency distribution by color preferences of some frugivorous birds of fruit colors must account for the predomi- of the deciduous forest region of eastern North nance of red and black, the lesser occurrence of America: (1) What colors (of food) are preferred blue and white, and the rarity of yellow, green, (or avoided) by fruit-eating birds? (2) How vari- and brown. A number of hypotheseshave been able are color preferences among conspecific birds? Do different bird specieshave similar col- or preferences?(3) Does a given bird make “tran- ’ ’ Received2 November 1989. Final acceptance26 sitive” (i.e., internally consistent, see Methods) February1990. choices?(4) Are color preferences stable in the * Presentaddress: Forestry Sciences Laboratory, P.O. Box 20909, Juneau,AK 99802-0909. face of exposure to other food colors? (5) Does 3Present address: The Morton Arboretum, Lisle, IL the background against which the fruits are dis- 60532. played affect color preferences? 15451 546 M. F. WILLSON, D. A. GRAFF AND C. J. WHELAN METHODS purple, red, or blue. All of these colors are ren- dered as perceived by human eyes; we do not SUBJECTS know what the birds actually see. Under the ini- We used Gray Catbirds (Dumetella carolinensis), tial hypothesis, the expected outcome would be Swainson’s Thrushes (Catharus ustulatus), and a preference for red and black, followed by blue, Hermit Thrushes (C. guttatus) for these experi- and then yellow. ments. The thrushes were entirely passagemi- Because we were interested in whether color grants, but catbirds probably included both mi- preferences are transitive (e.g., if red > black, grants and summer residents. The birds were and black > blue, then red > blue, see Moer- captured locally in mist nets in the fall of 1988 mond and Denslow 1983) we used a pairwise and housed individually in six aviaries (2 x 2 design, in which the direction of preference for x 2.5 m) at the Shelford Vivarium, University each pair of colors was determined indepen- of Illinois. Birds were held lessthan 3 weeks and dently, rather than a four-way “cafeteria” design. were releasedat the end of the experiment; three The order of the pairwise comparisons was de- birds escaped during the trials. The number of termined from a random-number table, with the birds tested was constrained by the number of constraint that alternating pairwise comparisons aviaries available, the need to release birds be- on the same day used all four colors (e.g., red vs. fore their local seasonof migration was over, and blue, alternating with black vs. yellow). The first the number escaping. Captive birds were fed comparison alternated with the second, until a chiefly on a banana-based maintenance diet de- predetermined number of replicates (see below) signed for fruit-eating birds (Denslow et al. 1987) was complete. Likewise, the third comparison but augmented with eight teaspoons of glucose alternated with the fourth, and the fifth alter- (per 2 l), supplemented with mealworms (Te- nated with the sixth. nebrio molitor), wax worms (Galleria mellonel- Because catbirds reacted quite differently in la), and real fruit, of various speciesand colors, the aviaries from both thrush species,we used collectedlocally. All birds were given several days slightly different experimental protocols for the to become accustomed to aviary conditions be- different species.Catbirds adjusted quickly to the fore testing..Although some of the test birds may aviaries and readily foraged in our presence. In have hatched in 1988, all were fully fledged and a given foraging trial, two cubes (of two colors) capable of foraging independently. Two cohorts were placed in each of four separatepetri dishes of catbirds were used: Cohort 1 (birds l-6), 8- (30 mm diameter) arrayed along a wooden perch. 12 September; Cohort 2 (birds 7-12), 28 Sep- Each bird was allowed to feed from this display tember-l 1 October. Swainson’s Thrushes (n = until four of the eight cubes had been eaten (usu- 4) were tested on 19-23 September, and Hermit ally < 15 min). For each catbird, there were four Thrushes (n = 6) on 16 October-2 November. replicates of each trial; we did not attempt to All birds used in these experiments ate well in analyze any possiblepatterns of variation among captivity. replicates. Because equal numbers of cubes of each color in each comparison were available, EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE we defined no color preference as no statistical Initial color preferences.Experiments consisted difference (determined with the binomial exact of paired choices of maintenance diet (prepared test) in the number of cubes of each color taken. in 4-mm cubes) treated with commercial food Most experiments were conducted between coloring. Colors used were red, blue, yellow, and 07:OOand 14:O0. “black.” An approximate indication of the colors In contrast to the catbirds, neither thrush achieved is given by comparison to Smithe’s species would eat in our presence. For these (1975) color standards: red #l 10, blue #68, yel- species,therefore, we weighed out a known quan- low #18, and black #89. These colors were not tity of dietary cubes in 1O-cm petri dishes (each intended to mimic those of any particular wild color in a separate dish), which were placed in fruits, but they fall within the natural range of the middle of the aviary floors. Placement of fruit colors (to our eyes). Black was produced by dishes with different colors of food was alter- a mixture of roughly equal parts of red and blue, nated to avoid positional bias. We then allowed producing an extremely dark purple; many nat- the birds to feed until roughly half of the total urally black fruits also are, in fact, very dark food was eaten (usually <2 hr) and weighed the FRUIT COLOR PREFERENCES IN FRUGIVORES 547 remainder. The difference was the amount eaten against a background of green foliage on the par- (a small weight loss, <5%, due to evaporation ent plant. We placed each pair of dietary cubes did not affect the analysis). Experiments were on a spray of green artificial foliage attached to conducted between 07:OOand 17:OO.There were the perch. A bird on the perch could reach any three replicatesofeach pairwise comparison, and of the pairs of cubes with two or three hops. the data were analyzed by two-way (bird, color) Results of these trials were compared with those ANOVA (seeSokal and Rohlf 198 1, p. 344-348) described earlier, in which the cubes were pre- and t-tests. The t-tests were done separately for sented in petri dishes. Trials that used different each bird and each pairwise color combination backgroundswere alternated until four replicates (see Appendix 2). Although there was a direc- were completed. A significant shift of preference tional prediction for most pairs of colors (e.g., was indicated by direct comparison of the pref- red > blue), we used a conservative two-tailed erences exhibited on the two different back- test to establish whether there was a significant grounds, using x2 (P < 0.05). difference in the amount eaten. The observed direction of color preference was then deter- RESULTS mined by inspection. This procedure allowed the identification of preferences that were opposite INITIAL COLOR PREFERENCES in direction to the prediction. Catbirds. The evidence of a general preference Stability of colorpreferences. Because most in- for red and black is not strong (Table 1). Of all dividual birds of all three species exhibited 62 trials, 40 (65%) indicated no significant pref- marked preferences for certain colors, we also erence between paired colors.
Recommended publications
  • Pale Intrusions Into Blue: the Development of a Color Hannah Rose Mendoza
    Florida State University Libraries Electronic Theses, Treatises and Dissertations The Graduate School 2004 Pale Intrusions into Blue: The Development of a Color Hannah Rose Mendoza Follow this and additional works at the FSU Digital Library. For more information, please contact [email protected] THE FLORIDA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF VISUAL ARTS AND DANCE PALE INTRUSIONS INTO BLUE: THE DEVELOPMENT OF A COLOR By HANNAH ROSE MENDOZA A Thesis submitted to the Department of Interior Design in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Fine Arts Degree Awarded: Fall Semester, 2004 The members of the Committee approve the thesis of Hannah Rose Mendoza defended on October 21, 2004. _________________________ Lisa Waxman Professor Directing Thesis _________________________ Peter Munton Committee Member _________________________ Ricardo Navarro Committee Member Approved: ______________________________________ Eric Wiedegreen, Chair, Department of Interior Design ______________________________________ Sally Mcrorie, Dean, School of Visual Arts & Dance The Office of Graduate Studies has verified and approved the above named committee members. ii To Pepe, te amo y gracias. iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I want to express my gratitude to Lisa Waxman for her unflagging enthusiasm and sharp attention to detail. I also wish to thank the other members of my committee, Peter Munton and Rick Navarro for taking the time to read my thesis and offer a very helpful critique. I want to acknowledge the support received from my Mom and Dad, whose faith in me helped me get through this. Finally, I want to thank my son Jack, who despite being born as my thesis was nearing completion, saw fit to spit up on the manuscript only once.
    [Show full text]
  • Differential Evolutionary History in Visual and Olfactory Floral Cues of the Bee-Pollinated Genus Campanula (Campanulaceae)
    plants Article Differential Evolutionary History in Visual and Olfactory Floral Cues of the Bee-Pollinated Genus Campanula (Campanulaceae) Paulo Milet-Pinheiro 1,*,† , Pablo Sandro Carvalho Santos 1, Samuel Prieto-Benítez 2,3, Manfred Ayasse 1 and Stefan Dötterl 4 1 Institute of Evolutionary Ecology and Conservation Genomics, University of Ulm, Albert-Einstein Allee, 89081 Ulm, Germany; [email protected] (P.S.C.S.); [email protected] (M.A.) 2 Departamento de Biología y Geología, Física y Química Inorgánica, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos-ESCET, C/Tulipán, s/n, Móstoles, 28933 Madrid, Spain; [email protected] 3 Ecotoxicology of Air Pollution Group, Environmental Department, CIEMAT, Avda. Complutense, 40, 28040 Madrid, Spain 4 Department of Biosciences, Paris-Lodron-University of Salzburg, Hellbrunnerstrasse 34, 5020 Salzburg, Austria; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] † Present address: Universidade de Pernambuco, Campus Petrolina, Rodovia BR 203, KM 2, s/n, Petrolina 56328-900, Brazil. Abstract: Visual and olfactory floral signals play key roles in plant-pollinator interactions. In recent decades, studies investigating the evolution of either of these signals have increased considerably. However, there are large gaps in our understanding of whether or not these two cue modalities evolve in a concerted manner. Here, we characterized the visual (i.e., color) and olfactory (scent) floral cues in bee-pollinated Campanula species by spectrophotometric and chemical methods, respectively, with Citation: Milet-Pinheiro, P.; Santos, the aim of tracing their evolutionary paths. We found a species-specific pattern in color reflectance P.S.C.; Prieto-Benítez, S.; Ayasse, M.; and scent chemistry.
    [Show full text]
  • Color Preferences for Different Topics in Connection to Personal Characteristics
    J_ID: COL Customer A_ID: COL21845 Cadmus Art: COL21845 Ed. Ref. No.: 13-030.R2 Date: 9-October-13 Stage: Page: 1 Color Preferences for Different Topics in Connection to Personal Characteristics Iris Bakker,1* Theo van der Voordt,2 Peter Vink,1 Jan de Boon,3 Conne Bazley4 1Faculty of Industrial Design Engineering, Delft University of Technology 2Faculty of Architecture, Delft University of Technology 3de Werkplaats GSB 4JimConna Received 5 April 2013; accepted 29 August 2013 Abstract: Studies on color preferences are dependent on objects by different types of people. VC 2013 Wiley Periodi- the topic and the relationships with personal character- cals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 00, 000–000, 2013; Published Online 00 istics, particularly personality, but these are seldom Month 2013 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI studied in one population. Therefore a questionnaire 10.1002/col.21845 was collected from 1095 Dutch people asking for color preferences about different topics and relating them to Key words: color preference; personal characteristics; personal characteristics. Color preferences regarding personality; mood different topics show different patterns and significant differences were found between gender, age, education and personality such as being technical, being emo- INTRODUCTION tional or being a team player. Also, different colors were mentioned when asked for colors that stimulate to Many Differing Viewpoints on Color Preference be quiet, energetic, and able to focus or creative. Since the end of the 19th century, studies on color Probably, due to unconsciousness of contexts, many preferences show many differences in human preferen- people had no color preference, a result that in the lit- ces.1–3 One of the earliest studies found no general order erature seldom is mentioned.
    [Show full text]
  • Relevance to Attraction in Humans
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by White Rose Research Online Sullivan et al., J Fashion Technol Textile Eng 2017, 5:3 DOI: 10.4172/2329-9568.1000157 Journal of Fashion Technology & Textile Engineering Review Article a SciTechnol journal and their longer-term mates (i.e. husband/wife/partner, fiancé, long- Colored Apparel - Relevance to term relationship) to be physically attractive [5]. For heterosexual groupings, studies have shown that when evaluating a female’s Attraction in Humans attractiveness, men focus not only on physical cues such as facial Sullivan CR1*, Kazlauciunas A2* and Guthrie JT2 expression and body language but also on the type and color of their clothing [6]. For centuries, females have been attracted to the use of color. In Abstract the 1930’s, Korda et al. [7] stated that color was particularly attractive There are numerous different dyes available, many varied fashion to female cinema audiences. Yevonda, in the 1930s, stated that trends, and various different ways to change/enhance physical women were more attached to the use of color photography than were aesthetics. Predicting color preferences and how colors and color men as the medium was better able to highlight visible signs of the combinations, in a shape context, stimulate certain emotions, times, such as red hair, uniforms, flawless complexions and cosmetic represents a challenging prospect. Color is a critical cue for sexual combinations (lips and colored finger nails) [8]. Such use allowed signaling, but what the preferred colors actually are in humans, is females to express themselves more fully [8].
    [Show full text]
  • Trapping Drosophila Repleta (Diptera: Drosophilidae) Using Color and Volatiles B
    Trapping Drosophila repleta (Diptera: Drosophilidae) using color and volatiles B. A. Hottel1,*, J. L. Spencer1 and S. T. Ratcliffe3 Abstract Color and volatile stimulus preferences of Drosophila repleta (Patterson) Diptera: Drosophilidae), a nuisance pest of swine and poultry facilities, were tested using sticky card and bottle traps. Attractions to red, yellow, blue, orange, green, purple, black, grey and a white-on-black contrast treatment were tested in the laboratory. Drosophila repleta preferred red over yellow and white but not over blue. Other than showing preferences over the white con- trol, D. repleta was not observed to have preferences between other colors and shade combinations. Pinot Noir red wine, apple cider vinegar, and wet swine feed were used in volatile preference field trials. Red wine was more attractiveD. to repleta than the other volatiles tested, but there were no dif- ferences in response to combinations of a red wine volatile lure and various colors. Odor was found to play the primary role in attracting D. repleta. Key Words: Drosophila repleta; color preference; volatile preference; trapping Resumen Se evaluaron las preferencias de estímulo de volátiles y color de Drosophila repleta (Patterson) (Diptera: Drosophilidae), una plaga molesta en las instalaciones porcinas y avícolas, utilzando trampas de tarjetas pegajosas y de botella. Su atracción a los tratamientos de color rojo, amarillo, azul, anaranjado, verde, morado, negro, gris y un contraste de blanco sobre negro fue probado en el laboratorio. Drosophila repleta preferio el rojo mas que el amarillo y el blanco, pero no sobre el azul. Aparte de mostrar una preferencia por el control de color blanco, no se observó que D.
    [Show full text]
  • Individual Differences in the Perception of Color Solutions
    foods Article Individual Differences in the Perception of Color Solutions Ulla Hoppu 1, Sari Puputti 1 , Heikki Aisala 1,2, Oskar Laaksonen 2 and Mari Sandell 1,3,* 1 Functional Foods Forum, University of Turku, 20014 Turku, Finland; ulla.hoppu@utu.fi (U.H.); sari.puputti@utu.fi (S.P.); heikki.aisala@utu.fi (H.A.) 2 Food Chemistry and Food Development, Department of Biochemistry, University of Turku, 20014 Turku, Finland; oskar.laaksonen@utu.fi 3 Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA * Correspondence: mari.sandell@utu.fi; Tel.: +358-40-352-4149 Received: 31 August 2018; Accepted: 17 September 2018; Published: 18 September 2018 Abstract: The color of food is important for flavor perception and food selection. The aim of the present study was to evaluate the visual color perception of liquid samples among Finnish adult consumers by their background variables. Participants (n = 205) ranked six different colored solutions just by looking according to four attributes: from most to least pleasant, healthy, sweet and sour. The color sample rated most frequently as the most pleasant was red (37%), the most healthy white (57%), the most sweet red and orange (34% both) and the most sour yellow (54%). Ratings of certain colors differed between gender, age, body mass index (BMI) and education groups. Females regarded the red color as the sweetest more often than males (p = 0.013) while overweight subjects rated the orange as the sweetest more often than normal weight subjects (p = 0.029). Personal characteristics may be associated with some differences in color associations. Keywords: color; visual; taste; perception; gender 1.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Individual Colour Preferences in Consumer Purchase Decisions
    This is a repository copy of The role of individual colour preferences in consumer purchase decisions. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/120692/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Yu, L, Westland, S orcid.org/0000-0003-3480-4755, Li, Z et al. (3 more authors) (2018) The role of individual colour preferences in consumer purchase decisions. Color Research and Application, 43 (2). pp. 258-267. ISSN 0361-2317 https://doi.org/10.1002/col.22180 © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. This is the peer reviewed version of the following article: Yu L, Westland S, Li Z, Pan Q, Shin M-J, Won S. The role of individual colour preferences in consumer purchase decisions. Color Res Appl. 2017;00:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1002/col.22180 ; which has been published in final form at https://doi.org/10.1002/col.22180. This article may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance with the Wiley Terms and Conditions for Self-Archiving. Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request.
    [Show full text]
  • Differences in Color Categorization Manifested by Males and Females: a Quantitative World Color Survey Study
    ARTICLE https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0341-7 OPEN Differences in color categorization manifested by males and females: a quantitative World Color Survey study Nicole A. Fider 1* & Natalia L. Komarova1* ABSTRACT Gender-related differences in human color preferences, color perception, and color lexicon have been reported in the literature over several decades. This work focuses on 1234567890():,; the way the two genders categorize color stimuli. Using the cross-cultural data from the World Color Survey (WCS) and rigorous mathematical methodology, a function is con- structed, which measures the differences in color categorization systems manifested by men and women. A significant number of cases are identified, where men and women exhibit markedly disparate behavior. Interestingly, of the regions in the Munsell color array, the green-blue (“grue”) region appears to be associated with the largest group of categorization differences, with females revealing a more differentiated color categorization pattern com- pared to males. More precisely, in those cases, females tend to use separate green and/or blue categories, while males predominantly use the grue category. In general, the cases singled out by our method warrant a closer study, as they may indicate a transitional cate- gorization scheme. 1 Mathematics Department, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697-3875, USA. *email: nfi[email protected]; [email protected] PALGRAVE COMMUNICATIONS | (2019) 5:142 | https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0341-7 | www.nature.com/palcomms 1 ARTICLE PALGRAVE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0341-7 Introduction t has been asserted (and subsequently, studied for the past The studies reported above encompass a range of fields, from Iseveral decades) that languages have specific categorization physiology to psychology and linguistics.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Influence to the Color Preference According to Product Category
    KEER2014, LINKÖPING | JUNE 11-13 2014 INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON KANSEI ENGINEERING AND EMOTION RESEARCH Cultural Influence to the Color Preference According to Product Category Kazuko Sakamoto Kyoto Institute of Technology, Japan, [email protected], Abstract: In this study, I focus on color, one of the factors involved in design. It has been assumed that color preference is affected by culture and geographical factors, and much international comparative research has been done on this issue. However, the conclusions vary widely, suggesting that it is difficult to generalize. Therefore, in addition to studying color preference itself, I investigated how basic stated color preference is correlated with specific color preference for commercial products. I analyzed how color preferences vary in different countries and product categories. I interviewed Japanese, Chinese Vietnam and Dutch students on their color preferences, and investigated the correlation between their basic color preference and their specific color preference for product categories such as clothes, cell phones, notebook computers, refrigerators, and vehicles. I found that Japanese participants tend to prefer dark colors. All of three nations other than China liked achromatic colors such as black and white for commercial products. By contrast, the color preferences of Chinese participants varied widely. The Chinese tend to have similar color preferences throughout product categories, whereas the Japanese, Vietnam and Dutch people showed different tendencies for different categories. Keywords: Color Preference, Product Category, International Comparison, Cultural Background 1. INTRODUCTION Thanks to the rapid progress of technology, the function and quality of home electronics have developed in a similar way worldwide. Therefore, many firms differentiate their products by form or color.
    [Show full text]
  • The Visual Perception System and Color
    The Visual Perception System and Color The Nature of Color Color is a perceptual phenomenon. Color is caused by receptors in our eyes that are stimulated by electromagnetic radiation of certain wavelengths. The production of color requires 3 elements: light source, object, and the eye-brain system of viewer. There are some physical dimensions to color, Spectral and Reflected Color. Spectral color refers to color that is emitted as light, we respond to spectral colors of the visible spectrum. Reflected color results from selective absorption of visible radiation. Spectral Color (Color as Light) Energy emitted from the sun comes in the form of the electromagnetic spectrum; colors come from a small portion of those wavelengths that are spectral hues of light. A hue is a specific color (red, green, blue, etc.) – see figure below. When all the hues are combined together you create 'white light'. In the textbook, you should read and be able to: 1) Explain Subtractive colors, as well as CMYK 2) Define/explain the color dimensions of Hue, Value and Saturation, and be able to draw the corresponding color space: Hue Value/Intensity Chroma/Saturation Color Space: 3) Explain the Munsel color space (see fig below) – a perceptually determined color space 4) Label the various parts of the eye, and explain their purpose/function in the visual system. Rods, Cones, Retina, Lens, Pupil, 5) Explain color connotations: color connotations must be taken into account in combination with intended use when designing a map (from Johnson, 2001, Cartographic Perspectives).
    [Show full text]
  • Study on Design Principle of Clothing Display Color
    http://sass.sciedupress.com Studies in Asian Social Science Vol. 2, No. 2; 2015 Study on Design Principle of Clothing Display Color Wanyue Hu1 & Yanmei Li1 1 Shanghai University of Engineering Science Fashion Institute, Shanghai, China Correspondence: Yanmei Li, Shanghai University of Engineering Science Fashion Institute, Shanghai, China. Tel: 86-180-1765-8490. E-mail: [email protected] Fund programs Shanghai University of Engineering Science innovation project “Design and implementation of virtual closet system based on mobile terminal” (E1-0903-14-01168) Shanghai University of Engineering Science innovation project “Development of clothing network closet system based on the mobile platform” (A1-5300-14-050137) Received: April 8, 2015 Accepted: April 26, 2015 Online Published: July 2, 2015 doi:10.5430/sass.v2n2p8 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5430/sass.v2n2p8 Abstract Display color design is a very important part of visual merchandising. The article briefly introduces the concept and design category of display color design, and show its important in clothing marketing and management, then elaborate the design principle of clothing display color mainly from two aspects of clothing brand positioning and consumer psychology. Clothing brand should focus on research the color psychology and preference of target consumers after clear their own target consumers positioning, in order to better for color design of clothing and display. Keywords: visual merchandising, display color design, brand positioning, color psychology Preface Visual merchandising (VMD) as a kind of product planning marketing strategy with visual as the center have been noticed by enterprise and display division in recent years (Mu & Pan, 2014), it show the enterprise information, product information, service concept and brand culture to consumers through the display of goods, and to promote sales, improve corporate profits, setting up enterprise brand image as well (Hu, 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Empower Software
    Empower Software System Administrator’s Guide 34 Maple Street Milford, MA 01757 71500031708, Revision A NOTICE The information in this document is subject to change without notice and should not be construed as a commitment by Waters Corporation. Waters Corporation assumes no responsibility for any errors that may appear in this document. This document is believed to be complete and accurate at the time of publication. In no event shall Waters Corporation be liable for incidental or consequential damages in connection with, or arising from, the use of this document. © 2002 WATERS CORPORATION. PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. THIS DOCUMENT OR PARTS THEREOF MAY NOT BE REPRODUCED IN ANY FORM WITHOUT THE WRITTEN PERMISSION OF THE PUBLISHER. Millennium and Waters are registered trademarks, and Empower, LAC/E, and SAT/IN are trademarks of Waters Corporation. Microsoft, MS, Windows, and Windows NT are registered trademarks of Microsoft Corporation. Oracle, SQL*Net, and SQL*Plus are registered trademarks, and Oracle8, Oracle8i, and Oracle9i are trademarks of Oracle Corporation. Pentium and Pentium II are registered trademarks of Intel Corporation. TCP/IP is a trademark of FTP Software, Inc. All other trademarks or registered trademarks are the sole property of their respective owners. Table of Contents Preface ....................................................................................... 12 Chapter 1 Introduction ...................................................................................... 18 1.1
    [Show full text]