Quick viewing(Text Mode)

Foraging Behaviour of the Slender Loris (Loris Lydekkerianus Lydekkerianus): Implications for Theories of Primate Origins

Foraging Behaviour of the Slender Loris (Loris Lydekkerianus Lydekkerianus): Implications for Theories of Primate Origins

Journal of 49 (2005) 289e300

Foraging behaviour of the slender (Loris lydekkerianus lydekkerianus): implications for theories of origins

K.A.I. Nekaris*

Department of Anthropology, Washington University, Campus Box 1114, One Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA

Received 3 May 2004; accepted 18 April 2005

Abstract

Members of the are characterised by a wide overlap of visual fields or optic convergence. It has been proposed that exploitation of either insects or angiosperm products in the terminal branches of trees, and the corresponding complex, three-dimensional environment associated with these foraging strategies, account for visual convergence. Although slender (Loris sp.) are the most visually convergent of all the primates, very little is known about their feeding ecology. This study, carried out over 10 ½ months in South , examines the feeding behaviour of L. lydekkerianus lydekkerianus in relation to hypotheses regarding visual of insects. Of 1238 feeding observations, 96% were of prey. Lorises showed an equal and overwhelming preference for terminal and middle branch feeding, using the undergrowth and trunk rarely. The type of prey caught on terminal branches (Lepidoptera, Odonata, Homoptera) differed significantly from those caught on middle branches (Hymenoptera, Coleoptera). A two-handed catch accompanied by bipedal postures was used almost exclusively on terminal branches where mobile prey was caught, whereas the more common capture technique of one-handed grab was used more often on sturdy middle branches to obtain slow moving prey. Although prey was detected with senses other than vision, vision was the key sense used upon the final strike. This study strongly supports the notion that hunting for animal prey was a key ecological determinant in selecting for visual convergence early on in primate evolution. The extreme specialisations of slender lorises, however, suggest that early primates were not dedicated faunivores and lend further support to the emerging view that both insects and fruits were probably important components of the diet of primates, and that exploitation of fruits may account for other key primate traits. Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Primates; Lorisiformes; ; Visual predation; Insectivory; Primate evolution

* Corresponding author. Oxford Brookes University, School of Social Sciences and Law, Department of Anthropology, Nocturnal Primate Research Group, Oxford OX3 OBP, United Kingdom. Tel.: C44 1865 483 760; fax: C44 1865 483937. E-mail address: [email protected]

0047-2484/$ - see front matter Ó 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.04.004 290 K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300

Introduction to an arboreal mode of life was essential for the evolution of primate traits such as grasping Optic convergence and the presence of a post- and feet characterised by nails (Wood Jones, 1916; orbital bar delineate the earliest known Rasmussen, 1990; Crompton, 1995), some authors primates from their forebears. These traits are suggest that acquisition of inconspicuous angio- crucial in distinguishing euprimates from other sperm products (fruits, flowers, nectar) in the basal , and their ecological function has terminal branches of trees was the essential been the question of lively debate (i.e. Wood selective pressure for the evolution of forward Jones, 1916; Smith, 1924; Cartmill, 1972, 1974a; facing eyes (Sussman, 1991; Bloch and Boyer, Sussman, 1991; Crompton, 1995; Kay et al., 1997). 2002). Evidence for this point of view is drawn One of the principal contending hypotheses from parallels with fruit-eating megachiropteran regarding the evolution of convergent optic axes bats, and phalangerid marsupials, which also have in primates is the Visual Predation Hypothesis relatively convergent eyes, yet rely mainly on fruit (Cartmill, 1972, 1974b, 1980, 1992). Cartmill (Sussman, 1995). Sussman also pointed out that argued that marked optic convergence tends to African (Perodicticinae, formerly classified be a trait of that rely on vision to track as Lorisinae) rely extensively on repugnant prey and capture prey items, i.e. visual predators. that may be detected by scent rather than vision Furthermore, he contended that increased stere- (Sussman, 1991). oscopy reduces parallax, diminishing depth per- Despite the acknowledged importance of the ception at a distance, but is useful for hunting at Lorisinae in this debate (Cartmill, 1972; Ross, close range. Amongst primates, these features in 1995; Gebo, 2004), no studies have clarified the combination with stealthy locomotion and grasp- extent to which lorises are ‘visual predators,’ ing extremities, used during hunting in the although they have been described as thus in the terminal branches of the canopy understory or absence of field data (Cartmill, 1974a; Ross, 1996). shrub layers, would favour the evolution of those Even the widely cited dietary study of African traits exhibited by the earliest primates (eupri- perodicticines relied on stomach contents analyses mates). In addition to convergent eyes, reduced and observations of wild animals under experi- incisors would be selected for as prey acquisition mental conditions, as field conditions were too functions transferred to the hands, as would difficult to observe hunting behaviour in detail a reduced snout as eye/ replaced nose/mouth (Charles-Dominique, 1977). coordination. Cartmill pointed out that these If visual predation as described by Cartmill was trends culminated in Loris, an Asian strepsirrhine indeed crucial to the selection of modern lorisine primate belonging to the subfamily Lorisinae. He traits, one would expect them to include at least suggested that members of this ‘‘are more 50% animal material in their diet, to catch animal highly specialised than any other living strepsir- prey with stealth, to procure this prey from small rhine for the mode of life whose adoption led to fine branches, and to use their hands whilst eating the differentiation of the order Primates from the it. Two field studies have now shown that slender other placentals (p. 120, 1972).’’ lorises are almost exclusively faunivorous, with Aspects of Cartmill’s hypothesis have been 96% (Loris lydekkerianus lydekkerianus) to 100% tested in the laboratory (Lemelin and Grafton, (Loris tardigradus) of their diet consisting of 1998; Lemelin, 1999), in the field (Garber, 1980, animal prey (Nekaris, 2002; Nekaris and Rasmus- 1991; Rasmussen, 1990) and against the fossil sen, 2003; Nekaris and Jayewardene, 2003). The record (Hamrick, 2001; Bloch and Boyer, 2002). diet of lorises is characterised by insects of small These experiments have provided evidence for and size that can be easily snatched, and taxa contain- against the Visual Predation hypothesis, which is ing high amounts of toxic secondary compounds, not without contention (Sussman and Raven, but also includes high energy animals like . 1978; Sussman, 1991, 1995; Bloch and Boyer, Slender lorises are capable of moving at high 2002). Although all authors concur that transition speed, (Nekaris, 2001; Nekaris and Stevens, 2005), K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300 291 and move swiftly and fluidly until within range of hand (small, covered by hand; medium, prey over- an evasive prey item, at which point they move laps the hand on the edges; large, several orders with more stealth, fix on it with their eyes and then larger than a loris’ hand). Prey capture technique pounce on it rapidly. Loris catches most prey on included swiftly catching the prey with one hand branches smaller than 5 cm in diameter almost (‘grab’), catching it with two hands (‘catch’), exclusively manually removing prey from a sub- tugging it forcibly from a substrate (‘pull’), and strate. Hands are also used to dismember prey, or removing it from the substrate with the mouth to hold prey whilst the teeth remove undesirable alone (‘mouth’). Method of prey detection, using elements (e.g. wings of cicadas). vision, scent or hearing, was recorded qualitatively Although characterising the diet, these studies based on the behaviour of the loris. Visual did not address where lorises acquired their prey detection included fixing the eyes on a prey item, and did not attempt to quantify how they obtained often from a distance of 2 m or gently head cocking their prey beyond the qualitative level. In this while gazing at a prey item; auditory detection paper, I consider prey detection method, prey involved twitching the ears; and olfactory detection acquisition strategy, and positional behaviour of involved pronounced sniffing, sometimes accom- slender lorises whilst foraging in relation to panied by drooling. locality within the arboreal environment. I address Non-parametric Mann Whitney U and c2 how factors of the arboreal environment relate to analyses were conducted with SPSS V11.0 with foraging behaviour of Loris as regards visual probability set at the 0.05 level. Figures were predation. In particular, is visual predation the drawn by A. Brady from video sequences taken by preferred hunting tactic of slender lorises? If so, Nekaris with a Hi8 Sony Video Camera with when, where and on what type of prey do they NightshotÓ. engage in this mode of food acquisition? Finally, what is the relevance of these observations in understanding the environmental and ecological Results variables that may have influenced the evolution of primate traits? A total of 1240 food acquisition events were recorded, 1189 of which involved capture and consumption of animal prey. Of prey items, 605 Methods could be identified to ordinal level (Nekaris and Rasmussen, 2003). When all feeding events are From 1997-1998, I conducted a socio-ecological considered, the one-handed grab was the preferred study of the Mysore (Loris lydekker- acquisition technique for all observations ianus lydekkerianus) over 10 ½ months in the (c2 Z 2401.57, df Z 3, p % 0.0001), as well as for Beerangi Karadu hill range of Ayyalur Interface identifiable food items (c2 Z 1066.67, df Z 3, Forestry Division, Dindigul District, , p % 0.0001). Slender lorises, when all items are South India. I describe this dry scrub forest in considered, acquired food in the following way: detail elsewhere (Nekaris, 2001). one-handed catch (85%), two-handed catch (8%), Feeding observations were recorded with all by mouth (5%), and by pulling (2%). occurrences sampling (Nekaris and Rasmussen, When identifiable items are considered, some 2003). As lorises hunted in dry scrub vegetation food items were obtained with a specific acquisi- replete with bushes and short trees (Nekaris, tion technique. A c2 cross tabulation revealed 2001), foraging locations were divided into four that the mouth was used for consuming gum, categories: fine terminal branches, sturdy middle grabbing with one hand was the preferred tech- branches, the trunk, and the ground. nique for Hymenoptera, whereas catching with Food items were classified according to type two hands was used significantly more for larvae, (defined to Order for prey items whenever Orthoptera and Lepidoptera (c2 Z 1002.54, possible), and size relative to the size of a loris’ df Z 24, p ! 0.0001) (Table 1). 292 K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300

Table 1 Proportion of observations that slender lorises used various acquisition methods for food items procured five times or more Catch Grab Mouth Pull Hymenoptera (n Z 368) 0.01 0.96***1 0.03 0 Coleoptera (n Z 69) 0.04 0.92 0.03 0.01 Larvae (n Z 33) 0.333 0.583 0.083 Orthoptera (n Z 74) 0.18 0.82 Lepidoptera (n Z 34) 0.62***1 0.38 Mollusca (n Z 16) 0.25 0.75***1 Odonata (n Z 6) 1.0 Homoptera (n Z 21) 0.19 0.81 Gum (n Z 35) 1.0***1 1 Four capture techniques were associated with food types significantly more than would be expected by chance at the p % 0.0001 level.

Only 1% (n Z 14) of observations involved walking, standing and climbing accompanied slender lorises acquiring food from the under- middle branch foraging. Of prey consumed on growth. In all cases, the animal hung bipedally the middle branches, Hymenoptera and Coleoptera from a low-hanging branch, listened for insects were consumed the most (c2 Z 648.09, df Z 5, moving in the litter, visually located the prey, p ! 0.0001), and were also consumed significantly and caught it with one hand. Although one , more in the middle branches than in the terminal one larva, and two hymenopterans were procured branches (c2 Z 70.93, df Z 7, p ! 0.0001). Small from the undergrowth, most prey items caught in prey, due to the predominance of ants consumed in this area were from the Blattidae of the this microhabitat, were consumed more than other Order Orthoptera (n Z 9) (c2 Z 17.43, df Z 4, prey sizes (c2 Z 292.36, df Z 3, p ! 0.0001). Lor- p Z 0.002). Most items were of a large size class ises consumed insects in bouts of up to 36 (n Z 10) (c2 Z 9.14, df Z 2, p ! 0. 01). individuals (Nekaris and Rasmussen, 2003). Insects Trunk foraging comprised 3.3% (n Z 42) of were consumed in bouts more on middle branches food acquisition episodes. Although four ants and than on terminal branches (U Z 35928.0, three beetles were procured from the trunk, most Z Z ÿ3.30, p Z 0.001) (Table 2). (n Z 35) of these incidences were of gum eating Terminal branch foraging in the mobile fine (c2 Z 47.29, df Z 2, p ! 0.0001). This type of branches occurred 49% of the time (n Z 612). feeding was accompanied by olfactory and visual Detection mode was usually visual, with the detection, oral consumption (no use of hands to animal fixing on a prey item from the sturdy extract food except to hold onto the trunk) middle branches, and then fluidly venturing out to (c2 Z 27.524, df Z 1, p ! 0.0001), and clinging the terminal branches to catch the prey item to the substrate (Schulze and Meier, 1995). (Fig. 2). On only two occasions did a loris catch Foraging in the more sturdy middle branches a prey item in the terminal branches and move comprised 46% (n Z 571) of all observations. In with it back to the middle branches; both of these general detection was visual with the animal fixing were large prey that required 3-5 minutes to eat. its eyes on the branch as it walked, plucking prey Other food items were eaten in situ. Flying insects, items with a one-handed grab, keeping the other particularly of small size class, sometimes caught three limbs firmly attached to the substrate a loris’ attention auditorily, after which it would (c2 Z 1444.59, df Z 3, p ! 0.0001) (Fig. 1). On visually fix on the insect for the final strike. Several four occasions olfactory detection was used prey types were caught more on the terminal whereby the animal raised its chin and pro- branches than anywhere else and included Lepi- nouncedly sniffed the air; this was used for doptera, Orthoptera, and unidentified larvae and tenebrionid beetles, which emit a (c2 Z 427.72, df Z 7, p ! 0.0001). Insects of the strong smelling chemical compound. Quadrupedal orders Odonata, Homoptera, and Diptera, as well K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300 293

Fig. 1. Shows a slender loris visually detecting a prey item on the sturdy middle branches, and procuring it with a one-handed grab, whilst keeping all other limbs on the substrate. as a , were caught more on the terminal middle branches (U Z 170853.50, Z Z ÿ2.388, branches, but sample sizes were not enough to test p Z 0.017). Other than grabbing, the most com- significance. Some of these prey items (dragonflies mon type of prey acquisition was the two-handed in particular) were removed from their sleeping catch, in general accompanied by the more sites, and were caught with such stealth that they acrobatic cantilever, bipedal hang or bipedal stand were not awoken. Ants comprised a large portion (c2 Z 718.26, df Z 3, p ! 0.0001) (Table 2). of the terminal branch foraging regime, and the overall predominant size class chosen on terminal Discussion branches was small (c2 Z 117.23, df Z 3, p ! 0.0001). Still, large and medium-sized prey Slender lorises were almost exclusively arboreal were caught more on terminal branches than on hunters, specialising in the small branch milieu. 294 K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300

Fig. 2. Shows a slender loris visually detecting a prey item in the flexible terminal branches, and procuring it with a two-handed grab, whilst maintaining hold on a branch with a cantilever stance.

Even when selecting prey items from the un- vision was not the only sense used by lorises to dergrowth, at least two limbs remained in contact perceive prey. Olfactory and auditory prey de- with a branch. This is unlike some other predatory tection played a limited role. Auditory localisation nocturnal (e.g. Tarsius spectrum, Sciur- of flying insects was not as marked as in ocheirus alleni cameronensis) which descend to the (Charles-Dominique, 1977). Olfactory inspection, ground for hunting up to 10% of observations used to search for slugs and repellent beetles, was (Gursky, 2000; Pimley, 2002). not as common as interpreted for some pottos In general, clear visual tracking of prey was (P. p. juju, P. p. )(Oates, 1984). It should be followed by a ballistic single or two-handed strike emphasised, however, that even when another of prey items. A similar technique has also been detection technique was used, as soon lorises came observed in some pottos (Perodicticus potto within close range, they fixed on an item visually in edwardsi)(Pimley, 2002) and order to snatch it. Galagos, too, although often (Schwenk, 2000). Although the primary technique, using auditory means to detect prey, rely chiefly on K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300 295

Table 2 This table shows the proportion of prey items (selected more than five times) making up dietary choices on the middle and terminal branches (n Z 577). A C shows prey types that were potentially evasive Food eaten Location middle terminal Formicidae/Isoptera (ants and ) 75.2% (n Z 188)***1 53.5% (n Z 175) Coleoptera (beetles and weevils) 15.2% (n Z 38) ***1 8.9% (n Z 29) COrthoptera (crickets, mantids, cockroaches, etc.) 6.0% (n Z 3) 9.2% (n Z 12)***1 CHomoptera (cicadas and relatives) 2.0% (n Z 5) 4.9% (n Z 16) Mollusca (slugs) 1.2% (n Z 3) 3.7% (n Z 12) CLepidoptera (moths and butterflies) 7.3% (n Z 24)***1 COdonata (dragonflies and damselflies) 1.8% (n Z 6) unidentified larvae 0.4% (n Z 1) 10.7% (n Z 35)***1 ***1indicates that these prey types were chosen more at their respective localities more than would be expected by chance at the 0.0001 level. vision when it comes to the catch (Bearder et al., in evasive prey (cicadas, cricket-like orthopterans, press; Bearder, pers. comm.). moths), as well as potentially evasive sleeping Slender lorises were not terminal branch spe- insects, which were rarely seen to be awakened by cialists, but spent an almost equal proportion of the stealthy loris. Disturbance of such prey could observations foraging on more stable middle result in a considerable energetic loss, making branches and slender mobile terminal branches. fluid, non-leaping locomotion beneficial. A similar It should be noted that in this scrub environment, strategy has been suggested for chameleons, which the ‘more stable’ middle branches were still of are renowned for their keen visual predation a size class between 5-10 cm, and generally fell at (Herrel et al., 2001). Evasive prey items were the lower end of the size spectrum (Nekaris, 2001). detected from a distance of approximately two The primary identifiable food items consumed by meters or less, stalked, and then snatched from lorises were ants, and these were caught in both the a branch or from the air whilst the loris was middle and terminal branches. engaged in a more acrobatic posture, usually When analysing locality of prey items, intrigu- accompanied by grasping of branches with only ing patterns emerge. The middle branches pro- two limbs. Often before seizing a prey item, the vided lorises with a venue on which to move loris noiselessly coiled up its rear limbs, com- steadily whilst actively visually scanning and pressed its ears, and visually focussed on the prey smelling their environment (Oates, 1984). Animals item (not unlike a house cat catching a mouse), hunted with three limbs securely attached to and almost always caught the insect. Lorises were a substrate, and easily obtained small prey also adept at catching prey on the wing, but did consumed in bouts. On only four occasions lorises this by detecting insects already flying, rather than lapped up ants or termites directly with the mouth, those it disturbed by moving through a tree (c.f. as has also been exhibited by thick-tailed bushba- Galagoides demidovii, Charles-Dominique, 1977). bies (Otolemur crassicaudatus), Allen’s bushbabies On-going studies of Sri Lankan slender lorises (S. a. cameronensis), and pottos (P. p. juju, P. p. (L. l. nordicus and Loris tardigradus tardigradus) potto)(Oates, 1984; Happold and Happold, 1992; corroborate the view that Loris is a visual predator Pimley, 2002). Ants were usually picked up one (Nekaris, 2002). Loris t. tardigradus relies on more after the next with the hand and placed in the vertebrate prey than L. lydekkerianus, stalking mouth. nocturnal geckoes, and relishing sleeping lizards, A marked difference in foraging strategy which camouflage themselves splayed amongst the occurred in the terminal branches. Although terminal branches; good vision with an efficient hunting in bouts decreased, acquisition of high strike is important, as these lizards awaken easily energy larger prey increased. These included (Nekaris and Jayewardene, 2003). A pattern is 296 K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300 beginning to emerge that the middle and terminal decisive selective factors of the ancestral primate branches offer distinct feeding niches for slender niche. Sussman’s elegant hypothesis, combined lorises. Further studies should consider the relative with the new data on Carpolestes, little nutrient intake of terminal versus middle branch room for a highly predatory loris to be of any prey not only amongst slender lorises, but also relevance to the evolution of primate traits e or amongst primates in general. does it? The slender loris, with its marked optic Not all authors agree that Carpolestes is convergence, appears to be the ultimate visual relevant to the question of primate origins. Firstly, predator, but do the data presented here corrob- it has been strongly argued that the plesiadapi- orate the visual predation hypothesis? This de- forms have closer affiliations to the order Der- pends on one’s view of another major scenario, moptera and are not a to primates which has been invoked to explain the origin of (Beard, 1993). Even if this interpretation is primate traits. The ‘Angiosperm Co-evolution’ rejected, other analyses of the material suggest hypothesis proposes that the origin and expansion that C. simpsoni resembles other plesiadapiforms of euprimates took place alongside an adaptive in all skeletal features but the feet (Kirk et al., radiation of angiosperms and tropical forests, and 2003). Gebo (2004) posited that including carpo- that sophisticated exploitation of angiosperm lestids as a euprimate sister group would create products in the terminal branch niche selected taxonomic havoc, and that at best one might be for primate traits (Sussman and Raven, 1978; able to consider C. simpsoni a ‘protoprimate’ with Sussman, 1991, 1995). Many primates are indeed little bearing on the question of primate origins. keystone pollinators of tropical forest species; Prehensility in the hands and feet not unlike these co-evolutionary relationships are clearly that exhibited by C. simpsoni has evolved in deep rooted. Sussman argues that primates would numerous other mammals, including a number of have required grasping hands and feed and carnivores, and marsupials (Cartmill, convergent vision in order to effectively manoeu- 1972; Soligo and Mu¨ ller, 1999; Hamrick, 2001). vre through the complex three dimensional envi- Lemelin (1999), in a comparative study of didel- ronment of the terminal branches, while at the phids, found that prehensility of the hands in- same time collecting fruits, many of which might creased in animals that rely more on smaller be small and cryptically coloured. gauged substrates, such as vines and terminal Sussman’s view has recently been potentially branches. His study supported the potential substantiated with a new important fossil find e importance of the terminal branch niche, but could a relatively complete fossil of Carpolestes simpsoni not address whether dietary choice was related to (Bloch and Boyer, 2002). This animal belongs to the evolution of cheiridial prehensility, especially a group of archaic mammals called plesiadapi- considering that didelphids tend to consume forms, which some authors suggest is the sister a combination of fruits and insects (Charles- group to primates, and thus should share primitive Dominique, 1983). In a related study of nocturnal traits with the first euprimates (Fox, 1993; Bloch primates, however, he did find that catching and and Silcox, 2001). The new specimen, which could moving live prey with the hands were related to be a basal primate, is replete with a grasping digit length, and subsequently with locomotor with a , as well as hands with grasping pattern in fine branches (Lemelin, 1996). These capabilities not unlike that of a tree shrew (Sargis, results suggest that Bloch and Boyer’s grasping 2001), but lacks orbital convergence. The authors plesiadapiform need not have been wholly frugiv- suggest that their evidence that grasping developed orous; Bloch and Boyer themselves suggest that all independently of other traits would be adaptive for carpolestids were probably (2002). angiosperm product exploitation, but not for The only other field study that has directly visual predation. Indeed, Cartmill (1992) has tested the Visual Predation hypothesis was again stated that discovering the order in which primate conducted on a primate-like marsupial, Caluromys traits evolved would be vital for understanding the derbianus (Rasmussen, 1990). Based on convergent K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300 297 similarities with primates, Rasmussen studied matter makes up from 50-100% in nine taxa substrate choice, diet and degree of arboreality of (Nekaris and Bearder, in press). moholi, Caluromys in relation to hypotheses regarding like L. lydekkerianus, subsists exclusively on primate origins. He found that Caluromys, which insects and gum (Bearder, 1987), and vision has visually foraged for insects in the terminal branch been shown to be the most important sense in this milieu, provided ‘a perfect living illustration of an species (Bearder et al., in press). Galagoides ancestral primate as envisioned by Cartmill’ rondoensis is almost exclusively insectivorous, (Rasmussen, 1990, p. 272). Unlike Loris, however, acquiring insects from the fine branches and Caluromys was not a dedicated visual predator, undergrowth by visually scanning these areas, but took advantage of resources available in the and leaping rapidly to acquire prey (Perkin, fine branch niche almost wholly ignored by Loris e 2001). In general, larger galagos which rely more fruits and flowers. on fruit have increasingly less convergent vision The complex terminal branch feeding strategy than their smaller more insectivorous relatives led Rasmussen (1990) to synthesise the Visual (Cartmill, 1974b; Ross, 1995). Further studies of Predation hypothesis with the Angiosperm Co- the degree of convergence of new galago taxa in evolution hypothesis. Rasmussen argues these relation to dietary preference will be of interest. hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, and sug- As mentioned at the outset of this article, the gested that early primates may have been lured to Lorisinae and Perodicticinae are posited in the the terminal branches to first exploit fruits, flowers literature as being the most specialised primate and nectar obtained by dexterity and co-ordina- visual predators (e.g. Cartmill, 1972). Available tion, complying with the view of Sussman, and evidence corroborates this view to a limited extent. also with the interpretation of C. simpsoni of Bloch Olfaction and hearing may be of greater impor- and Boyer. Once in this environment, those who tance for the African lorisines, with their galago- could also effectively exploit associated insect like interorbital breadths (Charles-Dominique, assemblages, procured by visual predation and 1974, 1977; Oates, 1984; Ross, 1995). Pottos do facilitated by their grasping hands, would have an catch prey visually, but the genus as a whole advantage, thus accounting for primate visual appears to rely mainly on fruits (Charles-Domi- convergence. The innovative aspect of this synthe- nique, 1977; Oates, 1984; Pimley, 2002). Recent sis was not suggesting that euprimates may have observations of pottos in Cameroon lend support been omnivores, but in acknowledging that the to Rasmussen’s synthesised view, in that pottos environmental stage set by Sussman could be were seen to bipedally hang over nectaries, occupied by a fruit eater that would ultimately also consuming both nectar and associated insects engage in visual predation. Indeed, Cartmill (1972) (Pimley, 2002). On the other hand, Walker himself suggested that ancestral primates formed (1969) suggested that careful hunting of prey, part of a shrub layer guild, taking including birds, probably accounts for the loco- advantage of insect resources in combination with motor anatomy of the potto. The , varying amounts of fruit. Crompton (1995) in- a shrub layer insectivore, relies on slow moving voked a similar ‘fruit first’ combined with oppor- pungent prey, perhaps with greater reliance on tunistic insectivory to explain extreme convergence olfaction (Charles-Dominique, 1977). It relishes in Tarsius. high energy prey in the captive setting, and may A comparison of slender loris feeding ecology exploit these resources in areas of its range where it with that of other lorisiforms provides further is in less competition with other lorisiforms. evidence that visual convergence is an important Amongst the Asian lorisines, Nycticebus pyg- for insect feeding, at least amongst this maeus has been shown to exhibit hunting adapta- infraorder. Insects are a key dietary component to tions not unlike that of Loris, consuming birds, most species of lorisiform primates (galagos, flying insects, and ants; gum forms an important pottos and lorises). Out of 17 species of lorisiforms addition to its diet (Tan and Drake, 2001; Streicher, where diet has been studied in some detail, animal 2004). Although consuming some insects, limited 298 K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300 observation of N. coucang at a study site in Malaysia traits of visual convergence and grasping hands suggest that they specialised on gum and nectar and feet to an extreme (Cartmill, 1974b). The (Wiens, 2002). Furthermore, unlike their ‘slender’ ecological parameters related to its divergence counterparts, slow lorises in captivity preferred from other lorisiforms to such an extent thus may larger and more stable supports, rather than flexible be relevant to reconstructing the ancestral primate small gauged branches (Dykyj, 1980). Still, slow niche. As Rasmussen (1990) rightly points out, lorises are known to relish birds, rodents and other both the Visual Predation and Angiosperm Co- animal prey in both captivity and semi-free ranging evolution theories for primate origins are not environments suggesting that these probably play mutually exclusive, both envisioning an acrobatic an important part in their diet in the wild (Fitch- grasping euprimate foraging in the fine branch Snyder and Schulze, 2001; pers. obs.). milieu. Even Cartmill concurs that ancestral The Mysore slender loris conforms to Cartmill’s primates probably consumed fruit to some extent, image of a small branch visual predatory specialist but that visual predation may be responsible for in numerous respects. Orbital approximation, in the trait of orbital convergence (Cartmill, 1972). combination with stealthy but swift locomotion, The present study provides strong evidence that allows lorises to catch close range prey upon the the adaptive advantage of orbital convergence is first strike, a process not hindered by low degrees probably linked to the visual acquisition of prey of orbital frontation (Cartmill, 1992; Ross, 1995). prior to a precision strike. Tiny hands allow lorises to grasp the smallest of the terminal branches, specialised retia mirabilia allow for extended grasping until the moment Acknowledgements comes to strike the prey, and vice-like feet allow for acrobatic hunting tactics (Osman Hill, 1953; I thank C. Southwick, S. M. Heinrichs and Cartmill, 1992; Rasmussen and Nekaris, 1998). C. A. Buzzell for assistance in the field, and D.T. Slender lorises are so specialised for these small Rasmussen, R. W. Sussman, S.K. Bearder, and supports, they are rarely seen on any support that S. M. Ford for valuable discussions on this proj- they cannot grasp fully (Nekaris, 2001; Nekaris ect. Editorial assistance was provided by A. Miehs and Jayewardene, 2003). and L. Birkett, and illustrations were prepared by Clearly the evidence from the lorisiforms lends A. Brady. Three anonymous reviewers improved itself to the Rasmussen synthesised view, with both the quality of this manuscript immensely. This insects and fruit playing a role. The slender lorises, field study was supported by grants from One with however, being the most faunivorous of the Nature of the Philadelphia Zoo, Sophie Danforth lorisiforms, demonstrate how selective pressures Conservation Biology Fund, Primate Conserva- surrounding visual predation can result in primate tion Inc., Bruce Wulff, Wenner-Gren Foundation to the extreme. (all to Nekaris), and an NSF dissertation improve- ment grant to Nekaris and D. T. Rasmussen (SBR-9714870). Conclusions

Traditionally, most researchers suggest that the References stem lineage of primates was probably small, and in accordance with diet being in part a function of Beard, K.C., 1993. Origin and evolution of gliding in early body size, they must have been partly insectivo- Dermoptera (Mammalia, Primatamorpha). In: rous (Gebo, 2004). With this in mind, a specialised MacPhee, R.D.E. (Ed.), Primates and Their Relatives in faunivore like the Mysore slender loris can by no Phylogenetic Perspectives. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 63e90. means be used as an exact model for the ancestral Bearder, S.K., 1987. Lorises, bushbabies and : diverse euprimate. It is, however, highly adapted to life in societies in solitary foragers. In: Smuts, B.B., Cheney, D.L., an arboreal environment, and exhibits the primate Seyfarth, R.M., Wrangham, R.W., Struhsaker, T.T. (Eds.), K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300 299

Primate Societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. Garber, P., 1991. Seasonal variation in diet and ranging 11e24. patterns in 2 species of tamarin . American Journal Bearder, S.K., Nekaris, K.A.I., Curtis, D.J., 2005. A re-evaluation of Physical Anthropology 12, 75. of the role of vision in the activity and communication of Gebo, D.L., 2004. A shrew-sized origin for primates. Yearbook nocturnal primates. Folia Primatologica, 2005, in press. of Physical Anthropology 47, 40e62. Bloch, J.I., Boyer, D.M., 2002. Grasping primate origins. Gursky, S., 2000. Effect of seasonality on the behaviour of an Science 298, 1606e1610. insectivorous primate, Tarsius spectrum. International Bloch, J.I., Silcox, M.T., 2001. New basicrania of - Journal of Primatolology 21, 477e495. Ignacius: Re-evaluation of the Plesiadapiform-Der- Hamrick, M.W., 2001. Primate origins: evolutionary change in mopteran link. American Journal of Physical Anthropology digital ray patterning and segmentation. Journal of Human 116 (3), 184e198. Evolution 40 (4), 339e351. Cartmill, M., 1972. Arboreal adaptations and the origin of the Happold, D., Happold, M., 1992. Termites as food for the order Primates. In: Tuttle, R. (Ed.), The Functional and thick-tailed bushbaby (Otolemur crassicaudatus) in Malawi. Evolutionary Biology of the Primates. Aldine Atherton, Folia Primatologica 58, 118e120. Chicago, pp. 97e122. Herrel, A., Meyers, J.J., Aerts, P., Nishikawa, K.S., 2001. Cartmill, M., 1974a. Rethinking primate origins. Science 184, Functional implications of supercontracting muscle in the 436e443. retractors. The Journal of Experimental Cartmill, M., 1974b. Pads and in . In: Biology 204, 3621e3627. Jenkins, F.A. (Ed.), Primate Locomotion. Academic Press, Kay, R.F., Ross, C., Williams, B.A., 1997. Anthropoid origins. New York, pp. 45e83. Science 275, 797e804. Cartmill, M., 1980. , function and evolution of the Kirk, E.C., Cartmill, M., Kay, R.F., 2003. Comment on anthropoid postorbital septum. In: Ciochon, R.L., ‘‘Grasping Primate Origins’’. Science 300 (5620), 741. Chiarelli, A.B. (Eds.), Evolutionary Biology of New World Lemelin, P., 1996. Relationships between hand morphology Monkeys and Continental Drift. Plenum Press, New York, and feeding strategies in small-bodied prosimians. American pp. 243e274. Journal of Physical Anthropology 22, 148. Cartmill, M., 1992. New views on primate origins. Evolutionary Lemelin, P., 1999. Morphological correlates of substrate use in Anthropology 3, 105e111. didelphid marsupials: Implications for primate origins. Charles-Dominique, P., 1974. Vie sociale de Perodicticus potto Journal of 247, 165e175. (Primates ). E´ tude de terrain en foreˆ te´ quatorial de Lemelin, P., Grafton, B.W., 1998. Grasping performance in l’ouest africain au Gabon. Mammalia 38, 355e379. Saguinus midas and the evolution of hand prehensility in Charles-Dominique, P., 1977. Ecology and Behaviour of primates. In: Strasser, E., Fleagle, J.G., Rosenberger, A.L., Nocturnal Primates. Duckworth, London. McHenry, H.M. (Eds.), Primate Locomotion: Recent Charles-Dominique, P., 1983. Ecology and social adaptations Advances. Plenum press, New York, pp. 131e144. in didelphid marsupials: Comparison with Eutherians of Nekaris, K.A.I., 2001. Activity budget and positional behav- similar ecology. In: Eisenberg, J.F., Kleiman, D.G. (Eds.), iour of the Mysore slender loris (Loris tardigradus Advances in the Study of Mammalian Behavior: ASM lydekkerianus): implications for ‘‘slow climbing’’ locomo- Special Publication No. 7. American Society of - tion. Folia Primatologica 72, 228e241. ogists, Shippensburg, Pennsylvania, pp. 395e442. Nekaris, K.A.I., 2002. More evidence for visual predation in Crompton, R.H., 1995. Visual predation, habitat structure, and the slender loris. American Journal of Physical Anthropol- the ancestral primate niche. In: Alterman, L., Doyle, G.A., ogy Supplement 33, 117. Izard, M.K. (Eds.), Creatures of the Dark: the Nocturnal Nekaris, K.A.I., Jayewardene, J., 2003. Pilot study and Prosimians. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 11e30. conservation status of the slender loris (Loris tardigradus Dykyj, D., 1980. Locomotion of the in a designed and L. lydekkerianus) in Sri Lanka. Primate Conservation substrate context. American Journal of Physical Anthro- 19, 83e90. pology 52, 577e586. Nekaris, K.A.I., Bearder, S.K., 2006. The lorisiform primates Fitch-Snyder, H., Schulze, H., 2001. Management of lorises in of mainland and Asia: diversity shrouded in captivity: a husbandry manual for Asian lorisines. Zoolog- darkness. In: Bearder, S.K., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K., ical Society of San Diego: Center for Reproduction of Panger, M., Campbell, C. (Eds.), Primates in Perspective. Endangered Species Press. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2006, in press. Fox, R.C., 1993. The primitive dental formula of the Nekaris, K.A.I., Rasmussen, D.T., 2003. Diet of the Carpolestidae (, Mammalia) and its phy- Slender Loris. International Journal of 24 (1), logenetic implications. Journal of Vertebrate 33e46. 13 (4), 516e524. Nekaris, K.A.I., Stevens, N.J., 2005. All lorises are not slow: Garber, P., 1980. Locomotor behavior and feeding ecology of rapid arboreal locomotion in the newly recognised red the Panamanian tamarin (Saguinus Oedipus geoffroyi, slender loris (Loris tardigradus tardigradus) of southwestern Callithrichidae, Primates). International Journal of Prima- Sri Lanka. American Journal of Physical Anthropology tology 1, 185e201. Suppl. 40, 156. 300 K.A.I. Nekaris / Journal of Human Evolution 49 (2005) 289e300

Oates, J.F., 1984. The niche of the potto, Perodicticus potto. Creatures of the Dark: the Nocturnal Prosimians. Plenum International Journal of Primatology 5 (1), 51e61. Press, New York, pp. 221e249. Osman Hill, W.C., 1953. Primates. and Schwenk, K., 2000. Feeding in lepidosaurs. In: Schwenk, K. . I. Strepsirrhini. Edinburgh University Press, (Ed.), Feeding: Form, Function and Evolution in Tetrapod Edinburgh. Vertebrates. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 175e292. Perkin, A.W., 2001. A field study on the conservation status Smith, G.E., 1924. The Evolution of Man. Oxford University and species diversity of galagos in the West Kilombero Press, London. Scarp Forest Reserve. In: Doody, K.Z., Howell, K.M., Soligo, C., Mu¨ ller, A.E., 1999. Nails and claws in primate Fanning, E. (Eds.), West Kilombero Scarp Forest Reserve - evolution. Journal of Human Evolution 36, 97e114. Zoological Report. MEMA, Tanzania, pp. 149e159. Streicher, U., 2004. Aspects of the Ecology and Conservation of Pimley, E.R.C., 2002. The behavioural ecology and of the Pygmy Loris Nycticebus pygmaeus in Vietnam. Ludwig- pottos (Perodicticus potto) and Allen’s bushbaby (Galago Maximilians Universita¨ t, Mu¨ nchen, Germany: Inaugural- alleni). Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Cambridge. Dissertation. Rasmussen, D.T., 1990. Primate origins: Lessons from a neo- Sussman, R.W., 1991. Primate origins and the evolution of tropical marsupial. American Journal of Primatology 22, angiosperms. American Journal of Primatology 23, 209e 263e277. 223. Rasmussen, D.T., Nekaris, K.A.I., 1998. The Evolutionary Sussman, R.W., 1995. How primates created the rainforest and History of the Lorisiform Primates. Folia Primatologica 69 vice versa. In: Alterman, L., Doyle, G.A., Izard, M.K. (Suppl. 1), 250e287. (Eds.), Creatures of the Dark: the Nocturnal Prosimians. Ross, C., 1995. Allometric and functional influences on primate Plenum Press, New York, pp. 1e10. orbit orientation and the origins of the Anthropoidea. Sussman, R.W., Raven, P.H., 1978. Pollination by and Journal of Human Evolution 29, 201e227. marsupials: An archaic co-evolutionary system. Science 200, Ross, C., 1996. Adaptive explanation for the origins of the 731e736. Anthropoidea (Primates). American Journal of Primatology Tan, C.L., Drake, J.H., 2001. Evidence of tree gouging and 40, 205e230. exudate eating in pygmy slow lorises (Nycticebus pygmaeus). Sargis, E.J., 2001. The grasping behaviour, locomotion and Folia Primatologica 72, 37e39. substrate use of the tree shrews Tupaia minor and T. tana Walker, A., 1969. The locomotion of the lorises with special (Mammalia, Scandentia). Journal of Zoology 253, 10e42. reference to the potto. East African Journal of Wildlife 7, 1e5. Schulze, H., Meier, B., 1995. Behaviour of captive Loris Wiens, F., 2002. Behaviour and ecology of wild slow lorises tardigradus nordicus: a qualitative description including (Nycticebus coucang): Social organisation, infant care some information about morphological bases of behaviour. system and diet. Ph.D. Dissertation, Bayreuth University. In: Alterman, L., Doyle, G.A., Izard, M.K. (Eds.), Wood Jones, F., 1916. Arboreal Man. Arnold, London.