Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Clinical Neurophysiology

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph

Invited Review The (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing in ageing and different clinical conditions ⇑ R. Näätänen a,b,c, , T. Kujala c,d, C. Escera e,f, T. Baldeweg g,h, K. Kreegipuu a, S. Carlson i,j,k, C. Ponton l a Institute of , University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia b Center of Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), University of Aarhus, Aarhus, Denmark c Cognitive Research Unit, Cognitive Science, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland d Cicero Learning, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland e Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior (IR3C), University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain f Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain g Institute of Child Health, University College London, London, UK h Great Ormond Street Hospital for Sick Children NHS Trust, London, UK i Neuroscience Unit, Institute of Biomedicine/Physiology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland j Brain Research Unit, Low Temperature Laboratory, Aalto University School of Science and Technology, Espoo, Finland k Medical School, University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland l Compumedics NeuroScan, Santa Fe, NM, USA article info highlights

Article history:  The mismatch negativity (MMN) indexes different types of central auditory abnormalities in different Accepted 20 September 2011 neuropsychiatric, neurological, and neurodevelopmental disorders. Available online 13 December 2011  The diminished amplitude/prolonged peak latency observed in patients usually indexes decreased audi- tory discrimination. Keywords:  An MMN deficit may also index cognitive and functional decline shared by different disorders irrespec- The mismatch negativity (MMN) tive of their specific aetiology and symptomatology. Central auditory processing MMN deficits index deficient N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor function affecting memory-trace Event-related potentials (ERP)  formation and hence cognition in different disorders. Auditory discrimination Neuropsychiatric disorders abstract Neurological disorders Neurodevelopmental disorders The NMDA-receptor system In this article, we review clinical research using the mismatch negativity (MMN), a change-detection response of the brain elicited even in the absence of attention or behavioural task. In these studies, the MMN was usually elicited by employing occasional frequency, duration or speech-sound changes in repetitive background stimulation while the patient was reading or watching videos. It was found that in a large number of different neuropsychiatric, neurological and neurodevelopmental disorders, as well as in normal ageing, the MMN amplitude was attenuated and peak latency prolonged. Besides indexing decreased discrimination accuracy, these effects may also reflect, depending on the specific stimulus paradigm used, decreased sensory-memory duration, abnormal perception or attention control or, most importantly, cognitive decline. In fact, MMN deficiency appears to index cognitive decline irrespective of the specific symptomatologies and aetiologies of the different disorders involved. Ó 2011 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction ...... 425 2. The MMN as an index of decreased auditory discrimination accuracy (Table 1)...... 431 3. The MMN as an index of shortened sensory-memory duration (Table 2)...... 433

⇑ Corresponding author at: Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Cognitive Science, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland. Tel.: +358 40 501 5740; fax: +358 9 191 29450. E-mail address: risto.naatanen@helsinki.fi (R. Näätänen).

1388-2457/$36.00 Ó 2011 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.020 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 425

4. The MMN as an index of abnormal auditory perception ...... 434 5. The MMN as an index of increased backward masking (Table 3)...... 434 6. The MMN as an index of abnormal involuntary attention switching: either too weak (Table 4) or too strong (Table 5)...... 434 7. The MMN attenuation caused by cerebral grey-matter loss or other structural change (Table 6)...... 435 8. The MMN as an index of pathological brain excitation/excitability (Table 7)...... 437 9. The MMN as an index of cognitive and functional deterioration (Table 8)...... 437 10. The MMN as an index of the level of consciousness (Table 9)...... 438 11. The MMN as an index of the progression/severity of the illness (Table 10) ...... 438 12. The MMN in predicting illness course/drug response (prognosis) (Table 11) ...... 439 13. The MMN as an index of genetic disposition to different pathologies (Table 12)...... 440 14. The MMN as an index of central auditory processing improvement or recovery (Table 13) ...... 441 15. Concluding discussion ...... 443 Acknowledgements ...... 444 References ...... 444

1. Introduction tion (or regularity-violation) response, the mismatch negativity (MMN; Näätänen et al., 1978; Näätänen and Michie, 1979) and Interestingly, central auditory processing is affected in a large its magnetoencephalographic (MEG) equivalent, the MMNm (Hari number of different clinical conditions with very different aetiolo- et al., 1984; Sams et al., 1985b). The MMN can be reliably (Pekko- gies and symptoms such as schizophrenia, dyslexia, stroke, specific nen et al., 1995a,b; Escera and Grau, 1996; Tervaniemi et al., 1999; language impairment (SLI), multiple sclerosis (MS), amyotrophic Escera et al., 2000b) recorded even in the absence of the subject or lateral sclerosis (ALS), epilepsy and autism, suggesting some patient’s attention or behavioural task, e.g., in sleeping infants shared aetiology in these different abnormalities (see also Gottes- (Ruusuvirta et al., 2009), stroke patients even at a very early post man and Gould, 2003; Bishop, 2009; Dolan et al., 1993; Hugdahl stroke-onset period (Ilvonen et al., 2001, 2004) and in comatose and Calhoun, 2010; Reite et al., 2009). These effects on central (Kane et al., 1993, 1996; Fischer et al., 1999; Fischer and Luauté, auditory processing in different clinical conditions and in ageing 2005) and persistent-vegetative-state (PVS) patients (Wijnen can now be objectively evaluated, and hence their degree of simi- et al., 2007). larity determined, by using the electrophysiological change-detec-

Fig. 1. Grand average difference waveforms (9 subjects) for changes in duration, frequency, and perceived sound-source location for six different magnitudes of deviance at electrode Fz and referenced to the mean of the two mastoid electrodes (upper panel). The same waveforms referenced to the nose electrode and shown from Fz and the right mastoid (RM); lower panel. Sound onset is always at 0 ms (From Pakarinen et al., 2007). 426 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

The auditory MMN response is mainly generated by a change- modality. By contrast, the sMMN appears to have, similar to the detection process occurring bilaterally in auditory cortices, in auditory MMN, sensory-specific (somatosensory cortex) and fron- which the current auditory input is found to differ from the repre- tal subcomponents (Restuccia et al., 2009; Spackman et al., sentation of the preceding auditory events, including regularities 2010), consistent with its fronto-central, contralaterally predomi- governing consecutive stimulus events (for reviews, see Näätänen nant scalp distribution (Wei et al., 2002). Currently, it is not clear et al., 2001, 2007, 2010, 2011b; Duncan et al., 2009; Winkler, whether the auditory and somatosensory modalities share the 2007). For an illustration, see Fig. 1. In subjects performing some frontal activation (generating the frontal MMN component) proba- primary, e.g., visual, task, this preconscious auditory-cortex bly serving attention switch to stimulus change (Näätänen, 1992; change-detection process reaches conscious perception by activat- Näätänen et al., 2002; Schröger, 1997). In addition, there is also ing, with a brief delay (Rinne et al., 2000; see also Rinne et al., 2005, an oMMN, with a long peak latency (about 500–600 ms) and parie- 2006), a right frontal-cortex process which, in turn, initiates further tally predominant scalp distribution (Krauel et al., 1999). cerebral processes which may lead to conscious change detection The MMN can also be recorded in different animals [in monkey (Näätänen, 1990, 1992; Näätänen et al., 2011b; Alain et al., 1998; (Javitt et al., 1992); cat (Csépe et al., 1987, 1988, 1989; Pincze et al., Berti and Schröger, 2001; Deouell et al., 2007; Giard et al., 1990; 2001, 2002); rabbit (Astikainen et al., 2001); rat (Astikainen et al., Schröger, 1996; Escera et al., 2000a). Hence, the MMN is composed 2006; Ruusuvirta et al., 1998, 2007; Roger et al., 2009; Tikhonravov of overlapping contributions from auditory- and frontal-cortex pro- et al., 2008, 2010); guinea pig (Kraus et al., 1994); and mouse cesses (the supratemporal and frontal MMN subcomponents, (Umbricht et al., 2005; Ehrlichman et al., 2008, 2009)]. respectively) (Giard et al., 1990). These two subcomponents of the The MMN enables one to determine discrimination accuracy, MMN can be inferred from the data presented in Fig. 2 showing usually with a good correspondence with behavioural discrimina- how ethanol selectively attenuates the MMN amplitude recorded tion (Sams et al., 1985a; Amenedo and Escera, 2000; Gottselig over the frontal cortex, whereas the polarity-reversed MMN re- et al., 2004; Lang et al., 1990; Näätänen et al., 1993, 2007; Leitman corded at the mastoids (with a nose reference) is unaffected. This et al., 2010; Kujala and Näätänen, 2010; Tremblay et al., 1998), sep- data pattern suggests that the frontally recorded MMN is composed arately for each auditory dimension (such as frequency, intensity of contributions from both the auditory and frontal cortices, and duration) as well as for the different speech sounds (Kraus whereas the mastoid ‘MMN’ gets a contribution from the audi- et al., 1996), with the MMN vanishing at about the behavioural dis- tory-cortex MMN generator only. Intracranial recordings in humans crimination threshold (Sams et al., 1985a; Winkler and Näätänen, also support both auditory- and frontal-cortex MMN generators 1994; Winkler et al., 1993). Therefore, it permits one to form objec- (Halgren et al., 1995a,b, 1998; Baudena et al., 1995; Kropotov tive deterioration profiles covering all important auditory dimen- et al., 1995, 2000; Liasis et al., 1999, 2000a,b; Rosburg et al., 2005, sions in different patient groups (Näätänen et al., 2004; Kujala 2007). et al., 2005a, 2006, 2007, 2010; Pakarinen et al., 2007, 2009, 2010). The MMNm, in turn, selectively reflects the temporal-lobe com- In addition, these MMN-based objective tests can be extended to ponent of the MMN because the MEG is insensitive to the radially all aspects of short-term auditory , too, such as its oriented frontal generators (Hämäläinen et al., 1993) of the MMN. Therefore, the MMNm is particularly well suited for detecting cen- tral auditory processing deficits specific for the temporal lobes. Consequently, the combined use of the (EEG) and MEG recordings helps one to separately determine to what extent the temporal and frontal MMN components are im- paired (Hämäläinen et al., 1993; Näätänen, 1992). Further, these two components have their functional magnetic resonance imag- ing (fMRI) (Molholm et al., 2005; Celsis et al., 1999; Opitz et al., 2002; Schall et al., 2003), positron emission tomography (PET) (Tervaniemi et al., 2000; Dittmann-Balcar et al., 2001; Müller et al., 2002) and optical-imaging (OI) (Rinne et al., 1999; Tse and Penney, 2008) equivalents as well. Moreover, there is event-related potential (ERP; Paavilainen et al., 1991; Doeller et al., 2003; Frodl- Bauch et al., 1997; Escera et al., 2002), MEG (Levänen et al., 1993, 1996; Rosburg, 2003) and fMRI evidence (Molholm et al., 2005) for the attribute-specific organisation of the supratemporal MMN generator. Moreover, the MMN has its equivalents in other sensory modal- ities, too [the visual MMN; vMMN (Alho et al., 1992; Amenedo et al., 2007; Astikainen and Hietanen, 2009; Tales et al., 1999, 2002a,b, 2008; Tales and Butler, 2006; Maekawa et al., 2005, 2009; Berti and Schröger, 2004, 2006; Stagg et al., 2004; Stefanics et al., 2011; Heslenfeld, 2003; Pazo-Alvarez et al., 2003, 2004; Fro- yen et al., 2010; Iijima et al., 1996; Kenemans et al., 2003; Kimura et al., 2009; Czigler, 2007; Czigler and Csibra, 1990, 1992; Czigler et al., 2002, 2004, 2006a,b; Czigler and Pató, 2009; Nordby et al., 1996; Wei et al., 2002; Woods et al., 1992); the somatosensory MMN; sMMN (Kekoni et al., 1997; Shinozaki et al., 1998; Akatsuka et al., 2005; Kida et al., 2001; Spackman et al., 2007, 2010; Astikai- Fig. 2. The differential effects of ethanol on MMN at the frontal (Fz) and left and nen et al., 2001; Näätänen, 2009); and the olfactory MMN; oMMN right mastoid (LM and RM, respectively) electrode positions. MMN was reduced at the frontal electrode but the polarity-inverted MMN recorded at the mastoids, (Krauel et al., 1999; Pause and Krauel, 2000)]. The vMMN has a suggesting the supratemporal MMN subcomponent, was not affected by ethanol parieto-occipital scalp distribution but there is no convincing evi- (0.55 g/kg) whereas the frontal subcomponent was affected by ethanol. Adapted dence for a frontal component analogous to that in the auditory from Jääskeläinen et al. (1996b). Table 1 The auditory MMN (MMNm) provides an objective index of affected auditory discrimination in:

– ADHD (Alexandrov et al., 2003; Barry et al., 2003; Gumenyuk et al., 2005; Huttunen et al., 2007; Huttunen-Scott et al., 2008; Kemner et al., 1996; Kilpeläinen et al., 1999; Oades et al., 1996; Rothenberger, 1995; Rothenberger et al., 2000; Satterfield et al., 1988; Sawada et al., 2010; Wild-Wall et al., 2005; Winsberg et al., 1993) – Ageing (Alain and Woods, 1999; Alain et al., 2004; Amenedo and Diaz, 1998; Bellis et al., 2000; Bertoli et al., 2005; Cooper et al., 2006; Czigler et al., 1992; Fabiani et al., 2006; Gaeta et al., 1998, 1999, 2001, 2002; Gunter et al., 1996; Horváth et al., 2007; Iijima et al., 1996; Ikeda et al., 2004; Jääskeläinen et al., 1999a; Karayanidis et al., 1995; Kiang et al., 2007, 2009; Kisley et al., 2004a,b, 2005; Kok, 2000; Mager et al., 2005; Osawa et al., 1996; Pekkonen, 2000; Pekkonen et al., 1995a, 1996a, 2005; Schiff et al., 2008; Schroeder et al., 1995; Verleger et al., 1991; Woods, 1992; Woods and Clayworth, 1986) – Alcohol intoxication, acute (Ahveninen et al., 2000a; Jääskeläinen et al., 1995, 1996a,b, 1998, 1999a,b; Kähkönen et al., 2001, 2004) Alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 2000a,b; Fein et al., 2004; Grau et al., 2001; Holguin et al., 1998; Kathmann et al., 1995; Marco-Pallares et al., 2007; Pekkonen et al., 1998; Polo et al., 2003; Realmuto et al., 1993; Rodriguez et al., 1998; van der Stelt and Belger, 2007; Zhang et al., 2001) – Alzheimer’s disease and dementia (Engeland et al., 2002; Gaeta et al., 1999; Katada et al., 2004; Kazmerski et al., 1997; Missioner et al., 1999; Pekkonen, 2000; Pekkonen et al., 1994, 1996b, 1999, 2001a,b; Riekkinen et al., 1997; Schroeder et al., 1995; Yokoyama et al., 1995) – Amusia(Congenital) (Moreau et al., 2009; Peretz et al., 2009) – Amusia (Caused by Stroke) (Kohlmetz et al., 2001; Särkämö et al., 2010b) Amyotrophic Lateral Schlerosis (ALS) (Gil et al., 1993; Hanagasi et al., 2002; Pekkonen et al., 2004; Raggi et al., 2008)

Anaesthesis and sedation (Csepe et al., 1989; Heinke and Koelsch, 2005; Heinke et al., 2004; Koelsch et al., 2006; Astikainen et al., 2006; Yppärilä et al., 2002; Simpson et al., 2002; van Hooff et al., 1995, 1997) 424–458 (2012) 123 Neurophysiology Clinical / al. et Näätänen R. – Anhedonia (Giese-Davis et al., 1993) – Aphasia and stroke (Aaltonen et al., 1993; Alain et al., 1998; Auther et al., 2000; Becker and Reinvang, 2007; Csépe et al., 2001; Ilvonen et al., 2003, 2004; Jacobs and Schneider, 2003; Kohlmetz et al., 2001; Peach et al., 1992, 1993, 1994; Pettigrew et al., 2004a, 2005; Särkämö et al. 2010a,b; Wertz et al., 1998) – Asperger syndrome (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003a, 2005; Korpilahti, 1995; Korpilahti et al., 2007; Kujala et al., 2005a, 2007, 2010; Lepistö et al., 2006, 2007) – Autism (Bomba and Pang, 2004; Ceponiene et al., 2003; Dunn et al., 2008; Ferri et al., 2003; Gomot et al., 2002; Kasai et al., 2005; Kemner et al., 1995; Kuhl et al., 2005; Lepistö et al., 2005, 2008; Näätänen and Kujala, 2011; Oram Cardy et al., 2005a,b; Roberts et al., 2008, 2011; Seri et al., 1999, 2007; Siegal and Blades, 2003) – Bipolar disorder (Hall et al., 2007, 2009; Takei et al., 2010) – Bipolar II disorder (Andersson et al., 2008) – Blindness (early) (Alho et al., 1993; Kujala et al., 1995a,b, 1997, 2000b) 999, 2000, 2001; Kohlmetz et al., 2001; Kotchoubey, 2007; – BrainKotchoubey lesions (Alain et et al. al., 2003, 1998; 2005 Alho; Mäkelä et al., 1994; et al., Auther 1998b; et Neumann al., 2000; andDeouell Kotchoubey, et al., 2000a,b; 2004; Hämäläinen Polo et al., 2002; et al., Reinvang 2007; Jacobs et al., and 2000; Schneider, Tarkka 2003; et al., Kaipio 2011; etWoods al., 1 and Knight, 1986); (see also Aphasia and Stroke) – Brainstem auditory processing syndrome (Kraus et al., 1993a) – CATCH-22 syndrome (Baker et al., 2005; Cheour et al., 1998a; Ceponiene et al., 2002) – Central auditory processing disorder (CAPD) (Bamiou et al., 2000) – Cerebellar degeneration (Moberget et al., 2008) – Childhood cancer and brain radiation (Järvelä et al., 2011; Lähteenmäki, 1999) – Chronic pain (Dick et al., 2003) – Chronic primary insomnia (Wang et al., 2001) – Cleft palate (different forms of) (Ceponiene et al., 2000, 2002; Cheour et al., 1998a, 1999) – Closed-head injury (Kaipio et al., 1999, 2000, 2001; Polo et al., 2002) – Cochlear-implant (CI) users (Groenen et al., 1996; Kelly et al., 2005; Kileny et al., 1997; Koelsch et al., 2004; Kraus et al., 1993b; Lonka et al., 2004; Nager et al., 2007; Ponton et al., 1996, 2000, 2009; Ponton and Don, 1995, 2003; Ponton and Eggermont, 2007; Roman et al., 2005; Salo et al., 2002; Sandmann et al., 2010; Wable et al., 2000) – Coma (Daltrozzo et al., 2007; Fischer et al., 1999, 2000, 2004, 2006a,b, 2008; Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Guérit et al., 1999; Kane et al., 1993, 1996, 2000; Kotchoubey et al., 2003; Laureys et al., 2005; Luauté et al., 2005; Morlet et al., 2000; Naccache et al., 2005; van der Stelt and van Boxtel, 2008; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2008) – Conduct disorder (Rothenberger et al., 2000) – Craniosynosthosis (Huotilainen et al., 2008) – Deafness to musical dissonance ( Acquired) (Brattico et al., 2003) – Dementia (Schroeder et al., 1995; Yokoyama et al., 1995; Osawa et al., 1996) – Depression (He et al., 2010; Iv et al., 2010; Kähkönen et al., 2007; Lepistö et al., 2004; Ogura et al., 1991, 1993; Takei et al., 2009) (continued on next page) 427 428 Table 1 (continued)

– Developmental dysphasia (Holopainen et al., 1997, 1998; Korpilahti and Lang, 1994) – Diabetes mellitus (late phase) (Vanhanen et al., 1996) – Distractibility (Escera et al., 2000a; Gumenyuk et al., 2004; Kaipio et al., 2000, 2001; Kilpeläinen et al., 1999; Mager et al., 2005) – Down’s syndrome (Diaz and Zurron, 1995; Lalo et al., 2005) – Drugs –Antipsychotics/Neuroleptics: – Clozapine (Horton et al., 2010; Schall et al., 1999; Umbricht et al., 1998) – Haloperidol (Kähkönen and Ahveninen, 2002; Kähkönen et al., 2001, 2002; Pekkonen et al., 2002) – Olanzapine (Korostenskaja et al., 2005) – Risperidone (Umbricht et al., 1999) – Alcohol: –(Hirvonen et al., 2000; Jääskeläinen et al., 1995, 1996a,b, 1998, 1999a; Kähkönen et al., 2001,2004,2005b; Kenemans et al., 2010) – Acetylcholine:

Tetrahydroaminoacridine (THA, Tacrine) (Engeland et al., 2002; Riekkinen et al., 1997) 424–458 (2012) 123 Neurophysiology Clinical / al. et Näätänen R. – (a) Muscarinic receptor antagonist: – Glycopyrrolate (Pekkonen et al., 2001a) – Scopolamine (Pekkonen et al., 2001a) – (b) Nicotinic receptor agonists: – Nicotine: (Baldeweg et al., 2006; Dulude et al., 2010; Engeland et al., 2002; Fisher et al., 2010; Harkrider and Hedrick, 2005; Inami et al., 2005, 2007; Knott et al., 2009; Martin et al., 2009; Pritchard et al., 2004) – AZD3480 (Dunbar et al., 2007) – Benzodiazepines: –(Kivisaari et al., 2007; Murakami et al., 2002) – Triatzolam (Nakagome et al., 1998) – Lorazepam (Rosburg et al., 2004) – GABA agonists/antagonists: – Bicuculline (Javitt et al., 1996) – Flumazenil (Smolnik et al., 1998) – Propofol (Heinke et al., 2004; Koelsch et al., 2006; Simpson et al., 2002) – Monoamines – Review: (Kähkönen and Ahveninen, 2002) – Adrenergic drugs: – ClonidineAtipamezole (Duncan (Mervaala and Kaye,et al., 1987;1993) Hansenne et al., 2003) – Noradrenergic agonist S 12024-4 (Missioner et al., 1999) – Histamine: – Antihistamines: Chlorpheniramine (Serra et al., 1996) – Dopamine: – Apomorphine (Hansenne et al., 2003) – Methamphetamine (Hosák et al., 2008) – Methylphenidate (Winsberg et al., 1993, 1997; Sawada et al., 2010; Verbaten et al., 1994) – Bromocriptine (Leung et al., 2007) – Domperidone (Leung et al., 2007) – Serotonin: – Serotonin (Kähkönen et al., 2005a; Oranje et al., 2008) – Acute tryptophan depletion (Kähkönen and Ahveninen, 2002) – Dimethyltryptamine (Heekeren et al., 2008) – Escitalopram (Oranje et al., 2008; Wienberg et al., 2010) – Psilocybin (Umbricht et al., 2003) – Neuropeptides/Hormones: – Vasopressin (Born et al., 1986, 1987a, 1998) – Oxytocin (Born et al., 1987a) – Cholecystokinin (Schreiber et al., 1995) – Desacetyl-alpha-MSH (Smolnik et al., 1999) – Hydrocortisone (Born et al., 1987a) – ACTH-related neuropeptides (Born et al., 1987b; Smolnik et al., 1999; Pietrowsky et al., 1990) – NMDA/Glutamate: – Ketamine (Ehrlichman et al., 2008; Heekeren et al., 2008; Javitt et al., 2000; Kreitschmann-Andermahr et al., 2001; Umbricht et al., 2002) – MK-801 (Javitt et al., 1996; Tikhonravov et al., 2010) – Phencyclidine (PCP) (Javitt et al., 1996) – Memantine (Korostenskaja et al., 2007) – Nitrous oxide (Pang and Fowler, 1999) – Glycine (Leung et al., 2008) – N-acetyl-cysteine (NAC; Lavoie et al., 2007) – Opiods: – Naltrexone (Jääskeläinen et al., 1998) – Opioid Dependence (Kivisaari et al., 2007; Näätänen, 2001) – Other/alternative therapies: – Hiba odour (Hiruma et al., 2002) – Purine nucleotides: – Adenosine (Hirvonen et al., 2000) – Caffeine (Rosburg et al., 2004) – Dyslexia (Ahissar, 2007; Ahissar et al., 2006; Alonso-Búa et al., 2006; Baldeweg et al., 1999; Bishop, 2007a,b; Blitz et al., 2007; Bonte et al., 2005, 2007; Bradlow et al., 1999; Bruder et al., 2011a,b; Corbera et al., 2006; Csépe,

2002; Czamara et al., 2011; Fisher et al., 2006; Froyen et al., 2008, 2009, 2010; Guttorm et al., 2005, 2001; Hämäläinen et al., 2008; Heim et al., 2000; Huttunen et al., 2007; Huttunen-Scott et al., 2008; Jansson-Verkasalo 424–458 (2012) 123 Neurophysiology Clinical / al. et Näätänen R. et al., 2010; Kujala, 2007; Kujala and Näätänen, 2001; Kujala et al., 2000a, 2001, 2003, 2006; Kurtzberg et al., 1995; Lachmann et al., 2005; Leppänen and Lyytinen, 1997; Leppänen et al., 1997, 1999, 2002, 2004, 2010; Lovio et al., 2010; Lyytinen et al., 2001, 2004, 2006, 2009; Maurer et al., 2003, 2009; Paul et al., 2006a,b; Pihko et al., 1999, 2008; Renvall and Hari, 2003; Richardson et al., 2003; Roeske et al., 2011; Schulte-Körne and Bruder, 2010; Schulte-Körne et al., 1998, 1999, 2001; Sebastian and Yasin, 2008; Shankarnarayan and Maruthy, 2007; Sharma et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2009; Stoodley et al., 2006; van Leeuwen et al., 2006) – Dysthymia (Giese-Davis et al., 1993) – Electromagnetic fields (Kwon et al., 2009, 2010) – Epilepsy (Boatman et al., 2008; Borghetti et al., 2007; Duman et al., 2008; Gene-Cos et al., 2005; Korostenskaja et al., 2010b; Liasis et al., 2000; Liasis et al., 2001; Liasis et al., 2006; Lin et al., 2007; Metz-Lutz and Philippini, 2006; Miyajima et al., 2011; Rosburg et al., 2005) – Frontal-lobe injury (Alho et al., 1994; Woods and Knight, 1986) – Herpes Simplex Encephalitis (Mäkelä et al., 1998a) – HIVHippocampus-Amygdala(Schroeder et al., 1994 Partial) Temporal-Lobe Resection (Hämäläinen et al., 2007) – Huntington’s disease (pathological enhancement of discrimination) (Beste et al., 2008) – Hyperkinetic disorder (Rothenberger, 1995) – Hypnosis (Jamieson et al., 2005; Kallio et al., 1999) – Insomnia (Wang et al., 2001) – Intellectual disability (Ikeda et al., 2004; Nakagawa et al., 2002; Kaga et al., 1999) – Introvert/extrovert personality (Sasaki et al., 2000) – Landau–Kleffner syndrome (Honbolygó et al., 2006; Metz-Lutz and Philippini, 2006) – Language-learning impairment (LLI) (Benasich and Tallal, 2002; Benasich et al., 2006; Bradlow et al., 1999; Choudhury and Benasich, 2011; Kraus et al., 1996; Kujala, 2007; Marler et al., 2002; Pihko et al., 2007; Shafer et al., 2005; Uwer et al., 2002; Weber et al., 2005) – Late talkers (Grossheinrich et al., 2010) – Learning-disabled children (Banai and Ahissar, 2005; Bradlow et al., 1999; Kraus et al., 1996) – Left sylvian infarct in neonate (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2004) – Locked-in patients (Ragazzoni et al., 2000) – Meditation (Srinivasan and Baijal, 2007) – Mental work load (Kramer et al., 1995) – Mental retardation (Holopainen et al., 1998; Ikeda et al., 2004; Kaga et al., 1999; Nakagawa et al., 2002; Yokoyama et al., 1995; Zurrón and Díaz, 1997) – Migraine (de Tommaso et al., 2004; Korostenskaja et al., 2010a; Valeriani et al., 2008) – Multiple schlerosis (MS) (Gil et al., 1993; Jung et al., 2006; Santos et al., 2006) – Narcolepsy (Naumann et al., 2001) ) – Neglect (Hemifield inattention) and extinction (Deouell et al., 2000a,b; Tarkka et al., 2011 – Noise and distraction (Bertoli et al., 2005; Gumenyuk et al., 2004; Mikkola et al., 2010; Shtyrov et al., 1998, 1999; Simoens et al., 2007) – Noise, different types of (Kozou et al., 2005) – Noise, occupational (Mäntysalo and Salmi, 1989) – Noise, occupational, long-term exposure (Brattico et al., 2005; Kujala and Brattico, 2009; Kujala et al., 2004) – Obsessive–compulsive disorder (Towey et al., 1994; Oades et al., 1996) – Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS) (Gosselin et al., 2006) – Opioid dependence (Kivisaari et al., 2007) – Parkinson’s disease (Brønnick et al., 2010; Karayanidis et al., 1995; Pekkonen et al., 1995c; Vieregger et al., 1994) 429 – Perinatal asphyxia (Leipälä et al., 2011) (continued on next page) 430

Table 1 (continued)

– Perinatal intracerebral hemorrhage (Leipälä et al., 2011) – Persistent vegetative state (Boly et al., 2011; Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Kotchoubey, 2007; Kotchoubey et al., 2003, 2007, 2008; Laureys et al., 2005; Wijnen et al., 2007; Zarza-Lucianez et al., 2007) – Personality differences (Matsubayashi et al., 2008; Franken et al., 2005) – Phonological deficit (Blitz et al., 2007) – Plagiocephaly (Huotilainen et al., 2008) .Nääe ta./Ciia erpyilg 2 21)424–458 (2012) 123 Neurophysiology Clinical / al. et Näätänen R. – Post-traumatic stress syndrome (PTSS) (Cornwell et al., 2007; Ge et al., 2011; Menning et al., 2008; Morgan and Grillon, 1999) – Psychosocial stress (Simoens et al., 2007) – Prematurely born (with very low birth weight, VLBW) infants (Bisiachi et al., 2009; Cheour-Luhtanen et al., 1996; Fellman and Huotilainen, 2006; Fellman et al., 2004; Gomot et al., 2007; Holst et al., 2005; Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2004, 2010; Leipälä et al., 2011; Mento et al., 2010; Mikkola et al., 2007, 2010) – REM-sleep deprivation (Zerouali et al., 2010) – Schizophrenia (Ahveninen et al., 2006; Baldeweg et al., 2002, 2004; Banati and Hickie, 2009; Bodatsch et al., 2011; Braff and Light, 2004; Brockhaus-Dumke et al., 2005; Catts et al., 1995; Davalos et al., 2003, 2005; Devrim-Ücok et al., 2008; Ehrlichman et al., 2008, 2009; Fisher et al., 2008, 2011; Grzella et al., 2001; Hermens et al., 2010; Hirayasu et al., 1998, 2000, 2001; Inami et al., 2007; Jahshan et al., 2011; Javitt, 1993, 2000, 2009a,b; Javitt et al., 1996, 1998, 1999, 2000, 2008; Jessen et al., 2001; Kasai et al., 2001, 2002a,b,c, 2003a,b; Kathmann et al., 1995; Kaur et al., 2011; Kawakubo et al., 2006, 2007; Kiang et al., 2007, 2009; Kircher et al., 2004; Korostenskaja et al., 2005; Kreitschmann-Andermahr et al., 1999, 2001; Lavoie et al., 2007; Leitman et al., 2010; Light and Braff, 2005a,b; Luck et al., 2011; Magno et al., 2008; Matthews et al., 2007; Michie, 2001; Michie et al., 2002; Minami and Kirino, 2005; Morlet et al., 2000; Näätänen and Kähkönen, 2009; Niznikiewicz et al., 2009; Oades et al., 2006; Oknina et al., 2005; Park et al., 2002; Potts et al., 1998; Price et al., 2006; Rasser et al., 2011; Rissling et al., 2010; Salisbury et al., 2002, 2007; Sato et al., 1998, 2002; Schall et al., 1998, 1999, 2003; Schreiber et al., 1992; Shelley et al., 1991; Shin et al., 2009; Shinozaki et al., 2002; Stone et al., 2010; Thönnesen et al., 2008; Todd et al., 2008; Todd et al., in press; Todd et al., 2003, 2008; Turetsky et al., 2007, 2009; Umbricht et al., 1998, 1999, 2000, 2002, 2003, 2006; van der Stelt and Belger, 2007; Verbaten and van Engeland, 1995; Wible et al., 2001; Wynn et al., 2010; Yamasue et al., 2004; Youn et al., 2003; for a meta-analysis of 32 studies, see Umbricht and Krljes, 2005) – Serotonin transporter gene variants, individuals with (Sysoeva et al., 2009) – Sensorineural loss (Oates et al., 2002; Stapells, 2002) – Sleep (Atienza et al., 1997, 2000, 2002; Atienza and Cantero, 2001; Campbell et al., 1991; Cheour et al. 2000,2002; Martynova et al., 2003; Nashida et al., 2000; Nittono et al., 2001; Ruby et al., 2008; Sallinen et al., 1994, 1996; Sculthorpe et al., 2009) – Sleep deprivation (Gumenyuk et al., 2010; Raz et al., 2001; Sallinen and Lyytinen, 1997; Salmi et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2001; Zerouali et al., 2010) – Socially withdrawn children (Bar-Haim et al., 2003) – Somatisation disorder (James et al., 1989) – Specific language impairment (SLI) (Barry et al., 2008; Bishop and McArthur, 2004; Friedrich et al., 2004; Korpilahti, 1995; Marler et al., 2002; Pihko et al., 2008; Rinker et al., 2007; Shafer et al., 2007; Uwer and von Suchodoletz, 2000; Uwer et al., 2002; Weber et al., 2005) – Stroke (see Aphasia) – Stuttering (Corbera et al., 2005; Wu et al., 1997) – Temporal-parietal brain lesions (Alain et al., 1998) – Thalamic infarction (Mäkelä et al., 1998b) – Tourette syndrome (TS) (Oades et al., 1996; Rothenberger et al., 2000; van Woerkom et al., 1988, 1994) – Tune deafness (Braun et al., 2008) – Velo-cardio-facial (DiGeorge) syndrome (Baker et al., 2005) R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 431

Table 2 THE MMN shows abnormally short auditory sensory-memory duration in:

– Alzheimer’s disease (Pekkonen et al., 1994) – Infants and children with cleft palate and CATCH-22 syndrome (Ceponiene et al., 2002; Cheour et al., 1997, 1998a, 1999) – Late talkers (Grossheinrich et al., 2010) – Chronic alcoholism (Grau et al., 2001; Marco-Pallares et al., 2007; Polo et al., 1999) – Normal ageing (Cooper et al., 2006; Grau et al., 2001; Jääskeläinen et al., 1999a; Pekkonen et al., 1996a) – Parents of children with specific language impairment (SLI) (Barry et al., 2008)

Table 3 language journals. Almost all these studies report, with only a few The MMN provides an index of increased backward masking (affecting speech exceptions, group-level results. perception) in:

– Chronic alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 1999) 2. The MMN as an index of decreased auditory discrimination – Dyslexia (Kujala et al., 2003) – Specific language impairment (SLI) (Marler et al., 2002) accuracy (Table 1)

In several clinical conditions, the MMN amplitude is attenuated, duration, capacity and accuracy (Pekkonen et al., 1996a; Cooper usually indexing decreased behavioural discrimination accuracy et al., 2006; Polo et al., 1999; Grau et al., 2001). Furthermore, the (Javitt et al., 1998; Rabinowicz et al., 2000; Matthews et al., auditory MMN can even index auditory long-term memory traces 2007). This amplitude reduction was usually found by recording such as the language-specific memory traces, enabling one to cor- the MMN to simple frequency or duration changes in sinusoidal rectly perceive the speech sounds of the mother tongue and other tones, for instance, in schizophrenia (Todd et al., 2003; Javitt familiar languages (Dehaene-Lambertz, 1997; Näätänen, 2001; et al., 2000; Michie et al., 2000; see also Javitt et al., 1998). In Näätänen et al., 1997; Pulvermuller et al., 2004; Shtyrov and Pulver- schizophrenia, the duration MMN, in particular that to duration muller, 2007; Winkler et al., 1999c; Shtyrov and Pulvermüller, increment, was even more affected than that to frequency change 2007). This linguistic MMN subcomponent is usually left-hemi- (Michie, 2001; Michie et al., 2000; for a meta-analysis, see Umb- spherically predominant and generated posteriorly to the bilateral richt and Krljes, 2005). The frequency MMN is attenuated in ampli- MMN subcomponent for mere acoustic change (Näätänen et al., tude in several other clinical groups, too, such as children with 1997; Shestakova et al., 2002). developmental dysphasia (Holopainen et al., 1997, 1998; Kor- The extensive MMN literature of clinical studies can be classi- pilahti and Lang, 1994), dyslexia (Baldeweg et al., 1999; Renvall fied according to the kind of clinically useful information that and Hari, 2003; Kujala et al., 2003, 2006; Sebastian and Yasin, can be obtained by using the MMN. The MMN can index: 2008) and SLI (Mengler et al., 2005; Rinker et al., 2007). In children and adults with dyslexia, the MMN to frequency (1) Auditory discrimination accuracy (which is decreased in a change but not that to duration change was considerably attenu- number of clinical groups and affected by different drugs) ated in amplitude (Baldeweg et al., 1999; Kujala et al., 2006). Fur- (Table 1); thermore, in keeping with this profile of MMN-amplitude (2) shortened sensory-memory duration (and hence possibly reduction, the behavioural frequency but not duration discrimina- decreased general brain plasticity) (Table 2); tion was affected (Baldeweg et al., 1999). In addition, this (3) abnormal auditory perception; frequency-MMN deficit strongly correlated with the severity of (4) increased backward masking (Table 3); the reading problem. Subsequently, Lachmann et al. (2005) (5) abnormal involuntary attention switching, either too weak (Table 4) or too strong (Table 5); (6) cerebral grey-matter loss and other structural changes (Table 6); (7) pathological brain excitation/excitability state (Table 7); MMN (8) cognitive and functional decline (Table 8); MMN (9) the level of consciousness (Table 9); (10) the progression of illness (Table 10); (11) future clinical condition (prognosis) (Table 11); Amplitude (12) genetic disposition to certain disorders (Table 12); and (13) recovery/improvement as a function of time or treatment Time (Table 13). Moreover, recent studies using the vMMN (Iijima et al., 1996; Lorenzo-Lopez et al., 2004; Tales and Butler, 2006; Left Right Tales et al., 2002a,b; Kenemans et al., 2010; Tales and Butler, hemisphere hemisphere 2006; Tales et al., 2002a, 2008; Maekawa et al., 2011; Froyen et al., 2010; Chang et al., 2010; Qiu et al., 2011; Horimoto et al., 2002; Hosák et al., 2008; Kremlácˇek et al., 2008, Ver- 3-5 days baten et al., 1994; Tales and Butler, 2006; Tales et al., 10 days 2008; Fisher et al., 2010; Urban et al., 2008; for a recent 3 months review, see Kimura et al., 2011) and sMMN (Restuccia et al., 2007; Akatsuka et al., 2005, 2007; Näätänen, 2009) Fig. 3. MMN shows how frequency discrimination recovered to normal levels have also provided clinically important results. within 3 months from a stroke onset (averaged data of 8 patients). This MMN recovery was slower for right-ear stimulation (with the main part of the auditory input going to the damaged left hemisphere) (curves on the left) than with left-ear The present review aims at covering all clinical MMN studies in stimulation (curves on the right). During the MMN recording, the patient was the auditory modality published in refereed international English- watching video-films. Adapted from Ilvonen et al. (2003). 432 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Table 4 The MMN shows decreased involuntary attention switching in:

– Alcohol intoxication (Jääskeläinen et al., 1995, 1996a,b, 1999b) – Schizophrenia (Baldeweg et al., 2002; Catts et al., 1995; Matthews et al., 2007; Oades et al., 2006; Sato et al., 2003) – Closed-head injury (Polo et al., 2002) – Frontal-lesion patients (Alho et al., 1994) – Right-hemisphere damage (unilateral neglect) (Deouell et al., 2000a,b) – Children with major depression (Lepistö et al., 2004)

showed that the MMN-attenuation profile involving frequency and see also Näätänen and Kujala, 2011) that in autistic children of consonant–vowel (CV) syllable changes could disentangle different 7–9 years, the MMNm peak latency for tone-frequency and vowel diagnostic subgroups of developmental dyslexia from each other. changes was delayed, in comparison with that of normally devel- Moreover, Shafer et al. (2005) and Uwer et al. (2002) found that oping children of the same age, and, further, that this delay was SLI children had attenuated MMN amplitudes for syllable change. considerably increased if the child also suffered from a delay in lin- Using the new multi-feature MMN ‘optimum’ paradigm (Näätä- guistic development. This might contribute to their speech-under- nen et al., 2004), permitting one to obtain five different MMNs in standing difficulties and hence to their social isolation. In addition, 15 min, Lovio et al. (2010) found a widespread auditory discrimi- in the Asperger syndrome, a condition belonging to the autism nation deficit in children at risk for dyslexia. Their MMNs for con- spectrum, the duration-increment MMN of children of about sonant, vowel, vowel duration and intensity changes of syllables 9 years of age was attenuated in amplitude over the left hemi- were diminished in amplitude. By contrast, Kujala et al. (2006), sphere, whereas it was enhanced over the right hemisphere also using the multi-feature paradigm, found that in adult dyslex- (Lepistö et al., 2006). Moreover, in this syndrome, the MMN ampli- ics, the frequency MMN was attenuated, whereas their location tude was also attenuated for changes in prosody (Kujala et al., MMN was in fact enhanced in amplitude. This shows that not all 2005a). Recently, a shortened MMN peak latency for frequency changes in central auditory processing in dyslexia signify deterio- change and increased MMN amplitudes for duration and gap rated discrimination. However, the MMN data may also suggest changes were found in Asperger adults by Kujala et al. (2007). the presence of cognitive deficiencies associated with dyslexia, at In addition, the MMNs for frequency and duration changes were least deficient phonotactic processing (Bonte et al., 2007). attenuated in amplitude in patients with a left-hemispheric stroke Furthermore, in children with autism, the MMN to duration (Csépe et al., 2001; Aaltonen et al., 1993; Auther et al., 2000; Ilvo- change (Lepistö et al., 2005) and the MMNm to speech-sound nen et al., 2001, 2003; Pettigrew et al., 2005). These two MMNs change (Kasai et al., 2005; for recent corroborating results, see Rob- showed somewhat different time courses of post-stroke recovery, erts et al., 2011) were attenuated in amplitude. In addition, the however (Ilvonen et al., 2003)(Fig. 3). Importantly, the MMN MMN to CV-syllable change was prolonged in peak latency but recovery indexed that of , there being a correla- only in those children who preferred non-speech analogy signals tion between the duration-MMN amplitude and the Boston Diag- to speech (Kuhl et al., 2005). By contrast, the MMN to frequency nostic Aphasia Examination Speech-Comprehension Test from change was enhanced in amplitude (Lepistö et al., 2005; Ferri 10 days to 3 months post-stroke onset (Ilvonen et al., 2003). Subse- et al., 2003) and shortened in peak latency (Gomot et al., 2002) quently, Särkämö et al. (2010b) found that the MMNm recovery in autistic children, consistent with their better-than-average pitch and behavioural discrimination could be expedited with music discrimination and musical abilities (Bonnel et al., 2003; Heaton and speech stimulation. In addition, the MMNm recovery corre- et al., 2001). Very recently, it was found by Roberts et al. (2011;

Fig. 4. Scatterplot of MMN amplitude at Fz versus age, for schizophrenia patients (open circles; dashed regression line) and normal control subjects (solid circles; solid regression line) (From Kiang et al., 2009). R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 433

Table 5 F3 Fz F4 The MMN shows increased involuntary attention switching in:

– Closed head injury (Kaipio et al., 2000) 0.5 s – Ageing (Gaeta et al., 2001) – Alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 2000b; Polo et al., 2003) – Children with major depression (Lepistö et al., 2004) – Sleep disorder (Gumenyuk et al., 2010) MMN

4.5 s lated with the recovery of behavioural speech-sound discrimination. Younger Males (mean 22 yrs) In epilepsy, the MMN is also affected. The MMN peak latencies Older Males (mean 59 yrs) were abnormally long or no clear MMN response could be found for speech (but could for tones) in patients with benign childhood Fig. 5. MMN to frequency change (700 ? 600 Hz) was normal in older males with epilepsy with contra-temporal spikes (Boatman et al., 2008; Du- an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 0.5 s but was considerably attenuated, relative to man et al., 2008). Patients with such spikes with atypical features that of younger males, with an ISI of 4.5 s, indexing faster auditory sensory-memory trace decay with aging (From Pekkonen et al., 2001a). and learning difficulties also exhibit attenuated MMN amplitudes (Metz-Lutz and Philippini, 2006). In addition, in the Landau–Kleff- ner syndrome, an MMN was obtained for phoneme change but not level of performance in different cognitive tasks is gradually de- for prosodic change (Honbolygó et al., 2006). By contrast, Miyajima creased (Cansino, 2009). There is also evidence linking these et al. (2011), studying adult patients with temporal-lobe epilepsy, MMN changes to cognitive deterioration (Kisley et al., 2005). Even found that at fronto-central sites, their MMN amplitude for tone- in healthy elderly persons, the MMN to a short gap in the middle of frequency change was enhanced, interpreted by the authors as a tone (Desjardin et al., 1999) was attenuated in amplitude, index- reflecting frontal-lobe hyperexcitability to compensate for tempo- ing their increased difficulties in correctly perceiving rapid speech- ral-lobe dysfunction indexed, according to the authors, by pro- sound patterns such as consonants (Bertoli et al., 2002, 2005). longed MMN peak latencies in patients relative to controls at The MMN and MMNm are also affected by different drugs both fronto-central and mastoid sites, consistent with previous re- (Table 1). ports (Lin et al., 2007; Borghetti et al., 2007). Furthermore, in the Miyajima et al. (2011) temporal-lobe epilepsy patients, the fron- to-centrally recorded MMN persisted longer than that in controls, 3. The MMN as an index of shortened sensory-memory duration consistent with the results of Gene-Cos et al. (2005) who suggested (Table 2) that a prolonged MMN duration might point to difficulty in ‘the closure mechanism of the MMN process’. The MMN elicitation depends on the presence of the sensory- The MMN can also be used to evaluate the possible central- memory trace representing the preceding stimuli and their regu- auditory-processing damage associated with a very premature larities at the moment of the delivery of a deviant stimulus (for a birth. Studying very prematurely born children (mean gestational review, see Näätänen et al., 2007). Hence, by gradually prolonging age 29 weeks) with a very low birth weight (mean 1115 g), Jans- the inter-stimulus interval (ISI), the MMN eventually vanishes, son-Verkasalo et al. (2003b, 2004) found that their MMN ampli- which enables one to assess sensory-memory duration in audition tude for consonant change at 4 years (from conception) was (a potential general index of brain plasticity; Näätänen and Kreegi- smaller than that of controls and, further, that the absence of the puu, 2011). MMN at 4–5 years of age predicted naming difficulties at 6 years. In young healthy adults, the trace duration, as estimated in this However, if the MMN amplitude nevertheless was normal at manner, approximates 10 s (Böttcher-Gandor and Ullsperger, 4 years, then there were no naming difficulties at 6 years. 1992; Sams et al., 1993), but is gradually shortened with ageing. Moreover, the MMN enables one to also determine central- With short ISIs such as 0.5 s, the MMN amplitude for frequency auditory-processing damage caused by long-term exposure to loud change was very similar in elderly (mean 59 years) and young occupational noise. In workers exposed to such a noise for 2– (mean 25 years) male subjects, suggesting that the memory-trace 17 years, the MMN to syllable change was attenuated in amplitude formation for sounds and, thus, perception (see Näätänen and Win- (even though their peripheral hearing was not affected), in keeping kler, 1999) were not affected by ageing. By contrast, with a stimu- with their increased speech-perception problems (Kujala et al., lus-onset asynchrony (SOA) of 4.5 s, the MMN amplitude of the 2004). In addition, the Brattico et al. (2005) MMN data showed that elderly was much more attenuated than that of the young (Pekko- long-term exposure to occupational noise altered the cortical orga- nen et al., 1996a)(Fig. 5), indicating that in ageing, the auditory nisation of speech-sound processing in these workers (in the ab- sensory-memory duration is shortened. Moreover, corresponding sence of peripheral hearing loss) but did not alter that of non- MMN data for frequency change in chronic alcoholics suggest speech processing. In addition, the discrimination of auditory stim- accelerated age-related shortening of the memory-trace duration uli was generally slowed down, as indexed by prolonged MMN- in these patients (Polo et al., 1999; see also Grau et al., 2001). peak latencies. The memory-trace duration is particularly short in patients Furthermore, by using the MMN, one can also monitor age-re- with Alzheimer’s disease, of course. Their MMN was, however, nor- lated changes in central auditory processing (Fabiani et al., 2006; mal with short ISIs (0.5 s), suggesting normal memory-trace for- Woods, 1992; Woods and Clayworth, 1986; Jääskeläinen et al., mation/perception (see Näätänen and Winkler, 1999) even in 1999a; Kiang et al., 2009; Cooper et al., 2006; Gaeta et al., 1998, these patients, but absent at long ISIs (3 s) (Pekkonen et al., 2001, 2002; Kisley et al., 2005; Pekkonen, 2000; Pekkonen et al., 1994), indexing their extremely short sensory-memory duration. 1995a, 1996a). A particularly convincing characterisation of the Interestingly, this data pattern is orthogonal to that observed in pa- MMN–age relationship was provided by Kiang et al. (2009) who tients with schizophrenia in whom no normal-amplitude MMN can carefully plotted the MMN and amplitudes for tone-duration be obtained in any condition, not even with very short ISIs and increment across adulthood for 147 normal subjects and 253 wide frequency deviations (Javitt et al., 1998). This suggests that schizophrenic patients (Fig. 4). Along with an increasing age, the their memory-trace formation, and hence auditory perception 434 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Table 6 4. The MMN as an index of abnormal auditory perception The MMN correlates with structural changes in:

– Schizophrenia (Hirayasu et al., 2000, 2001; Kasai et al., 2003a,b; Park et al., In the foregoing, it was already mentioned that in patients with 2002; Rasser et al., 2011; Salisbury et al., 2002, 2007; Yamasue et al., 2004; schizophrenia, no normal-size MMN can be obtained irrespective Youn et al., 2003; for a review, see van der Stelt and Belger, 2007) of experimental manipulations, not even with very short SOAs (Ja- – Cleft palate (children with) (Ceponiene et al., 1999) vitt et al., 1998). This suggests a fundamental abnormality in mem- ory-trace formation, and thus in auditory perception (see Näätänen and Winkler, 1999), in these patients (Näätänen and Kähkönen, 2009). Moreover, a number of further studies in patients with schizophrenia (Oades et al., 1996; Hirayasu et al., 1998; Schall et al., 1999; Youn et al., 2003; Kasai et al., 2002a,b) showed that the frequency-MMN amplitude deficit tends to correlate with the severity of auditory hallucinations. In addition, Youn et al. (2003) found that the abnormality of the left–right MMN-amplitude -4 µV asymmetry for frequency change of a tone correlated with the po- sitive scores of the positive-and-negative syndrome scale (PANSS), in particular with its hallucination subscale. For corroborating MMNm data, see Thönnesen et al. (2008). Moreover, Fisher et al. F4 (2008) found a smaller duration-MMN amplitude in patients with +1 µV 350 ms auditory hallucinations than in those with no hallucinations. In addition, the MMN amplitude for intensity change was also smaller 350-ms ISI in hallucinating than non-hallucinating patients. Furthermore, 700-ms ISI both patient groups showed an attenuated MMN amplitude for fre- 1400-ms ISI quency change. This MMN deficiency however was differently dis- tributed in frontal areas in the two patient groups. Fig. 6. MMN for tone-frequency change was of normal amplitude in 7–9 year old children with oral clefts when a short ISI (350 ms) was used but was considerably smaller than that of controls when the ISI was prolonged to 700 ms and to 1400 ms, 5. The MMN as an index of increased backward masking suggesting an accelerated sensory-memory trace in patients (From Ceponiene et al., 1999). (Table 3)

Auditory masking refers to any observation such that informa- (Näätänen and Winkler, 1999), is abnormal, consistent with clini- tion in a test auditory stimulus is reduced by the presentation of cal observations (for a review, see Näätänen and Kähkönen, another (masking) auditory stimulus (Massaro, 1973). The percep- 2009). By contrast, their sensory-memory duration, as judged from tion of sequentially presented auditory stimulation may be hin- the ISI–MMN relationship in these patients, is unaffected (Javitt dered by backward masking, with a sound preventing the et al., 1998; see also March et al., 1999). perception of an immediately preceding sound, apparently by The auditory memory-trace duration shows developmental affecting its memory-trace formation (Massaro, 1975), in particular changes also in the beginning of the life, but to the opposite direc- when the later sound is stronger in intensity than the preceding tion, of course, being only 0.5 s in newborns, as judged from MMNs sound (see Hawkins and Presson, 1986). Massaro (1975) concludes recorded during sleep (Cheour et al., 2002b), but is thereafter grad- that backward masking occurs because the masking stimulus ually prolonged during infancy and childhood. By using the Grau essentially overwrites the auditory image of the test stimulus et al. (1998) MMN paradigm for effectively determining the sen- and, therefore, terminates the perceptual processing of the test sory-memory duration, Glass et al., 2008a,b) found that this dura- stimulus. tion was about 1–2 s at the age of 2–3 years but was prolonged to Backward masking can be assessed both behaviourally and by 3–5 s by the age of 6 years (see also Gomes et al., 1999). using the MMN: a masker presented shortly after each stimulus This memory-trace duration is affected in some child patient of the causes the MMN to deviants and the sub- groups, too. In children of 7–8 years with cleft palate, the MMN ject’s ability to discriminate them behaviourally to disappear in for tone-frequency change was of normal amplitude with short parallel (Winkler and Näätänen, 1994; Winkler et al., 1993). By SOAs (350 and 700 ms), whereas the prolongation of the SOA to using this type of paradigm, it was found by Kujala et al. (2003) 1400 ms attenuated this amplitude much more than it attenuated that backward masking is enhanced in dyslexic individuals. In- that of control children (Ceponiene et al., 1999)(Fig. 6). Further- creased backward masking, as reflected by MMN-data pattern, more, an analogous data pattern was obtained in small infants with was also reported in chronic alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 1999). cleft palate, which was even more abnormal in children with the Importantly, this MMN deficit in chronic alcoholism correlated Catch-22 syndrome (Cheour et al., 1997, 1998a, 1999). with the amount of their alcohol consumption and predicted their In addition, a short auditory sensory-memory duration even in working-memory performance deficit (Ahveninen et al., 1999). normal healthy children is associated with delayed linguistic development. Late talkers had a shorter sensory-memory duration 6. The MMN as an index of abnormal involuntary attention than that of controls, as judged from MMN data for tone-frequency switching: either too weak (Table 4) or too strong (Table 5) change as a function of the ISI recorded at the age of 4 years 7 months, even though the language development was normal by As already mentioned, in addition to its auditory-cortex gener- that age (Grossheinrich et al., 2010). ators, the MMN also has a frontal (usually right-hemispherically Furthermore, the Barry et al. (2008) recent MMN data for tone- predominant (Giard et al., 1990)) generator contributing, together frequency change as a function of the ISI suggested a shorter audi- with the auditory-cortex MMN generator (see Fig. 2), to the fron- tory sensory-memory duration in parents of children with SLI than tally and centrally recorded MMN amplitudes (Deouell, 2007; that in parents of normally developing children. Giard et al., 1990; Rinne et al., 2000). The frontally recorded MMN amplitude is considerably attenuated, whereas the R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 435

Table 7 A similar (but transient) dampening of involuntary attention The MMN indexes pathological brain excitation/excitability in: switching can also be observed in acute alcohol intoxication – Alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 2000a; Pekkonen et al., 1998) (Jääskeläinen et al., 1995, 1996a,b, 1998, 1999b). Even a moderate – ALS (with bulbar signs) (Pekkonen et al., 2004) dose of ethanol selectively weakened the frontal-MMN process, – Chronic primary insomniacs (Wang et al., 2001) whereas the supratemporal MMN process (with most of it being – Epilepsy (Miyajima et al., 2011) separately observable in nose-referenced mastoid recordings) – Huntington’s disease, Syndromatic Phase (Beste et al., 2008) – Major depression, children with (Lepistö et al., 2004) was unaffected (Jääskeläinen et al., 1996b)(Fig. 2). Furthermore, – Pediatric migraine (Valeriani et al., 2008; see, however, de Tommaso et al., ethanol abolished the performance decrement caused by auditory 2004) distraction in a visual task (Jääskeläinen et al., 1999b), i.e., damp- – Schizophrenia (pathological microglia activation, indexing local disease ened the effects of auditory distracting stimuli on brain mecha- process and neuronal death) (Banati and Hickie, 2009) – Stuttering (Corbera et al., 2005; see also Wu et al., 1997) nisms controlling for environment-driven involuntary attention switches in audition (Escera et al., 2000a; Escera and Corral, 2007; Jääskeläinen et al., 1999b). This explains, in part, why alco- hol increases accident risk in traffic (cf. Näätänen and Summala, Table 8 1976). In addition, the Alho et al. (1994) MMN data suggested de- The MMN indexes cognitive and functional decline in: creased involuntary attention switching to auditory changes as a consequence of a frontal-lobe lesion (see Table 4). – Ageing (Gaeta et al., 2002; Kisley et al., 2005; Schroeder et al., 1995) – Alzheimer’s disease and dementia (Engeland et al., 2002; Schroeder et al., Conversely, the frontal involuntary attention-switching mecha- 1995) nism may also become too sensitive, in this way disturbing pri- – Amyotropic lateral schlerosis (ALS) (Raggi et al., 2008) mary-task performance (Table 5). In patients with closed head – Autism (Seri et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2008, 2011; Näätänen and Kujala, injury suffering from dramatically increased distractibility, this 2011) – CATCH-22 syndrome: children (for a review, see Ceponiene et al., 2002), mechanism is abnormally sensitive, judging from their very large adults (Baker et al., 2005) frontally recorded MMN amplitudes at the presence of normal – Children with cleft palate (Ceponiene et al., 2002) mastoid-recorded (nose-referenced) MMN amplitudes (Kaipio – Chronic alcoholism (Polo et al., 1999) et al., 1999, 2000) (cf. Fig. 2). A subsequent study (Polo et al., – Coma, survivors of (Kane et al., 2000) 2002) did not confirm this finding, however, which might result – Developmental dysphasia (Holopainen et al., 1997, 1998; Korpilahti and Lang, 1994) from the abnormally fast vigilance decrement of these patients – Diabetes mellitus (advanced phase) (Vanhanen et al., 1996) (Kaipio et al., 2001). – Down’s syndrome (Lalo et al., 2005) Moreover, in children suffering from major depression, short- – Dyslexia (Bonte et al., 2007) ened MMN latencies and increased P3a amplitudes (an index of – Epilepsy (Boatman et al., 2008; Liasis et al., 2000, 2001, 2006; Metz-Lutz and Philippini, 2006) the occurrence of an attention switch; Squires et al., 1975; Escera – HIV (Schroeder et al., 1994) et al., 2000a) might be associated with these patients’ increased – Intellectual disability (Ikeda et al., 2004; Nakagawa et al., 2002) distractibility and difficulties in concentrating on task perfor- – Multiple schlerosis (MS) (Gil et al., 1993; Jung et al., 2006; Santos et al., 2006) mance, as shown by the Lepistö et al. (2004) MMN data for CV-syl- – Parkinson’s disease (Brønnick et al., 2010; Karayanidis et al., 1995; Pekkonen lable change. Analogous MMN evidence for increased et al., 1995c) – Persistent-vegetative-state (PVS), survivors of (Kotchoubey, 2007; Wijnen distractibility was also obtained in chronic alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 2007; van der Stelt and van Boxtel, 2008; Zarza-Lucianez et al., 2007) et al., 2000a,b) and ageing (Gaeta et al., 2001). – Preterm children with very low birth weight (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2004, 2010) – REM-sleep deprivation (Zerouali et al., 2010) – Schizophrenia (e.g., Baldeweg et al., 2004; Hermens et al., 2010; Kawakubo 7. The MMN attenuation caused by cerebral grey-matter loss or et al., 2007; Light and Braff, 2005a,b; Rasser et al., 2011; Toyomaki et al., other structural change (Table 6) 2008) – Sleep deprivation (Raz et al., 2001) The MMN is also attenuated by structural damage in the central – Stroke and aphasia (Auther et al., 2000; Ilvonen et al., 2003, 2004; Pettigrew auditory system and even elsewhere in the brain. In schizophrenia et al., 2005; Särkämö et al., 2009, 2010a,b; Wertz et al., 1998) – Velo-cardio-facial syndrome (Baker et al., 2005) patients, a gradual loss of the left-hemisphere temporal grey-mat- ter volume has been observed, which was reflected by attenuated MMN amplitudes for frequency change (Hirayasu et al., 1998). mastoid-recorded MMN (with nose reference) is unaffected in pa- More recently, Salisbury et al. (2007) found, by using magnetic res- tients with schizophrenia (Baldeweg et al., 2002, 2004; Sato et al., onance imaging (MRI), that at first hospitalisation, schizophrenia 1998, 2002), suggesting dampened involuntary attention switch- patients’ left Heschl-gyrus volume did not differ from that of bipo- ing to auditory change and hence probably accounting for lar-disorder patients and normal controls and, further, that this some of their withdrawal (negative) symptoms, as proposed by was reflected by similar MMN amplitudes for tone-frequency Näätänen and Kähkönen (2009) (cf. Fig. 2). Consistent with this, change. However, in an 18-month follow-up, this volume was re- Matthews et al. (2007) found that the MMN at fronto-central elec- duced in schizophrenia patients only, and the magnitude of the trodes for change in the interaural time difference, one of the two Heschl’s gyrus volume reduction in these patients strongly corre- main cues for sound lateralisation, was attenuated in amplitude in lated with the parallel attenuation of their MMN amplitude. Also, patients with schizophrenia. This result was attributed by the Yamasue et al. (2004) found that in schizophrenia patients, MMNm authors to patients’ impaired ability to use temporal cues to sound deficiency for phoneme change in the left hemisphere correlated lateralisation in natural settings, a deficit probably affecting, with the magnitude of the left planum-temporale grey-matter loss, according to the authors, their ability to control the focus of atten- suggesting that these structural abnormalities may underlie the tion effectively. The dampened bottom-up attention-switching functional abnormalities of fundamental language-related process- function in schizophrenia patients might also contribute to their ing in these patients. In addition, very recently, Rasser et al. (2011) gradual cognitive decline because of the resulting reduction of cog- found that MMN-amplitude attenuation for frequency change in nitive and social stimulation (cf. sensory deprivation) (Näätänen patients with schizophrenia (reflecting their impaired day-to-day and Kähkönen, 2009). For further supportive MMN evidence, see functioning level) correlated with the magnitude of grey-matter Hermens et al. (2010). loss in the left Heschl’s gyrus as well as in the motor and executive 436 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Fig. 7. Post Hoc correlation analyses of reduced gray matter measures – averaged over specific gyri defined on the Deformed ‘‘Montreal Neurological Institute’’ brain template – with reduced frequency mismatch negativity (MMN) peak amplitudes recorded at fronto-central electrodes in control subjects (blue) and schizophrenia patients (Red). Solid regression lines indicate significant correlation (P < .05, uncorrected) (From Rasser et al., 2011). R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 437

Table 9 Consistent with the Pekkonen et al. (2004) above-reviewed ALS The MMN can be used to on-line monitor the patient’s level-of-consciousness in: results, at late, symptomatic, stages of Huntington’s disease, the – Anaesthesis: depth and recovery (Heinke et al., 2004; Heinke and Koelsch, MMN to frequency change was considerably enhanced in ampli- 2005; Koelsch et al., 2006; Yppärilä et al., 2002) tude, along with behavioural improvement in an auditory signal- – Coma (Kane et al., 1993, 1996, 2000; Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Luauté et al., detection task, compared to the control group and Huntington 2005; Morlet et al., 2000; Naccache et al., 2005) patients in the early, presymptomatic, stage (Beste et al., 2008). – Persistent vegetative state (Kotchoubey, 2007; Kotchoubey, 2007; Wijnen et al., 2007) This paradoxical effect, as contrasted to effects of neurodegenera- tive diseases in general, was attributed by the authors to toxic-le- vel overexcitability of the NMDAR system (due to increased glutamate concentrations). regions of the frontal cortex while MMN reduction for duration In addition, very importantly, Banati and Hickie (2009); (see deviants also correlated with grey-matter loss in the right Heschl’s also Banati, 2003) found in patients in schizophrenic that gyrus (Fig. 7). MMN deficiency for frequency and duration deviants indexed It is possible that the MMN deficiency observed in schizophre- pathological microglia (the main constituent of the brain’s innate nia can be linked to grey-matter loss occurring in these patients. immune system) activation, reflecting local ‘disease activity’ and Olney and Farber (1995) suggested that N-methyl-D-aspartate neuronal death in these patients. The authors emphasised that this (NMDA)-receptor (NMDAR) hypofunction accounts for structural microglia activation is not a diagnostically specific pathological brain changes in these patients. The data reviewed by the authors process but rather that this activation can serve as a generic mar- showed, among other things, that a repeated treatment with an ker that relates more directly to disease progression and may be NMDA antagonist during a 3- to 4-day period induces a pattern correlated with other meaningful illness measures such as the of neurodegenerative changes distributed over several corticolim- MMN. bic brain regions. The authors concluded that these and other find- Pathological brain excitation, as suggested by increased MMN ings signify that the persistent suppression of the NMDAR function amplitudes, can also be found in temporal-lobe epilepsy (Miyajima lasting for a few days only can lead to relatively subtle but perma- et al., 2011; Gene-Cos et al., 2005) even though MMN peak laten- nent structural changes that are distributed over the neocortical cies may be prolonged, a possible sign of central-auditory-process- and limbic brain regions in a pattern roughly similar to the pattern ing disturbance in these patients (Miyajima et al., 2011). of structural changes observed in schizophrenia (Bogerts, 1993). Pathological excitation might also explain some results in pa- The fact that the MMN is an index of NMDAR dysfunction would tients suffering from post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), who then explain the relationship between MMN deficiency and grey- showed considerably elevated MMN amplitudes for frequency matter loss (Salisbury et al., 2007; Rasser et al., 2011). change (Morgan and Grillon, 1999; Ge et al., 2011), whereas Men- The MMN can also reflect structural changes (craniofacial mal- ning et al. (2008) could not confirm these results. formations) associated with cleft palate (for a review, see Ceponi- ene et al., 2002). In children with cleft palate of 7–9 years of age, it was found by Ceponiene et al. (1999) that the more posteriorly 9. The MMN as an index of cognitive and functional delimited the cleft was, the smaller was the MMN amplitude for deterioration (Table 8) tone-frequency change and the more severe was the cognitive defect. Baldeweg et al. (2004) found a relationship between the fron- tally recorded duration-increment MMN deficit and impairments 8. The MMN as an index of pathological brain excitation/ in memory functions of patients with schizophrenia, which are evi- excitability (Table 7) dent in these patients (Sullivan et al., 1995; Jahshan et al., 2010). Subsequently, Light and Braff (2005a) obtained a strong correlation In some conditions, an increased MMN amplitude appears to in- between the global-assessment-of-functioning (GAF) ratings and dex pathologically increased central nervous system (CNS) or cen- the fronto-central MMN amplitude for tone-duration prolongation tral-auditory-system excitation/excitability. For example, in in these patients. Furthermore, the MMN amplitude was highly abstinent chronic alcoholics, the MMN for frequency change was predictive of the patients’ level of independence in their domestic abnormally enhanced in amplitude, which might be associated life. This correlation was replicated in longitudinal studies 1– with their increased distractibility (Ahveninen et al., 2000a,b). In 2 years later, indicating a stable relationship (Light and Braff, addition, in patients with persistent developmental stuttering, 2005b). Subsequently, the relationship between the duration- the MMN to phonetic contrasts was strongly enhanced in ampli- increment MMN amplitude and patients’ functional status was also tude, whereas that to simple tone contrasts (both frequency and found by Kawakubo et al., 2007; social skills acquisition) and by duration) was unaffected (Corbera et al., 2005). This might index Toyomaki et al. (2008; executive functioning). (For corroborating abnormal excitability located specifically in the speech-sound results with the frequency-MMN amplitude, see Rasser et al. MMN generation region (Näätänen et al., 1997). This MMN finding (2011).) Interestingly, this relationship for duration-increment correlated with the patients’ subjective appraisal of their speech- MMN amplitude is also present in normal healthy subjects (Light production problems. These results might indicate that proper et al., 2007). For previous analogous results with the frequency- speech production requires the presence of stable sensory refer- MMN amplitude in normal healthy subjects, see Bazana and Stel- ence provided by the speech-sound-specific memory traces of mack (2002) and Beauchamp and Stelmack (2006), and with the the left temporal cortex (Näätänen, 2001; Näätänen et al., 1997; MMN amplitude for syllable change in children, Liu et al. (2007). Shestakova et al., 2003). For recent corroborating results, see Troche et al. (2009, 2010). In the same vein, an MMNm enhancement to tone-duration In a similar vein, Jung et al. (2006) found that the MMN-ampli- decrement found by Pekkonen et al. (2004) in patients with ALS tude attenuation for duration decrement in patients with MS in- with bulbar signs might result from cortical overactivity of excit- dexed cognitive decline occurring in part of these patients, being atory neurotransmitter glutamate observed in ALS (Rowland and larger in magnitude for greater cognitive loss. For corroborating re- Shneider, 2001). Raggi et al. (2008), however, found MMN-ampli- sults, see Gil et al., 1993) and Santos et al. (2006). Furthermore, tude decrement for frequency change in non-demented patients MMN (MMNm) deficit indexes cognitive decline also in chronic with sporadic ALS. alcoholism (Polo et al., 1999, 2003), stroke (Ilvonen et al., 2003; 438 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Table 10 tude increase when the patient crossed the boundary from uncon- The MMN can be used to monitor the patient’s state in: sciousness to consciousness. In addition, in locked-in patients, the – Stroke: recovery (Ilvonen et al., 2003; Särkämö et al., 2010a,) frequency MMN can give evidence for the presence of at least some – Schizophrenia: illness progression (Banati and Hickie, 2009; Bodatsch et al., level of consciousness (Ragazzoni et al., 2000). Moreover, the MMN 2011; Javitt et al., 2000; Oades et al., 2006; Oknina et al., 2005; Salisbury can also index anaesthesia depth (for a review, see Heinke and et al., 2007; Umbricht and Krljes, 2005; Umbricht et al., 2003, 2006) and Koelsch, 2005): a small-amplitude MMN was recorded to fre- temporary recovery (Shinozaki et al., 2002) quency change during deep sedation, whereas in complete uncon- sciousness, this MMN was absent. It, however, returned while the patient was recovering from anaesthesia (Heinke et al., 2004). Interestingly, an MMN type of response was also elicited by mu- Särkämö et al., 2009, 2010a,; Auther et al., 1998, 2000; Pettigrew sic-syntactic violations in deep sedation (Heinke et al., 2004; Koe- et al., 2005; Wertz et al., 1998), epilepsy (Boatman et al., 2008; Lia- lsch et al., 2006), indicating that this kind of complex analysis sis et al., 2006), velo-cardio-facial (DiGeorge) syndrome (Baker following the rules of Western music can occur even in deep et al., 2005), Down’s syndrome (Lalo et al., 2005), children with sedation. certain forms of cleft palate and those with the Catch-22 syndrome Very recently, it was found by Boly et al. (2011) that in the veg- (Ceponiene et al., 2002) and, of course, in Alzheimer’s disease etative state, feed-forward processes are preserved whereas top- (Pekkonen et al., 1994; vMMN: Tales and Butler, 2006; Tales down processes are impaired. The effective connectivity was et al., 2008), dementia (Schroeder et al., 1995), intellectual disabil- measured by using the MMN for tones presented in a roving para- ity (Nakagawa et al., 2002; Ikeda et al., 2004) and mild cognitive digm (Baldeweg et al., 2004). The authors found that the only dif- impairment (MCI) (vMMN: Tales and Butler, 2006; Tales et al., ference between patients in the vegetative state and controls was 2008). an impairment of backward connectivity from frontal to temporal MMN-amplitude attenuation (at least for frequency change) can cortices, suggesting that a selective disruption of top-down pro- also index cognitive and functional degeneration in patients with cesses from high levels of a cortical hierarchy can lead to loss of an advanced stage of diabetes mellitus (Vanhanen et al., 1996), consciousness in brain-damaged patients, and can clearly differen- Parkinson’s disease (Karayanidis et al., 1995; Pekkonen et al., tiate the vegetative state from minimally conscious state. In addi- 1995b; Brønnick et al., 2010), HIV (Schroeder et al., 1994) and also tion to its neuroscientific relevance, the present approach could, in normal ageing (Gaeta et al., 2001; Kisley et al., 2005; Cooper according to Boly et al. (2011), constitute a new diagnostic tool et al., 2006). In addition, the MMN amplitude indexes decreased to quantify the level of consciousness electrophysiologically at cognition in different subnormal states such as sleep deprivation the patients’ bedside. (Raz et al., 2001), rapid eye movement (REM)-sleep deprivation The MMN can also reflect effects of hypnosis (Jamieson et al., (Zerouali et al., 2010) and in patients in the comatose (Fischer 2005; Kallio et al., 1999) and concentrative meditation (Srinivasan et al., 1999, 2006b) or PVS (Wijnen et al., 2007) state. and Baijal, 2007). Recently, Bisiachi et al. (2009) found smaller-amplitude MMN responses to tone-frequency change in newborns of less than 30 gestational weeks than in those with a longer gestational period. 11. The MMN as an index of the progression/severity of the This amplitude strongly correlated with several maturational fac- illness (Table 10) tors (gestational age, weight, length and cranial circumference). These results suggest, according to the authors, the role of the ges- In patients with schizophrenia, the MMN amplitude in particu- tational age as the main factor explaining cortical functioning at lar to frequency change is attenuated concomitantly with disease the early age. progression (for a meta-analysis, see Umbricht and Krljes, 2005). Consistent with this, several studies (Salisbury et al., 2002, 2007; 10. The MMN as an index of the level of consciousness (Table 9) Umbricht et al., 2006; Devrim-Ücok et al., 2008; Todd et al., 2008) showed that the frequency MMN deficit was more robust In patients in the comatose state, no MMN can usually be re- in chronic patients than in first-episode patients in whom the ef- corded unless a latent recovery process has started, leading to fect was smaller in size or had not yet emerged. In addition, Shino- the return of consciousness and cognitive capacities in the near fu- zaki et al. (2002) found that a temporary recovery of acute ture. Therefore, the MMN can be used as a tool in coma-outcome symptoms of schizophrenia patients was accompanied by an prediction (Kane et al., 1993, 1996; Fischer et al., 1999, 2004, MMN amplitude increase for tone-frequency change recorded at 2006a,b; Luauté et al., 2005; Morlet et al., 2000; Fischer and mastoids as polarity-reversed MMN (whereas the frontally re- Luauté, 2005; Daltrozzo et al., 2007; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., corded MMN did not correlate with this temporary recovery). In 2008). Moreover, the MMN peak latency measured at hospitalisa- addition, Urban et al. (2008), using the vMMN, found that the tion of survivors of coma caused by a severe head injury predicted, reduction of its amplitude for motion-direction deviants was larger to some extent, their expressive language ability and visuo-spatial for a more severe state and longer duration of the illness of the performance 1 year after the injury (Kane et al., 2000). Kotchoubey patient. (2007) stressed that in using the MMN for the objective assessment Moreover, MMN data also reflected clinical severity in patients of the patient’s remaining cognitive capacity, it is better to use with persistent developmental stuttering (Corbera et al., 2005), complex rather than simple sound stimuli which may result in se- dyslexia (Baldeweg et al., 1999; Stoodley et al., 2006), PTSD (Men- vere underestimation of the remaining capacity. For a meta-analy- ning et al., 2008) and in newborns and 6-month-old infants with sis of studies on the MMN in coma, see Daltrozzo et al. (2007). cleft palate (with clinical severity being determined as the ante- Recently, it was concluded by Vanhaudenhuyse et al. (2008) that rior–posterior extent of the cleft; Ceponiene et al., 1999, 2000a,b, MMN results in comatose patients converge to the conclusion that 2002). In a similar vein, Moberget et al. (2008) found that patients MMN is a very good predictor of recovery from coma, in particular exhibiting the most severe symptoms of ataxia, a sign of cerebellar in anoxic coma. cortical atrophy, produced smaller-amplitude MMN responses to In patients in the PVS, the frequency-MMN recovery occurred in tone-duration change than other patients. parallel with the recovery of consciousness (Wijnen et al., 2007; Studying stroke patients, Särkämö et al. (2008, 2009, 2010a, ad- Kotchoubey, 2007; Kotchoubey, 2007), showing a step-wise ampli- dressed amusia caused by a left or right middle cerebral artery R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 439

Table 11 The MMN predicts:

– Recovery from coma (Daltrozzo et al., 2007; Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Fischer et al., 1999, 2002, 2004, 2006a,b; Morlet et al., 2000; Naccache et al., 2005; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2008 (with the MMN being the best predictor of positive outcome, i.e., recovery without getting into the persistent vegetative state: Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Luauté et al., 2005); for a meta analysis, see Daltrozzo et al., 2007) – Recovery from the persistent vegetative state (Kotchoubey, 2007; Kotchoubey et al., 2007; Wijnen et al., 2007 (with the MMN recorded at hospilization also predicting the level of consciousness at discharge and even the extent of cognitive recovery during the next 2 years; Wijnen et al., 2007)) – Recovery from locked-in state (Ragazzoni et al., 2000) – Recovery from alcoholism (Kathmann et al., 1995; Pekkonen et al., 1998) – Social-skills learning in schizophrenia Kawakubo et al., 2007) – Social cognition in schizophrenia (Wynn et al., 2010) – Schizophrenia (first-episode onset) risk (Bodatsch et al., 2011; Stone et al., 2010) – Progression of schizophrenia (Bodatsch et al., 2011; Devrim-Ücok et al., 2008; Kawakubo et al., 2007; Kiang et al., 2009; Light and Braff, 2005b; Salisbury et al., 2007; Umbricht and Krljes, 2005; Umbricht et al., 2006) – Dyslexia (Blitz et al., 2007; Leppänen et al., 2010; van Leeuwen et al., 2006; Stoodley et al., 2006) – Language development (Choudhury and Benasich, 2011; for a review, see Stix, 2011) – Language development in prematurely born children (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003b, 2004, 2010) Fig. 8. Grand-average difference waves showing the duration mismatch negativity – Drug-therapy outcome in schizophrenia (Horton et al., 2010; Javitt et al., in the frontal (upper row) and central electrodes (lower row) for converting (dashed 2008; Lavoie et al., 2007; Schall et al., 1999) line) and nonconverting subjects (solid line). AR-C = at risk – coinversion, AR- – Working-memory performance decrement in chronic alcoholism (Ahveninen NC = at risk – no conversion (From Bodatsch et al., 2011). et al., 1999)

Table 12 The MMN shows increased genetic risk of: stroke. They found that amusia caused by a right-hemisphere dam- age, especially that to temporal and frontal areas, was more severe – Alcoholism (Rodriguez et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 2001) than that caused by a left-hemisphere damage. Furthermore, in – Asperger syndrome (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2005; Korpilahti et al., 2007) – Dyslexia (Blitz et al., 2007; Friedrich et al., 2004; Leppänen et al., 2002, amusic right-hemisphere patients, the severity of amusia corre- 2010; Leppänen and Lyytinen, 1997; Lyytinen et al., 2001, 2004, 2006; lated with weaker frequency-MMNm responses. Additionally, Maurer et al., 2009; Pihko et al., 1999; Richardson et al., 2003; van within the right-hemisphere-damaged group, the amusic patients Leeuwen et al., 2006) who had damage in the showed worse recovery – Language learning impairment (LLI) (Benasich et al., 2006; Choudhury and from amusia as well as weaker MMNm responses throughout the Benasich, 2011; Friedrich et al., 2004; Weber et al., 2005) – Schizophrenia in children (Schreiber et al., 1992), adults (Ahveninen et al., 6-month follow-up than did the non-amusic patients or the amusic 2006; Bodatsch et al., 2011; Brockhaus-Dumke et al., 2005; Hall et al., patients with no auditory-cortex damage. Also, the amusic patients 2007, 2009; Jessen et al., 2001; Magno et al., 2008; Michie et al., 2002; (both with and without auditory-cortex damage) performed worse Shin et al., 2009), and in Individuals with the CATCH syndrome (with a 108/158 108/ than did the non-amusic patients on a number of tests of different higher risk in individuals carrying the COMT Val than COMT 158Met allele) (Baker et al., 2005) aspects of cognitive performance (including working memory, – Specific language impairment (SLI) (Barry et al., 2008; Benasich and Tallal, attention and cognitive flexibility). These findings suggested, 2002; Benasich et al., 2006; Friedrich et al., 2004; Weber et al., 2005) according to the authors, domain-general cognitive deficits as the primary mechanism underlying amusia without auditory-cortex damage, whereas amusia with auditory-cortex damage is associ- Furthermore, in patients with schizophrenia, MMN data can ated with both auditory and cognitive deficits. predict future developments of the patient’s state and the effi- In conclusion, because of the large number of different disor- ciency of different treatments. For example, Umbricht et al. ders affecting the MMN, it appears that the MMN provides a gen- (2006) found that MMN deficiency at illness onset may index the eric illness measure, useful for assessing illness severity, as presence of a more pervasive brain pathology and be observed only suggested by Banati and Hickie (2009) for both the MMN and in a subgroup of patients, possibly in those with more precursors of microglia activation. schizophrenia or with an elevated risk of developing chronic schizophrenia. In addition, Kawakubo et al. (2007) found that the MMN in patients with schizophrenia was predictive of the acquisi- 12. The MMN in predicting illness course/drug response tion of social skills during a 3-month social skills training (prognosis) (Table 11) programme. Importantly, it was recently found by Lavoie et al. (2007) that A striking finding was that the recovery of the MMN in a coma- the deficient MMN amplitude for frequency change in patients tose patient strongly predicted, as already mentioned, the recovery with schizophrenia was corrected towards normal levels by a glu- of consciousness in the near future (Kane et al., 1993, 1996; Fischer tathione precursor, N-acetyl-cysteine (NAC), which improved their et al., 2000, 2006a,b; see also Fischer et al., 2008; Luauté et al., NMDAR function, supporting the role of the NMDARs in MMN 2005; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2008; for a meta-analysis, see Dal- generation. trozzo et al., 2007). In addition, the MMN in patients in the PVS re- The MMN improvement was, however, found in the absence of corded at hospitalisation predicted the magnitude of cognitive any robust changes in clinical-severity indices, even though they recovery even 2 years ahead (Wijnen et al., 2007; see also Kotchou- were observed in a larger and more prolonged clinical study (La- bey, 2007; Kotchoubey et al., 2007). voie et al., 2007; Berk et al., 2008a,b; for supporting evidence, 440 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Table 13 MMN indexes central-auditory-processing improvement in:

– Autistic patients (with frequency processing better than in Controls; Gomot et al., 2002) – Children with specific language impairment (SLI) receiving language and speech training (Pihko et al., 2007) – Cochlear-implant (CI) patients as a function of time since CI installation (Kraus et al., 1993b; Lonka et al., 2004; Ponton et al., 2000; Ponton and Eggermont, 2001) and training (Ponton et al., 2009) – Dyslexic children presented with consonant–vowel syllables with lengthened formant-transition duration (Bradlow et al., 1999) – Dyslexic children receiving audio-visual rehabilitation (Kujala et al., 2001) – Early Child development, mainly speech-sound learning (Alho and Cheour-Luhtanen, 1997; Alho et al., 1990; Carral et al., 2005; Cheour et al., 1998b, 2002a,b; Cheour- Luhtanen et al., 1996; Choudhury and Benasich, 2011; Draganova et al., 2005, 2007; Gomes et al., 2000; Gomot et al., 2000; Fellman and Huotilainen, 2006; Huotilainen et al. 2005, 2007; Kraus and Cheour, 2000; Kraus et al., 1992, 1993a, 1999; Kuhl, 2004; Kujala et al., 2004; Kushnerenko et al., 2002, 2007; Leppänen et al., 2004; Lovio et al., 2009; Lyytinen et al., 2001, 2006; Molholm et al., 2004; Novitski et al., 2007; Oades et al., 1996; Pang et al., 1998; Pihko et al., 2005; Tanaka et al., 2001; Trainor et al., 2001, 2003; Uwer and von Suchodoletz, 2000) – Early-onset blindness (Kujala et al., 1995a,b, 2000b, 2005b; Alho et al., 1993) – Epileptic patients with vagus-nerve stimulation (Borghetti et al., 2007) – Infants operated for craniosynosthosis (Huotilainen et al., 2008) – Patients with hearing aids (Rudner et al., 2008) – Patients with ultrasound hearing aids (Hosoi et al., 1998) – Schizophrenic patients progressing from the Acute to Post-Acute Phase (Shinozaki et al., 2002) – Sleeping infants exposed to auditory discrimination training (Cheour et al., 2002a) – Stroke patients as a function of time since stroke onset (Ilvonen et al., 2003; Särkämö et al., 2010a,) – Stroke patients receiving music or audio-book stimulation (Särkämö et al., 2010a,)

see Coyle, 2006; Schwieler et al., 2008). Hence, an MMN enhance- discriminate non-native phonemes was preserved in prematurely ment may precede improvement in clinical severity, which, born infants. Moreover, it was also found that the better this ability according to the authors, highlights the possible utility of the was preserved (reflected by larger MMN-amplitudes for non-na- MMN as a biomarker of treatment efficacy, and, therefore, also as tive contrasts) at the age of 1 year, the worse was the performance a tool in drug development (see also Javitt, 2007, 2009; Javitt in native-language tests at the age of 2 years. Thus, decline in sen- et al., 2008). Previously, in the Schall et al. (1999) study, the sitivity to non-native phonemes, as reflected by MMN attenuation MMN predicted the outcome of clozapine therapy in patients with for non-native phoneme contrasts, served as a positive predictor schizophrenia. Moreover, more recently, Horton et al. (2011) found for further age-appropriate language development. that clozapine increased the MMN amplitude for tone-frequency Moreover, the Weber et al. (2005) MMN data showing a deficit change, with increasing doses being associated with increasing fre- of prosodic perception in 5-month-old infants suggested that the quency-MMN amplitudes (but attenuated that for tone-duration MMN might be taken as an early marker of SLI risk. increments and decrements). Very recently, it was found by Bodatsch et al. (2011) that in individuals with a high schizophrenia risk, the MMN for tone-dura- 13. The MMN as an index of genetic disposition to different tion decrement (but not for tone-frequency change) was lower in pathologies (Table 12) amplitude (resembling that of first-episode patients) in those pa- tients who subsequently, during an observation period of 2 years, Some studies suggest that in schizophrenia there is a genetic converted to the first-episode psychosis than that in those who contribution to its aetiology. An MMN-amplitude attenuation for did not convert to psychosis during that period (Fig. 8). This result duration increment was found by Michie et al. (2002) and that was remarkable as these groups were thought to share a similar for frequency change by Jessen et al. (2001) in symptom-free clinical risk of psychosis at baseline. The authors proposed that first-order relatives of patients with schizophrenia but studies with the duration-MMN reduction in their converters might reflect both larger samples have either not confirmed these findings (Bramon structural and neurochemical alterations associated with the con- et al., 2004) or showed a trend only (Price et al., 2006). However, version to psychosis. Furthermore, such results do not, according to Hall et al. (2006) recently found a significant, although modest, ge- the authors, only contribute to the prediction of conversion and netic correlation between the duration-increment MMN-ampli- lead time to it but also contribute to a more individualised risk tude deficit and schizophrenia, and therefore suggested that the assessment and hence also to risk-adapted prevention. In addition, MMN is a potentially valid endophenotype in schizophrenia. By recently, Stone et al. (2010) found in subjects at risk of psychosis, contrast, Ahveninen et al. (2006), using the MMN to tone-fre- an attenuated frontal-MMN amplitude for tone-duration incre- quency changes in their study of twin pairs discordant for schizo- ment and, further, that this MMN deficit was related to reduced phrenia, obtained results suggesting, according to the authors, that thalamic glutamate plus glutamine and NMDA levels. MMN abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia might reflect Further, a phonological deficit in young elementary-school chil- predominantly state-dependent neurodegeneration. A similar con- dren, reflected by a reduced MMN amplitude to syllable change clusion was made by Magno et al. (2008) on the basis of their dura- (but not that to simple frequency change), appeared to predict tion- and frequency-MMN amplitude results. their dyslexia risk (Blitz et al., 2007). Also, in very prematurely In patients with the 22q11 deletion syndrome, the considerably born children, the MMN amplitude for consonant change at 4 years increased rates of schizophrenia are attributed to the loss of one al- of age predicted, as already mentioned, naming performance at the lele containing the catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT) gene. age of 6 years (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003b, 2004). Furthermore, Symptom-free carriers showed attenuated MMN amplitudes for Jansson-Verkasalo et al. (2010) found that the perceptual narrow- both simple auditory (duration and frequency) and syllabic ing for non-native phoneme contrasts occurring in normal healthy changes as well as corresponding decrements in behavioural sound infants from 6 to 12 months of age (Kuhl et al., 1991; Kuhl, 2004), discrimination (Baker et al., 2005). Moreover, these MMN and reflected by attenuated MMNs for non-native phoneme contrasts behavioural effects were stronger in subjects carrying the COMT in these infants (Cheour et al., 1998b), does not occur in very pre- 108/158Met allele on their single intact chromosome (associated maturely born infants who showed large MMNs even for non-na- with a greater risk of schizophrenia) than in those carrying the tive phoneme contrasts. This indicates that the ability to COMT 108/158Val allele (a smaller schizophrenia risk). The authors R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 441 concluded that because the MMN depends on the NMDAR function cooperate. The MMNs in the first two sessions were quite small (Javitt et al., 1996), the MMN modification by the COMT Val 108/ in amplitude for sound-duration and -frequency changes, but be- 158Met polymorphism points towards abnormal dopamine–gluta- came larger thereafter, disclosing normal-like amplitudes at mate interactions in the MMN-generator system. 3 months post-stroke (Fig. 3). Furthermore, the duration-MMN Furthermore, children with a parent or sibling with dyslexia amplitude increase correlated with the improvement in the Boston have an increased risk of dyslexia, and this is reflected by their Diagnostic Aphasia Examination test scores from 10 days to attenuated MMN amplitudes to simple frequency (Maurer et al., 3 months. 2003) or duration changes (Friedrich et al., 2004; Leppänen et al., Subsequently, as already mentioned, it was found by Särkämö 2002) or to phoneme-category change (van Leeuwen et al., et al. (2010b) that the recovery of the MMNm and behavioural dis- 2006). Similarly predictive were the lack of a left-hemisphere crimination can be expedited with music and audio-book stimula- MMN response to speech-sound change (Maurer et al., 2003) and tion. Dividing their 60 middle cerebral artery stroke patients into the bilateral distribution of this MMN response (Maurer et al., music and audio-book listening and control groups, these authors 2009). recorded patients’ MMNm to frequency and duration changes in Also, studying 6-month-old infants with a family history of lan- a binaurally delivered complex tone. (In addition, all patients re- guage-related learning difficulties, Benasich et al. (2006) found ceived the normal hospital treatment and rehabilitation of stroke that these infants had smaller mismatch responses (MMRs; the po- patients, of course.) It was found, as expected, that the MMNm sitive-polarity analogue of MMN; Maurer et al., 2003) than those of amplitudes were, in general, smaller in the lesioned than opposite control infants with no such family history. In addition, as already hemisphere. Importantly, the frequency-MMNm amplitude in- mentioned, Barry et al. (2008) obtained MMN data suggesting that creased more in both music and audio-book groups than in the parents of children with SLI have a shorter auditory sensory-mem- control group during the 6-month post-stroke period. In addition, ory duration than that of control parents. the duration-MMNm amplitude increased more in the audio-book Very recently, a major breakthrough in the research of the ge- group than in the other groups. Moreover, the frequency-MMNm netic background of dyslexia was made by Roeske et al. (2011) amplitude changes correlated with the behavioural improvement who found that a certain marker (rs4234898), located on chromo- of verbal memory and focussed attention induced by music listen- some 4q32.1, was associated with the MMN deficit for a CV-sylla- ing. The authors interpreted their results as demonstrating that the ble change. Moreover, this MMN was also associated with a mere listening to music or speech starting soon post-stroke onset two-marker haplotype of rs4234898 and rs11100040, one of its can induce long-term plastic changes in early sensory processing, neighbouring single-nucleotide polymorphisms. The authors con- as reflected by the MMNm enhancement, which, in turn, may facil- cluded that their results suggest a possible transregulation effect itate the recovery of higher cognitive functions. These remarkable on SLC2A3, the predominant facilitative glucose transporter in results encourage the use of music and speech stimulation in the neurons, which might lead to glucose deficits in dyslexic children rehabilitation of stroke patients in particular in the early post- and could explain their attenuated MMN in passive listening tasks, stroke period. as observed in this study. Moreover, in a very recent study of the The deficit of the auditory/language system may be inborn, as is same group, Czamara et al. (2011) obtained results pointing to the case in developmental dyslexia, SLI or in the autism spectrum. an association between the late MMN for speech-sound change Because of the high prevalence of these disorders and their devas- and rare variants in a candidate region for dyslexia. tating effects on the individual’s academic success and thereby self-esteem, early identification and remediation methods should be developed. Recent evidence fortunately shows that the aberrant 14. The MMN as an index of central auditory processing neural substrate of language deficits can be ameliorated with improvement or recovery (Table 13) appropriate intervention. For instance, Pihko et al. (2007) found that language-impaired children at the age of 5–6 years benefited Spontaneous and training-induced improvement. A number of from language and speech exercises. Furthermore, this training studies in normal healthy subjects (Atienza and Cantero, 2001; also changed the neural processing of speech sounds so that the Kraus et al., 1995; Gottselig et al., 2004; Menning et al., 2000; MMNm for syllable changes became stronger in the left Tremblay et al., 1997, 1998; Näätänen et al., 1993; for a review, hemisphere. see Kujala and Näätänen, 2010) demonstrated that the MMN re- In dyslexic first-grade children, intervention, judging from an flects plastic neural changes associated with discrimination learn- MMN-amplitude enhancement, induced neural plastic changes ing. Therefore, it is a very attractive tool for following up recovery and ameliorated reading difficulties (Kujala et al., 2001). These 7- and intervention effects in various patient groups, for instance, in year-old school children were exposed to an audiovisual training stroke, coma, PVS or cochlear-implant (CI) patients or in children programme with no linguistic items (Audilex Program; Karma, with developmental disorders. 1999) for 7 weeks. Their reading skills as well as the MMN elicited In stroke and other brain-injured patients, it would be impor- by tone-order reversals were assessed before and after this training tant to determine the changes in perceptual abilities very soon period. After this period, the training group made fewer reading er- after the damage. However, such patients are often unable to coop- rors and tended to read faster than did the age-matched control erate or to understand instructions, particularly if the neural net- group of dyslexic children. Moreover, the MMN amplitude for work involved in speech perception is affected. Aaltonen et al. tone-order reversals was increased in the training group but not (1993), in an early study, found that the MMN to speech-sound in the control group. These results consequently showed that read- change was abolished in posterior left-hemispheric stroke patients, ing deficits can be ameliorated with audiovisual training and, fur- whereas an anterior left-hemispheric stroke had no such effect. By ther, that the improvement of reading skills is associated with contrast, the tone-frequency MMN was normal in both patient plastic neural changes of the central auditory system as reflected groups. Subsequently, as already mentioned, Ilvonen et al. (2003) by the MMN enhancement. Furthermore, these positive training followed up the recovery of left-hemisphere stroke patients, results were obtained by using no linguistic stimuli, which sug- recording the MMN at 4 and 10 days, and then at 3 and 6 months gests that impaired auditory-perceptual processes other than defi- after stroke onset. Language-comprehension tests (Boston Diag- cient phonology may also contribute to reading impairments. nostic Aphasia Examination and Token tests) were also adminis- Similar effects were also obtained with extremely low-birth- tered in all but the first session in which the patients could not weight children of 6 years of age (Huotilainen et al., 2011). These 442 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 children were allocated to a 5-week training programme with comparable response. Furthermore, Kelly et al. (2005) reported a Audilex (training group) or to a control group playing computer similar failure to obtain the MMN from CI users defined as poor games. Before and after the intervention, auditory ERPs to sound performers. In addition, Lonka et al. (2004), in longitudinally track- changes were recorded and reading-related skills were assessed. ing the changes in the language function after a small group of deaf The MMN responses to frequency and duration deviants increased adults received a CI, reported that while the MMN was absent for in amplitude in the Audilex training group but not in the control- approximately 12 months post-surgery, the response subsequently game group. However, the reading skills were similar in the two emerged, first for a large vowel contrast, and then later for more groups after invention and 2 years later. subtle vowel distinctions. Converging behavioural improvement As already mentioned, the shortening of the abnormally long in a speech-discrimination test was also observed during the fol- MMN-peak latency and the MMN-amplitude recovery in drug- low-up period. resistant adult epilepsy patients after vagus-nerve stimulator Trautwein et al. (1998) and Ponton et al. (2000), who assessed implantation was recently found by Borghetti et al. (2007), sug- the CI users with non-speech stimuli, obtained results that relate gesting a positive effect of vagus-nerve stimulation on central back to both speech performance and training. First, Trautwein auditory processing in these patients. et al. (1998) found that while psychophysical thresholds were Drug-induced improvement.InTable 1, short-term effects of dif- superior to MMN-estimated discrimination thresholds for duration ferent drugs are reviewed. In addition, in the foregoing, some contrasts in the control group, the opposite was found for a group favourable drug effects in schizophrenia patients were already of adult CI users. Thus, in several individuals, there was neuro- mentioned (NAC, Lavoie et al., 2007; clozapine, Schall et al., physiological evidence of discrimination based on the MMN, with- 1999; Horton et al., 2011). Moreover, very recently, Dulude et al. out the individual being able to behaviourally perceive the (2010) found that nicotine, relative to placebo, not only prolonged difference. Based on the apparent separation of discrimination at the peak latency of the duration MMN but also increased its ampli- the level of sensation versus perception, Ponton et al. (2009) devel- tude in patients to a level comparable with that seen in controls. By oped a protocol based on pre-training MMN evaluation to deter- contrast, the frequency MMN was not affected. The authors con- mine whether there was neurophysiological evidence of cluded that these preliminary findings demonstrate that acute nic- discrimination in the absence of perceptual discrimination. This otine can normalise temporal aspects of central auditory knowledge was then used to develop a training protocol which re- processing in patients with schizophrenia, an effect that might be sulted in significant improvements in frequency discrimination as mediated by the activation of a7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors, well as combined consonant and vowel discrimination in a single the functioning of which is diminished in schizophrenia. The highly trained adult implant user. authors further concluded that these ameliorating effects of nico- As noted by Roman et al. (2005), there is a clear application of tine may have implications for understanding the increased tobac- the MMN in clinical testing and training of CI populations. How- co smoking in schizophrenic patients and for developing nicotinic ever, refinements are needed in methods and recording technique, pharmacotherapeutics to alleviate sensory-memory impairments to optimise the protocols for frequency discrimination and speech in schizophrenia. In addition, a positive effect of nicotine in both testing. smoking and non-smoking normal healthy subjects on temporal- Recently, music perception of CI users was also studied by using interval processing (indexed by an MMN-amplitude enhancement the MMN. Koelsch et al. (2004) found that CI users can perceive to ISI deviants) was recently found by Martin et al. (2009). timbre, judging from the MMN elicited by timbre deviants in them Furthermore, very recently, it was found by Sawada et al. (2010) (the timbre MMN: Tervaniemi et al., 1997; Toiviainen et al., 1998). that in children of 7–13 years of age with attention deficit hyperac- Moreover, music-syntactic violations elicited an early right ante- tivity disorder (ADHD), the intake of osmotic-release methylpheni- rior negativity which might be interpreted as a music-syntactic date (MPH) increased the MMN amplitude for tone-frequency MMN in CI users, which again was considerably smaller in ampli- change and simultaneously alleviated the severity of their ADHD tude than that in controls but nevertheless showed that CI users’ symptoms. central neural mechanisms underlying music perception are acti- Central auditory discrimination and training in CI users. For pro- vated by musical stimulation (even though they reported that they foundly deaf individuals, a sensation of hearing can be restored had difficulties in discriminating musical information and that by a CI. CIs are currently the most successful and widely used sen- they were in general frustrated by their inability to accurately hear sory neuroprotheses. In a large percentage of those individuals fit- music). Consistent with this, subsequently, Sandmann et al. (2010), ted with a CI, a remarkable degree of language communication via by using the multi-feature MMN paradigm with six different levels the auditory domain is restored. Nevertheless, objective, non- of the magnitude of deviation for each feature (Pakarinen et al., behavioural measures of auditory discrimination such as the 2007)(Fig. 1) found that CI users had difficulties in discriminating MMN remain a potentially powerful, but as yet under-utilised, small changes in the acoustic properties of musical sounds. It was application for this population. concluded by Sandmann et al. (2010) that this type of paradigm The first MMN recordings from CI users were reported by Kraus could be of substantial clinical value by providing a comprehensive et al. (1993b) who employed speech-syllable contrasts. For the profile of the extent of restored hearing in CI users. Moreover, an group of individuals investigated in that study, Kraus et al. found inverse relationship was found between the MMN amplitude and that the MMN elicited from CI users was remarkably similar to that the duration of profound deafness; however, there was also evi- obtained from a group of normal-hearing listeners. These results dence for experience-related plastic changes as a function of the suggested that despite the differences in peripheral input, the brain duration of CI use (Sandmann et al., 2010). was processing basic speech units in a very similar fashion. Ultrasonic hearing. Some profoundly deaf individuals can per- Ponton et al. (2000; see also Ponton and Don, 1995, 2003; Pon- ceive ultrasound only, which occurs by bone conduction. Hosoi ton and Eggermont, 2007), recording the MMN evoked by a single et al. (1998) found that in normal healthy subjects, ultrasound contrast in stimulus duration from groups of normal-hearing modulated by different speech sounds produces discriminable adults and adult CI users, also found that MMNs of the two groups stimuli, which points to the possibility of developing ultrasonic were quite similar. In a more detailed study in which subjects were hearing aids as an alternative for CIs for those profoundly deaf indi- separated into groups based on benefit gained by using a CI, Groe- viduals who can sense ultrasound only. Hosoi and colleagues deliv- nen et al. (1996) reported that whereas the group classified as good ered ultrasound to the right sterno-cleidomastoid through a performers showed an MMN, poor performers failed to generate a ceramic vibrator. Substantial N1m responses were elicited by both R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 443 audible air-conducted sound stimuli and bone-conducted ultra- development, for example, the prediction of coma or PVS outcome sonic stimuli. Further, when the (40 kHz) ultrasound stimuli mod- and even that of the extent of the cognitive recovery of these pa- ulated by vowels/i/ and/a/ were presented as standards and tients (during the next 2 years; Wijnen et al., 2007), or the moni- deviants, respectively, substantial MMNm responses were ob- toring of, and predicting, the progress of schizophrenia. served for deviants, indicating that the two sounds were discrimi- Moreover, importantly, very recent results of Bodatsch et al. nable. Moreover, the equivalent current dipole (ECD) for the (2011) showed that the duration MMN can predict, in individuals MMNm was located within the auditory cortex, in the vicinity of in schizophrenia-risk group, conversion to first-episode schizo- the MMNm observed in a separate condition with the respective phrenia and even when this would occur in the period of 2 years. air-conducted stimuli. The authors also tested 53 profoundly deaf In addition, another recent study (Banati and Hickie, 2009) sug- individuals behaviourally and found that 28 of them could detect gested that the duration and frequency MMNs might enable one ultrasonic stimuli and, further, that 11 of them could discriminate to online monitor local disease activity and neuronal death in pa- ultrasounds modulated by different speech signals. These results tients with schizophrenia. provide, according to the authors, the rationale for developing Furthermore, in case of schizophrenia (see Lavoie et al., 2007; ultrasound hearing aids, which do not require surgery, for those Horton et al., 2011; Javitt et al., 2008; Banati and Hickie, 2009; profoundly deaf who can sense ultrasound. see also Berk et al., 2008a,b), and perhaps of other severe neuro- Improved discrimination in early blind individuals. A special case psychiatric, neurological and neurodevelopmental disorders, too, of enhanced central auditory function of compensatory nature the MMN can be used as an online index of treatment efficacy. can be encountered in early blinds. The MMN elicited by sound- As far as dyslexia is concerned, such a use of the MMN in assessing location changes was larger in amplitude in early-blind individuals the effectiveness of different rehabilitation programmes is already than in sighted control subjects (Kujala et al., 1995a). (The MMN well established (Kujala et al., 2001). In addition, in the foregoing, was recorded while subjects’ attention was directed to the tactile we reviewed results of Särkämö et al. (2010a) demonstrating how modality.) This suggests enhanced spatial auditory perception the MMNm can be used in evaluating the effectiveness of different and discrimination in the blind. The result is consistent with auditory stimulus material in an attempt at facilitating stroke behavioural data showing that the blind are more accurate than recovery. the sighted in detecting loci of origin of sounds (Lessard et al., Some further clinically important uses of MMN data are, among 1998; Röder et al., 1999). The scalp topography of MMN response other things, the possibility to determine the dyslexia risk in new- of Kujala et al., 1995a) did not differ between the two groups, how- borns and infants (Maurer et al., 2003; Leppänen et al., 2002; van ever. It, therefore, behaved differently from the N2b (Näätänen Leeuwen et al., 2006; Friedrich et al., 2004), the magnitude of cog- et al., 1982; for a review, see Näätänen and Gaillard, 1983) re- nitive and functional deficit associated with different clinical con- sponse elicited by auditory targets in an active discrimination con- ditions (Näätänen et al., 2011a) and the degree of severity of dition, which had a scalp topography in the blind that was different conditions (in Schizophrenia: Umbricht et al., 2006; Dev- posterior to that in the sighted (Kujala et al., 1992, 1995a,b). Thus, rim-Ücok et al., 2008; Todd et al., 2008; Light and Braff, 2005a,b; the MMN appears to be hard-wired in the central auditory system Salisbury et al., 2002, 2007; Stuttering: Corbera et al., 2005; PTSD: of the blind, reflecting plastic changes within the auditory cortex, Menning et al., 2008; Cleft Palate: Ceponiene et al., 1999, 2000, whereas the generators of the N2b, associated with attentional 2002; Stroke and Aphasia: Ilvonen et al., 2003; Pettigrew et al., processes in audition, may become part of the neural system nor- 2005; Särkämö et al., 2010b; Dyslexia: Baldeweg et al., 1999; Kuj- mally subserving vision. This was supported by imaging studies ala et al., 2001). suggesting occipital activity in the blind during auditory change- One might wonder why auditory discrimination is affected in detection tasks (Kujala et al., 1995b, 2007, 2000b, 2005b). such a large number of different clinical conditions (Table 1), which indeed points to a possible common pathology shared by these different conditions. A major part of this common pathology 15. Concluding discussion appears to be found in the deficient functioning of the NMDAR sys- tem, as there is very strong evidence indicating that MMN defi- This article has shown that central auditory processing, as in- ciency reflects deficient NMDAR functioning (Javitt et al., 1996; dexed by the MMN and the MMNm, is affected in a wide range Näätänen et al., 2011a). For example, sub-anaesthetic doses of ket- of different clinical conditions and in ageing. Most of these effects amine, an NMDAR antagonist, attenuated the MMN amplitude of are seen as indexing decreased auditory discrimination accuracy. healthy volunteers (Kreitschmann-Andermahr et al., 2001: In some cases, however, the duration of auditory short-term sen- MMNm; Umbricht et al., 2000, 2002), monkey (Javitt et al., sory memory, essential, for instance, in speech perception and 1996), rat (Tikhonravov et al., 2008, 2010) and mouse (Ehrlichman understanding, is affected. This further decreases automatic dis- et al., 2008), and caused schizophrenic kinds of psychotic experi- crimination when SOAs are prolonged. Moreover, these data also ences in normal subjects (Umbricht et al., 2000, 2002; see also show effects on mechanisms controlling for involuntary shifts of Sauer and Volz, 2000; Pang and Fowler, 1999). Furthermore, keta- attention (passive attention) or on those determining the charac- mine disturbed the performance in a memory task in normal sub- teristics of backward masking in different patient groups. jects in a manner similar to that normally observed in Consequently, this article has shown that the MMN provides a schizophrenia, as shown by Umbricht et al. (2000). Importantly, unique window to the neuropsychology of central auditory pro- it was found by Umbricht et al. (2002) that the smaller was the cessing, and hence a possibility for the objective assessment of subject’s MMN amplitude for frequency and duration changes be- auditory discrimination and sensory memory, in different patient fore ketamine administration, the stronger were the psychotic groups, which previously could be mainly inferred from behav- experiences caused by the drug. Therefore, the authors concluded ioural performance only. This is a major improvement, in particular that the MMN in the normal state of consciousness indexes the when considering the special communicational and motivational vulnerability to disruption of the NMDAR system in normal healthy problems with patients, deducting from the reliability of the re- subjects. Consequently, the MMN appears to index the functional sults, and the fact that reliable behavioural measurements are state of the NMDAR-mediated neurotransmission even in subjects not at all possible in some clinical groups and often in small in- demonstrating no psychopathology and may therefore ‘‘become a fants. These new possibilities in particular involve the improved useful tool in studies on NMDAR functioning in humans’’ (Umb- monitoring of the patient’s state and the prediction of its future richt et al., 2002). 444 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Consequently, the MMN attenuation observed in a large num- creases the amount of information that can be gathered within a ber of different clinical populations and conditions (Table 1) can, specific time period. Equally as important are methods that would to a large extent, be accounted for by NMDAR dysfunction associ- further reduce the amount of time required to acquire sufficient ated with different clinical conditions. For instance, as already re- data that reach some objective criteria of identification. viewed, the dysfunction of the NMDAR system plays a central One such approach has recently been described by Rahne et al. role in the brain pathology of schizophrenia and in particular in (2008). In this modification to standard averaging, individual the cognitive deficit in these patients (Javitt et al., 1996; Coyle, epochs to stimulus presentation are sorted on the signal-to-noise 2006). Moreover, the cognitive decline occurring in several other ratio (SNR) to each individual epoch. This epoch with the highest neurocognitive abnormalities might also be accounted for, at least SNR, that is, the lowest background noise, is added first to average. in part, by the dysfunctional NMDAR system (Näätänen et al., Individual epochs are then added to the average in rank order 2011a). Alzheimer’s disease, stroke, traumatic brain injury and based on the background noise in each epoch. Thus, averaging is even normal ageing are accompanied by widespread dysfunctions continued until the SNR is optimised. Objective or statistical detec- in the NMDAR system (Lipton and Rosenberg, 1994; Magnusson tion of these responses could then easily be achieved by using one et al., 2010; Biegon et al., 2004; Dalkara et al., 1996; Izquidero, of a number of online algorithms involving randomisation or boot- 1991; McGeer and McGeer, 2010). For example, Dhawan et al. strapping analyses such as those proposed by, for example, Ponton (2010) recently found that middle central artery occlusion in rat et al. (1997) or Fujioka et al. (2005). Moreover, further progress to resulted in widespread decreases in NMDAR density in brain re- this end has been very recently made by Cong et al., 2009, 2010, gions extending far beyond the infarct area, including even regions 2011); Burger et al. (2007); Kalyakin et al. (2007, 2008a,b, 2009) contralateral to the lesion important to cognitive function. Dhawan and Marco-Pallares et al. (2005). et al. (2010), therefore, concluded that a persistent and widespread Needless to say, a lot of work is still needed to bring the MMN loss of NMDARs may contribute to cognitive and other neurological methodology to clinics as a tool of everyday patient work in which deficits in stroke patients, which cannot be localised to the side of reliable measurements have to be carried out at the level of infarction. individual patients (Bishop and McArthur, 2004; Boly et al., Importantly, this NMDAR functional deficiency may hence ac- 2011; Taylor and Baldeweg, 2002; Lang et al., 1995), but studies count for, besides decreased discrimination accuracy, even the cog- as those reviewed in the foregoing have shown that this goal is nitive and functional decline occurring in a number of these both valuable and probably also attainable. conditions (Table 8) (for a review, see Näätänen et al., 2011a). It is, for instance, well established that the adequate functioning of Acknowledgements the NMDAR system plays a crucial role in the initiation of long- term and working memories (Javitt et al., 1996; Newcomer et al., RN and KK were supported by Grant # 8332 from the Estonian 1998). Therefore, the MMN and MMNm deficiency reflecting a defi- Science Foundation. TK was supported by Grant # 128840 and RN cient NMDAR function can also index cognition and its decrement by grant # 122745 from The Academy of Finland. CE was supported in ageing and in different neurological and neuropsychiatric abnor- by grants from ICREA Academia 2010 and CSD2007-00012, malities (Näätänen et al., 2011a). Consequently, as an easily acces- EUI2009-04806, PSI2009-08063, and SGR2009-11. We wish to sible non-invasive measure of the functional condition of the acknowledge Ms. Piiu Lehmus’ skilful and patient text-editing NMDAR system, the MMN (MMNm) might play an important role work. in future attempts at understanding what is common in the differ- ent neuropsychiatric diseases and abnormalities and what is spe- References cific to each of them (Umbricht et al., 2003). These conclusions are further supported by studies showing that pharmacological Aaltonen O, Tuomainen J, Laine M, Niemi P. Cortical differences in tonal versus manipulations enhancing cognition also enhance the MMN vowel processing as revealed by an ERP component called mismatch negativity. (MMNm) response (Baldeweg et al., 2006; Dunbar et al., 2007; Brain Lang 1993;44:139–52. Ahissar M. Dyslexia and the anchoring-deficit hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci Inami et al., 2005; Fisher et al., 2010; Harkrider and Hedrick, 2007;11:458–65. 2005; Engeland et al., 2002; Knott et al., 2006, 2009) and vice versa Ahissar M, Lubin Y, Putter-Katz H, Banai K. Dyslexia and the failure to form a (Pang and Fowler, 1999; Nakagome et al., 1998; Serra et al., 1996; perceptual anchor. Nat Neurosci 2006;9:1558–64. Ahveninen J, Escera C, Polo MD, Grau C, Jääskeläinen IP. Acute and chronic effects of Jääskeläinen et al., 1996a), and, further, that the MMN-cognition alcohol on pre-attentive auditory processing as reflected by mismatch relationship holds even in normal adults (Light et al., 2007; Bazana negativity. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000a;5:303–11. and Stelmack, 2002; Beauchamp and Stelmack, 2007; Troche et al., Ahveninen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Osipova D, Huttunen MO, Ilmoniemi RJ, Kaprio J, et al. Inherited auditory-cortical dysfunction in twin pairs discordant for 2009, 2010) and children (Liu et al., 2007). schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 2006;60:612–20. Optimising MMN protocols for clinical environments. Perhaps the Ahveninen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Pekkonen E, Hallberg A, Hietanen M, Mäkelä R, et al. most frequent issues associated with use of the MMN in clinical Suppression of mismatch negativity by backward masking predicts impaired working-memory performance in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res environments are time required to record enough data to obtain 1999;23:1507–14. an identifiable response and the expertise required to identify Ahveninen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Pekkonen E, Hallberg A, Hietanen M, Näätänen R, et al. when a response is present or absent. By definition, the traditional Increased distractibility by task-irrelevant sound changes in abstinent alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2000b;24:1850–4. MMN protocol is very time consuming to obtain information Akatsuka K, Wasaka T, Nakata H, Inui K, Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R. Mismatch response regarding even a single stimulus contrast. In a standard paradigm, related to temporal discrimination of somatosensory stimulation. Clin the standard stimulus is presented 80–90% of the time, with devi- Neuropshysiol 2005;116:1930–7. ant stimulus presentations, which evoke the MMN, randomly Akatsuka K, Wasaka T, Nakata H, Kina T, Hoshiyama M, Tamura Y, et al. Objective examination for two-point stimulation using a somatosensory oddball interspersed through the sequence. The introduction of the multi- paradigm: an MEG study. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:403–11. ple-deviant paradigm for tones (Näätänen et al., 2004; Pakarinen Alain C, Woods DL. Age-related changes in processing auditory stimuli during visual et al., 2007, 2010; Fisher et al., 2011; Grimm et al., 2008; Jankowiak attention: evidence for deficits in inhibitory control and sensory memory. Psychol Aging 1999;14:507–19. and Berti, 2007; see also Vaz Pato et al., 2002; Deacon et al., 1998) Alain C, Woods DL, Knight RT. A distributed cortical network for auditory sensory and for speech (Pettigrew et al., 2004b; Pakarinen et al., 2009; memory in humans. Brain Res 1998;812:23–37. Sorokin et al., 2010; Lovio et al., 2009) and music (Vuust et al., Alain C, McDonald KL, Ostroff JM, Schneider B. Aging: a switch from automatic to controlled processing of sounds? Psychol Aging 2004;19:125–33. 2011) provided a method for rapidly acquiring information on a Alexandrov AA, Karpina NV, Stankevich LN. Mismatch negativity in evoked brain number of stimulus contrasts. This approach considerably in- potentials in adolescents in normal conditions and attention deficit in response R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 445

to presentation of short-duration acoustic stimuli. Neurosci Behav Physiol Baudena P, Halgren E, Heit G, Clarke JM. Intracerebral potentials to rare target and 2003;33:671–5. distractor auditory and visual stimuli. III. Frontal cortex. Electroencephalogr Alho K, Cheour-Luhtanen M. Auditory discrimination in infants as revealed by the Clin Neurophysiol 1995;94:251–64. mismatch negativity of the event-related brain potential. Dev Neuropsychol Bazana PG, Stelmack RM. Intelligence and information processing during an 1997;13:157–65. auditory discrimination task with backward masking: an event-related Alho K, Kujala T, Paavilainen P, Summala H, Näätänen R. Auditory processing in the potential analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002;83:998–1008. visual brain areas of the early blind: evidence from event-related potentials. Beauchamp CM, Stelmack RM. The chronometry of mental ability: an event-related Electroen Clin Neuro 1993;86:418–27. potential analysis of an auditory oddball discrimination task. Intelligence Alho K, Sainio K, Sajaniemi N, Reinikainen K, Näätänen R. Event-related brain 2006;34:571–86. potential of human newborns to pitch change of an acoustic stimulus. Electroen Becker F, Reinvang I. Mismatch negativity (MMN) to tone duration and speech Clin Neuro 1990;77:151–5. sound changes in aphasics. Brain Lang 2007;100:69–78. Alho K, Woods DL, Algazi A, Knight RT, Näätänen R. Lesions of frontal cortex Bellis TJ, Nicol T, Kraus N. Aging affects hemispheric asymmetry in the neural diminish the auditory mismatch negativity. Electroen Clin Neuro representation of speech sounds. J Neurosci 2000;20:791–7. 1994;91:353–62. Benasich AA, Choudhury N, Friedman JT, Realpe-Bonilla T, Chojnowska C, Gou Z. The Alho K, Woods DL, Algazi A, Näätänen R. Intermodal selective attention II: effects of infant as a prelinguistic model for language learning impairments: predicting attentional load on processing auditory and visual stimuli in central space. from event-related potentials to behaviour. Neuropsychologia 2006;44: Electroen Clin Neuro 1992;82:356–68. 396–411. Alonso-Búa B, Dı´ az F, Ferraces J. The contribution of AERPs (MMN and LDN) to Benasich AA, Tallal P. Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later studying temporal vs. linguistic processing deficits in children with reading language impairment. Behav Brain Res 2002;136:31–49. difficulties. Int J Psychophysiol 2006;59:159–67. Berk M, Copolov DL, Dean O, Lu K, Jeavons S, Schapkaitz I, et al. N-Acetyl Cysteine for Amenedo E, Diaz F. Automatic and effortful processes in auditory memory reflected depressive symptoms in bipolar disorder-a double-blind randomized placebo- by event-related potentials. Age-related findings. Electroen Clin Neuro control trial. Biol Psychiat 2008a;64:468–75. 1998;108:361–9. Berk M, Copolov D, Dean O, Lu K, Jeavons S, Schapkaitz I, et al. N-acetyl cysteine as a Amenedo E, Escera C. The accuracy of sound duration representation in the human glutathione precursor for schizophrenia – a double-blind, randomized, placebo- brain determines the accuracy of behavioural perception. Eur J Neurosci controlled trial. Biology 2008b;64:361–8. 2000;12:2570–4. Berti S, Schröger E. A comparison of auditory and visual distraction effects: Amenedo E, Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F. Vertical asymmetries in pre-attentive behavioural and event-related indices. Cogn Brain Res 2001;10:265–73. detection of changes in motion direction. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;64:184–9. Berti S, Schröger E. Distraction effects in vision: behavioural and event-related Andersson S, Barder HE, Hellvin T, Løvdahl H, Malt UF. Neuropsychological and potentials indices. NeuroReport 2004;15:665–9. electrophysiological indices of neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar II disorder. Berti S, Schröger E. Visual distraction: a behavioural and event-related brain Bipol Disord 2008;10:888–99. potential study in humans. NeuroReport 2006;2:151–5. Astikainen P, Hietanen JK. Event-related potentials to task-irrelevant changes in Bertoli S, Smurzynski J, Probst R. Temporal resolution in young and elderly subjects facial expressions. Behav Brain Funct 2009;5:30. as measured by mismatch negativity and a psychoacoustic gap detection task. Astikainen P, Ruusuvirta T, Korhonen T. Somatosensory event-related potentials in Clin Neurophysiol 2002;113:396–406. the rabbit cerebral and cerebellar cortices: a correspondence with mismatch Bertoli S, Smurzynski J, Probst R. Effects of age, age-related hearing loss, and responses in humans. Neurosci Lett 2001;298:222–4. contralateral cafeteria noise on the discrimination of small frequency changes: Astikainen P, Ruusuvirta T, Wikgren J, Penttonen M. Memory-based detection of rare psychoacoustic and electrophysiological measures. J Assoc Res Otolaryng sound feature combinations in anaesthetized rats. NeuroReport 2006;17:1561–4. 2005;6:207–22. Atienza M, Cantero JL. Complex sound processing during human REM sleep by Beste C, Saft C, Gunturkun O, Falkenstein M. Increased cognitive functioning in recovering information from long-term memory as revealed by the mismatch symptomatic Huntington’s Disease as revealed by behavioral and event-related negativity (MMN). Brain Res 2001;901:151–60. potential indices of auditory sensory memory and attention. J Neurosci Atienza M, Cantero JL, Gomez CM. The mismatch negativity component reveals the 2008;28:11695–702. sensory memory during REM sleep in humans. Neurosci Lett 1997;237:21–4. Biegon A, Fry PA, Paden CM, Alexandrowitch A, Tsenter J, Shohami E. Dynamic Atienza M, Cantero JL, Gomez CM. Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace changes in N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors after closed head injury in mice: during wakefulness and REM sleep. Psychophysiology 2000;37:485–93. implications for treatment of neurological and cognitive deficits. P Natl Acad Sci Atienza M, Cantero JL, Dominguez-Marin E. Mismatch negativity (MMN): an USA 2004;101:5117–22. objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep. Bishop DVM. Curing dyslexia and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder by training Int J Psychophysiol 2002;46:215–25. motor co-ordination: miracle or myth? J Paediatr Child H 2007a;43:653–5. Auther LL, Wertz RT, Miller TA, Kirshner HS. Relationships among the mismatch Bishop DVM. Genes, cognition, and communication. Insights from negativity (MMN) response, auditory comprehension, and site of lesion in neurodevelopmental disorders. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2009. Ann aphasic adults. Aphasiology 2000;14:461–70. NY Acad Sci 2009;1156:1–18. Baker K, Baldeweg T, Sivagnanasundaram S, Scambler P, Skuse D. COMTVal108/ Bishop DVM. Using mismatch negativity to study central auditory processing in 1581653 Met polymorphism modifies mismatch negativity and cognitive developmental language and literacy impairments: where are we, and where function in 22q11 deletion syndrome. Biol Psychiat 2005;58:23–31. should we be going? Psychol Bull 2007b;133:651–72. Baldeweg T, Klugman A, Gruzelier JH, Hirsch SR. Impairment in frontal but not Bishop DVM, McArthur GM. Immature cortical responses to auditory stimuli in temporal components of mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Int J specific language impairment: evidence from ERPs to rapid tone sequences. Psychophysiol 2002;43:111–22. DevSci 2004;7:F11–8. Baldeweg T, Klugman A, Gruzelier J, Hirsch SR. Mismatch negativity potentials and Bisiachi PS, Mento G, Suppiej A. Cortical auditory processing in preterm newborns: cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2004;69:203–17. an ERP study. Biol Psychol 2009;82:176–85. Baldeweg T, Richardson A, Watkins S, Foale C, Gruzelier J. Impaired auditory Blitz U, Gust K, Spitzer M, Kiefer M. Phonological deficit in school children is frequency discrimination in dyslexia detected with mismatch evoked reflected in the mismatch negativity. NeuroReport 2007;18:911–5. potentials. Ann Neurol 1999;45:1–9. Boatman DF, Trescher WH, Smith C, Ewen J, Los J, Wied HM, et al. Cortical auditory Baldeweg T, Wong D, Stephen KE. Nicotinic modulation of human auditory sensory dysfunction in benign rolandic epilepsy. Epilepsia 2008;49:1018–26. memory: evidence from mismatch negativity potentials. Int J Psychophysiol Bodatsch M, Ruhrmann S, Wagner M, Müller R, Schultze-Lutter F, Frommann I, et al. 2006;59:49–58. Prediction of psychosis by mismatch negativity. Biol Psychiat 2011;69:959–66. Bamiou DE, Liasis A, Boyd S, Cohen M, Raglan E. Central auditory processing Bogerts B. Recent advances in the neuropathology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull disorder as the presenting manifestation of subtle brain pathology. J Audiol 1993;19:431–45. 2000;39:168–72. Boly M, Garrido MI, Gosseries O, Bruno M-A, Boveroux P, Schnakers C, et al. Banai K, Ahissar M. Psychoacoustics and working memory in dyslexia. In: Fourth Preserved feedforward but impaired top-down processes in the vegetative conference on plasticity of the central auditory system and processing of state. Science 2011;332:858–62. complex acoustic signals, July 07–10, 2003. Plasticity and Signal Representation Bomba MD, Pang EW. Cortical auditory evoked potentials in autism: a review. Int J in the Auditory System, Prague, Czech Republic; 2005. p. 233–42. Psychophysiol 2004;53:161–9. Banati RB. Neuropathological imaging: in vivo detection of glial activation as a Bonnel AC, Mottron L, Peretz I, Trudel M, Gallun E, Bonnel AM. Enhanced pitch measure of disease and adaptive change in the brain. Br Med Bull sensitivity in individuals with autism: a signal detection analysis. J Cognit 2003;65:121–31. Neurosci 2003;15:1–10. Banati R, Hickie IB. Therapeutic signposts: using biomarkers to guide better Bonte ML, Mitterer H, Zellagui N, Poelmans H, Blomert L. Auditory cortical tuning to treatment of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders. Med J Aust statistical regularities in phonology. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:2765–74. 2009;190:S26–32. Bonte ML, Poelmans H, Blomert L. Deviant neurophysiological responses to Bar-Haim Y, Marshall PJ, Fox NA, Schorr EA, Gordon-Salant S. Mismatch negativity phonological regularities in speech in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia in socially withdrawn children. Biol Psychiat 2003;54:17–24. 2007;45:1427–37. Barry JG, Hardiman MJ, Line E, White KB, Yasin I, Bishop DVM. Duration of auditory Borghetti D, Pizzanelli C, Maritato P, Fabbrini M, Jensen S, Iudice A, et al. Mismatch sensory memory in parents of children with SLI: a mismatch negativity study. negativity analysis in drug-resistant epileptic patients implanted with vagus Brain Lang 2008;104:75–88. nerve stimulator. Brain Res Bull 2007;73:81–5. Barry RJ, Johnstone SJ, Clarke AR. A review of electrophysiology in attention-deficit/ Born J, Bothor R, Pietrowsky R, Fehm-Wolfsdorf G, Pauschinger P, Fehm HL. hyperactivity disorder: II. Event-related potentials. Clin Neurophysiol Influences of vasopressin and oxytocin on human event-related potentials in an 2003;114:184–98. attention task. J Psychophysiol 1987a;4:351–60. 446 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Born J, Brüninger W, Fehm-Wolsdorf G, Pauschinger P, Fehm HL. Dose-dependent Cheour M, Martynova O, Näätänen R, Erkkola R, Sillanpää M, Kero P, et al. Speech influences on electrophysiological signs of attention in humans after sounds learned by sleeping newborns. Nature 2002a;415:599–600. neuropeptide ACTH 4-10. Exp Brain Res 1987b;67:85–92. Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Leppänen P, Alho K, Kujala T, Renlund M, et al. Auditory Born J, Fehm-Wolsdorf G, Lutzenberger W, Voigt KH, Fehm HL. Vasopressin and sensory memory trace decays rapidly in newborns. Scand J Psychol electrophysiological signs of attention in man. Peptides 1986;7:189–93. 2002b;43:33–9. Born J, Pietrowsky R, Fehm HL. Neuropsychological effects on vasopressin in healthy Cheour-Luhtanen M, Alho K, Sainio K, Rinne T, Reinikainen K, Pohjavuori M, et al. humans. Prog Brain Res 1998;119:619–43. The ontogenetically earliest discriminative response of the human brain. Böttcher-Gandor C, Ullsperger P. Mismatch negativity in event-related potentials of Psychophysiol Special Rep 1996;33:478–81. auditory stimuli as a function of varying interstimulus interval. Choudhury N, Benasich AA. Maturation of auditory evoked potentials from 6 to 48 Psychophysiology 1992;29:546–50. months: prediction to 3 and 4 year language and cognitive abilities. Clin Bradlow AR, Kraus N, Nicol TG, McGee TJ, Cunningham J, Zecker SG. Effects of Neurophysiol 2011;122:320–38. lengthened formant transition duration on discrimination and neural Cong F, Kalyakin I, Huttunen-Scott T, Li H, Lyytinen H, Ristaniemi T. Single-trial representation of synthetic syllables by normal and learning-disabled based independent component analysis on mismatch negativity of children. Int children. J Acoust Soc Am 1999;106:2086–96. J Neural Syst 2010;20:279–92. Braff DL, Light GA. Preattentional and attentional cognitive deficits as targets for Cong F, Ristaniemi T, Lyytinen H. A potential real-time procedure to evaluate treating schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology 2004;174:75–85. correlation of recordings among single trials (CoRaST) for mismatch negativity Bramon E, Croft RJ, McDonald C, Virdi GK, Gruzelier JG, Baldeweg T, et al. Mismatch (MMN) with Fourier transformation. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;4:725–30. negativity in schizophrenia: a family study. Schizophr Res 2004;67:1–10. Cong F, Sipola T, Huttunen-Scott T, Xu X, Ristaniemi T, Lyytinen H. Hilbert-Huang Brattico E, Kujala T, Tervaniemi M, Alku P, Ambrosi L, Monitillo V. Long-term versus Morlet wavelet transformation on mismatch negativity of children in exposure to occupational noise alters the cortical organization of sound uninterrupted sound paradigm. Nonlinear Biomed Phys 2009;3:1. processing. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:190–203. Cooper RJ, Todd J, McGill K, Michie PT. Auditory sensory memory and the aging Brattico E, Tervaniemi M, Välimäki V, van Zuijen T, Peretz I. Cortical correlates of brain: a mismatch negativity study. Neurobiol Aging 2006;27:752–62. acquired deafness to dissonance. Ann NY Acad Sci 2003;999:158–60. Corbera S, Corral M-J, Escera C, Idiazábal MA. Abnormal speech sound Braun A, McArdie J, Jones J, Nechaev V, Zalewski C, Brewer C, et al. Tune deafness: representation in persistent developmental stuttering. Neurology 2005;65: processing melodic errors outside of conscious awareness as reflected by 1246–52. components of the auditory ERP. PloS ONE 2008;3:e2349. Corbera S, Escera C, Artigas J. Impaired duration mismatch negativity in Brockhaus-Dumke A, Tendolkar I, Pukrop R, Schultze-Lutter F, Klosterkötter J, developmental dyslexia. NeuroReport 2006;17:1051–5. Ruhrmann S. Impaired mismatch negativity generation in prodromal subjects Cornwell BR, Baas JMP, Johnson L, Holroyd T, Carver FW, Lissek S, et al. Neural and patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005;73:297–310. responses to auditory stimulus deviance under threat of electric shock revealed Brønnick KS, Nordby H, Larsen JP, Aarsland D. Disturbance of automatic auditory by spatially-filtered magnetoencephalography. NeuroImage 2007;37:383–9. change detection in dementia associated with parkinson’s disease: a mismatch Coyle JT. Glutamate and schizophrenia: beyond the dopamine hypothesis. Cell Mol negativity study. Neurobiol Aging 2010;31:104–13. Neurobiol 2006;26:365–84. Bruder J, Leppänen PHT, Bartling J, Csépe V, Démonet J-F, Schulte-Körne G. Children Csépe V, Dyslexia. Different Brain, Different behaviour. In: Csépe V, editor. with dyslexia reveal abnormal native language representations: evidence from Neuropsychology and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic Publisher; a study of mismatch negativity. Psychophysiology 2011a;48:1107–18. 2002. p. 81–112. Bruder J, Leppänen PHT, Bartling J, Csépe V, Démonet J-F, Schulte-Körne G. An Csépe V, Karmos G, Molnár M. correlates of stimulus deviance investigation of prototypical and atypical within-category vowels and non- during wakefulness and sleep in cat: Animal model of mismatch negativity. speech analogues on cortical auditory evoked related potentials (AERPs) in 9 Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1987;66:571–8. year old children. Int J Psychophysiol 2011b;79:106–17. Csépe V, Karmos G, Molnár M. Subcortical evoked potential correlates of sensory Burger M, Hoppe U, Kummer P, Lohscheller J, Eysholdt U, Dollinger M. Wavelet- mismatch process in cats. In: Bajic M, editor. Advances in based analysis of MMN responses in children. Biomed Tech 2007;52:111–6. biosciences. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 1988. p. 43–6. Campbell K, Bell I, Bastien C. Evoked potential measures of information processing Csépe V, Karmos G, Molnár M, Winkler I. Cortical and intracerebral mismatch during natural sleep. In: Broughton RJ, Ogilvie RD, editors. Sleep arousal and negativity - animal-model of stimulus comparison. Int J Psychophysiol performance. Cambridge, MA: Birkhauser, Boston; 1991. p. 88–116. 1989;7:166–8. Cansino S. Episodic memory decay along the adult lifespan: a review of behavioral Csépe V, Osman-Sági J, Molnár M, Gósy M. Impaired speech perception in aphasic and neurophysiological evidence. Int J Psychophysiol 2009;71:64–9. patients: event-related potential and neurophysiological assessment. Carral V, Huotilainen M, Ruusuvirta T, Fellman V, Näätänen R, Escera C. A kind of Neuropsychologia 2001;39:1194–208. auditory ‘primitive intelligence’ already present at birth. Eur J Neurosci Czamara D, Bruder J, Becker J, Bartling J, Hoffman P, Ludwig KU, et al. Association of 2005;21:3201–4. a rare variant with mismatch negativity in a region between KIAA0319 and Catts SV, Shelley A-M, Ward PB, Liebert B, McConaghy N, Andrews S, et al. Brain DCDC2 in dyslexia. Behav Genet 2011;41:110–9. potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia. Am Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity: violation of nonattended environmental J Psychiat 1995;152:213–9. regularities. J Psychophysiol 2007;21:224–30. Celsis P, Boulanouar K, Doyon B, Ranjeva JP, Berry I, Nespoulous JL, et al. Differential Czigler I, Csibra G. Event-related potentials in a visual discrimination task: negative fMRI responses in the left posterior superior temporal gyrus and left waves related to detection and attention. Psychophysiology 1990;27:669–76. supramarginal gyrus to habituation and change detection in syllables and Czigler I, Csibra G. Event-related potentials and the identification of deviant visual tones. NeuroImage 1999;9:135–44. stimuli. Psychophysiology 1992;29:471–84. Ceponiene R, Haapanen M-L, Ranta R, Näätänen R, Hukki J. Auditory sensory Czigler I, Pató L. Unnoticed regularity violation elicits change-related brain activity. impairment in children with oral clefts as indexed by event-related potentials: Biol Psychol 2009;80:339–47. a review. J Craniofac Surg 2002;13:554–66. Czigler I, Balázs L, Pató LG. Visual change detection: event-related potentials are Ceponiene R, Hukki J, Cheour M, Haapanen M-L, Koskinen M, Alho K, et al. dependent on stimulus location in humans. Neurosci Lett 2004;364:149–53. Dysfunction of the auditory cortex persists in infants with certain cleft types. Czigler I, Balázs L, Winkler I. Memory-based detection of task-irrelevant visual Dev Med Child Neurol 2000;42:258–65. changes. Psychophysiology 2002;39:869–73. Ceponiene R, Hukki J, Cheour M, Haapanen M-L, Ranta R, Näätänen R. Cortical Czigler I, Csibra G, Csontos A. Age and inter-stimulus interval effects on event- auditory dysfunction in children with oral clefts: relation with cleft type. Clin related potentials to frequent and infrequent auditory-stimuli. Biol Psychol Neurophysiol 1999;110:1921–6. 1992;33:195–206. Ceponiene R, Lepistö T, Shestakova A, Vanhala R, Näätänen R, Yaguchi K. Speech Czigler I, Weisz J, Winkler I. ERPs and deviance detection: visual mismatch sound-selective auditory impairment in infantile autism: can perceive but will negativity to repeated visual stimuli. Neurosci Lett 2006a;401:178–82. not attend. P Natl Acad Sci USA 2003;100:5567–72. Czigler I, Winkler I, Pató L, Várnagy A, Weisz J, Balázs L. Visual temporal window of Chang Y, Xu J, Shi N, Zhang B, Zhao L. Dysfunction of processing task-irrelevant integration as revealed by the mismatch negativity event-related potential to emotional faces in major depressive disorder patients revealed by expression- stimulus omission. Brain Res 2006b;1104:129–40. related visual MMN. Neurosci Lett 2010;472:33–7. Dalkara T, Ayata C, Demirci M, Erdemli G, Onur R. Effects of cerebral ischemia on N- Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Hukki J, Haapanen M-L, Näätänen R, Alho K. Brain methyl-D-aspartate and dihydropyridine-sensitive calcium currents: an dysfunction in neonates with cleft palate revealed by the mismatch negativity electrophysiological study in the rat hippocampus in situ. Stroke (MMN). Electroen Clin Neuro 1999;110:324–8. 1996;27:127–33. Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Lehtokoski A, Luuk A, Allik J, Alho K, et al. Development of Daltrozzo J, Wioland N, Mutschler V, Kotchoubey B. Predicting coma and other low language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nature Neurosci responsive patients’ outcome using event-related potentials: a meta-analysis. 1998b;1:351–3. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:606–14. Cheour M, Haapanen M-L, Ceponiene R, Hukki J, Ranta S, Näätänen R. Mismatch Davalos DB, Kisley MA, Polk SD, Ross RG. Mismatch negativity in detection of negativity (MMN) as an index of auditory sensory memory deficit in cleft-palate interval duration deviation in schizophrenia. NeuroReport 2003;14:1283–6. and catch-syndrome children. NeuroReport 1998a;9:2709–12. Deacon D, Nousak JM, Pilotti M, Ritter W, Yang CM. Automatic change detection: Cheour M, Haapanen M-L, Hukki J, Ceponiene R, Kurjenluoma S, Alho K, et al. The does the auditory system use representations of individual stimulus features or first neurophysiological evidence for cognitive brain dysfunctions in CATCH gestalts? Psychophysiology 1998;35:413–9. children. NeuroReport 1997;8:1785–7. Dehaene-Lambertz G. Electrophysiological correlates of categorical phoneme Cheour M, Leppänen P, Kraus N. Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for perception in adults. NeuroReport 1997;8:919–24. investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and Dehaene-Lambertz G, Pena M, Christphe A, Landrieu P. Phoneme perception in a children. Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111:4–16. neonate with left sylvian infarct. Brain Lang 2004;88:26–38. R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 447

Deouell LY. The frontal generator of the mismatch negativity revisited. J Fein G, Whitlow B, Finn P. Mismatch negativity: no difference between controls and Psychophysiol 2007;27:188–203. abstinent alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2004;28:137–42. Deouell LY, Hämäläinen H, Bentin S. Unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere Fellman V, Huotilainen M. Cortical event-related potentials in newborn infants. damage: contributions from event-related potentials. Audiol Neuro-Otol Semin Fetal Neonat Med 2006;11:452–8. 2000a;5:225–34. Fellman V, Kushnerenko E, Mikkola K, Ceponiene R, Leipälä J, Näätänen R. Atypical Deouell LY, Bentin S, Soroker N. Electrophysiological evidence for an early (pre- auditory event-related potentials in preterm infants – a possible sign of attentive) information processing deficit in patients with right hemisphere cognitive dysfunction? Pediatr Res 2004;56:291–7. damage and unilateral neglect. Brain 2000b;123:353–65. Ferri R, Elia M, Agarwal N, Lanuzza B, Musumeci SA, Pennisi G. The mismatch Desjardin RN, Trainor LJ, Hevenor S, Polak CP. Using mismatch negativity to negativity and the P3a components of the auditory event-related potentials in measure auditory temporal resolution thresholds. NeuroReport autistic low-functioning subjects. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:1671–80. 1999;10:2079–82. Fischer C, Luauté J. Evoked potentials for the prediction of vegetative state in the de Tommaso M, Guido M, Libro G, Losito L, Difruscolo O, Sardaro M, et al. Interictal acute stage of coma. Neuropsychol Rehab 2005;15:372–80. lack of habituation of mismatch negativity in migraine. Cephalalgia Fischer C, Dailler F, Morlet D. Novelty P3 elicited by the subject’s own name in 2004;24:663–8. comatose patients. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;19:2224–30. Devrim-Ücok M, Keskin-Ergen HY, Ücok A. Mismatch negativity at acute and post- Fischer C, Luauté J, Adelaine P, Morlet D. Predictive value of sensory and cognitive acute phases of first-episode schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psy Clin N evoked potentials for awakening from coma. Neurology 2004;63:669–73. 2008;258:179–85. Fischer C, Luauté J, Némoz C, Morlet D, Kirkorian G, Mauguière F. Improved Dhawan J, Benveniste H, Nawrocky M, Smith SD, Biegon A. Transient focal ischemia prediction of awakening or nonawakening from severe anoxic coma using tree- results in persistent and widespread neuroinflammation and loss of glutamate based classification analysis. Crit Care Med 2006a;34:1–5. NMDA receptors. NeuroImage 2010;51:599–605. Fischer C, Luauté J, Némoz C, Morlet D, Kirkorian G, Mauguière F. Editorial response: Diaz F, Zurron M. Auditory evoked potentials in Down’s syndrome. Electroen Clin evoked potentials can be used as prognosis factor for awakening. Crit Care Med Neuro 1995;96:526–37. 2006b;34:2025. Dick BD, Connolly JF, McGrath PJ, Finley GA, Stroink G, Houlihan ME, et al. The Fischer C, Morlet D, Bouchet P, Luaute J, Jourdan C, Salord F. Mismatch negativity disruptive effect of chronic pain on mismatch negativity. Clin Neurophysiol and late auditory evoked potentials in comatose patients. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:1497–506. 1999;110:1601–10. Dittmann-Balcar A, Juptner M, Jentzen W, Schall U. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex Fischer C, Morlet D, Giard M-H. Mismatch negativity and in comatose activation during automatic auditory duration mismatch processing in humans: patients. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:192–7. a positron emission tomography study. Neurosci Lett 2001;308:119–212. Fisher AE, Barnes GR, Hillebrand A, Holliday IE, Witton C, Richards IL. Abnormality Doeller CF, Opitz B, Mecklinger A, Krick C, Reith W, Schröger E. Prefrontal cortex of mismatch negativity in response to tone omission in dyslexic adults. Brain involvement in preattentive auditory deviance detection: neuroimaging and Res 2006;1077:90–8. electrophysiological evidence. NeuroImage 2003;20:1270–82. Fisher DJ, Grant B, Smith DM, Borracci G, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Effects of auditory Dolan RJ, Bench CJ, Liddle PF, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Grasby M, et al. Dorsolateral hallucinations on the mismatch negativity (MMN) in schizophrenia as prefrontal cortex dysfunction in the major psychoses – symptom or disease measured by a modified ‘‘optimal’’ multi-feature paradigm. Int J specificity. J Neurol Neurosur Ps 1993;56:1290–4. Psychophysiol 2011;81:245–51. Draganova R, Eswaran H, Murphy P, Huotilainen M, Lowerey C, Preissl H. Sound Fisher DJ, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Auditory hallucinations and the mismatch negativity: frequency change detection in fetuses and newborns, a processing speech and non-speech sounds in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol magnetoencephalographic study. NeuroImage 2005;28:354–61. 2008;70:3–15. Draganova R, Eswaran H, Murphy P, Lowery C, Preissl H. Serial Fisher DJ, Labelle A, Knott VJ. The right profile: mismatch negativity in magnetoencephalographic study of fetal and newborn auditory discriminative schizophrenia with and without auditory hallucinations as measured by a evoked responses. Early Hum Dev 2007;83:199–207. multi-feature paradigm. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:909–21. Dulude L, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Acute nicotine alteration of sensory memory Fisher DJ, Lynne Scott T, Shah DK, Prise S, Thompson M, Knott VJ. Light up and see: impairment in smokers with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol enhancement of the visual mismatch negativity (vMMN) by nicotine. Brain Res 2010;5:541–8. 2010;1313:162–71. Duman O, Kizilay F, Fettahoglu C, Ozkaynak S, Haspolat S. Electrophysiologic and Fisher DJ, Grant B, Smith DM, Knott WJ. Effects of deviant probability on the neuropsychologic evaluation of patients with centrotemporal spikes. Int J ‘‘optimal’’ multi-feature mismatch negativity (MMN) paradigm. Int J Neurosci 2008;118:118–22. Psychophysiol 2011;79:311–5. Dunbar G, Boieijinga PH, Demazières A, Cisterni C, Kuchibhatla R, Wesnes K, et al. Franken IHA, Nijs I, Van Strien JW. Impulsivity affects mismatch negativity (MMN) Effects of TC.1734 (AZD3480), a selective neuronal nicotinic receptor measures. Biol Psychol 2005;70:161–7. antagonist, on cognitive performance and the EEG of young healthy male Friedrich M, Weber C, Friederici AD. Electrophysiological evidence for delayed volunteers. Psychopharmacology 2007;191:919–29. mismatch response in infants at-risk for specific language impairment. Duncan CC, Kaye WH. Effects of clonidine on event-related potential measures of Psychophysiology 2004;41:772–82. information processing. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophys 1987(Suppl. 40): Frodl-Bauch T, Kathmann N, Möller H-J, Hegerl U. Dipole localization and test– 527–31. retest reliability of frequency and duration mismatch negativity generator Duncan CC, Barry RJ, Connolly JF, Fischer C, Michie PT, Näätänen R, et al. Event- processes. Brain Topogr 1997;10:3–8. related potentials in clinical research: guidelines for eliciting, recording, and Froyen D, van Atteveldt N, Blomert L. Exploring the role of low level visual quantifying mismatch negativity, , and . Clin Neurophysiol processing in letter-speech sound integration: a visual MMN study. Front Integr 2009;120:1883–908. Neurosci 2010;4:9. Dunn MA, Gomes H, Gravel J. Mismatch negativity in children with autism and Froyen D, van Atteveldt N, Bonte M, Blomert L. Crossmodal enhancement of the typical development. J Autism Dev Disord 2008;38:52–71. MMN to speech sounds indicates early and automatic integration of letters and Ehrlichman RS, Maxwell CR, Majumdar S, Siegel SJ. Deviance-elicited changes in speech sounds. Neurosci Lett 2008;430:23–8. event-related potentials are attenuated by ketamine in mice. J Cognit Neurosci Froyen D, Bonte M, van Atteveldt N, Blomert L. The long road to automation: 2008;20:1403–14. neurocognitive development of letter-speech sound processing. J Cognit Ehrlichman R, Luminais S, White S, Rudnick ND, Ma N, Dow HC, et al. Neuregulin 1 Neurosci 2009;21:567–80. transgenic mice display reduced mismatch negativity, contextual fear Froyen D, Willems G, Blomert L. Evidence for a specific cross-modal association conditioning and social interactions. Brain Res 2009;1294:116–27. deficit in dyslexia: an electrophysiological study of letter-speech sound Engeland C, Mahoney C, Mohr E, Ilivitsky V, Knott VJ. Acute nicotine effects on processing. Dev Sci 2010;14:635–8. auditory sensory memory in tacrine-treated and nontreated patients with Fujioka T, Trainor LJ, Ross B, Kakigi R, Pantev C. Automatic encoding of polyphonic Alzheimer’s disease. An event-related potential study. Pharmacol Biochem Be melodies in musicians and non-musicians. J Cognit Neurosci 2005;17:1578–92. 2002;72:457–64. Ge Y, Wu J, Sun X, Zhang K. Enhanced mismatch negativity in adolescents with Escera C, Alho K, Schröger E, Winkler I. Involuntary attention and distractibility as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Int J Psychophysiol 2011;79:231–5. evaluated with event-related brain potentials. Audiol Neuro-Otol Gaeta H, Friedman D, Ritter W, Cheng J. An event-related potential study of age- 2000a;5:151–66. related changes in sensitivity to stimulus deviance. Neurobiol Aging Escera C, Corral MJ. Role of mismatch negativity and novelty-P3 in involuntary 1998;19:447–59. auditory attention. J Psychophysiol 2007;21:251–64. Gaeta H, Friedman D, Ritter W, Cheng J. Changes in sensitivity to stimulus deviance Escera C, Corral MJ, Yago E. An electrophysiological and behavioral investigation of in Alzheimer’s disease: an ERP perspective. NeuroReport 1999;10:281–7. involuntary attention towards auditory frequency, duration and intensity Gaeta H, Friedman D, Ritter W, Cheng J. An event-related potential evaluation of changes. Cogn Brain Res 2002;14:325–32. involuntary attentional shifts in young and older adults. Psychol Aging Escera C, Grau C. Short-term replicability of the mismatch negativity. Electroen Clin 2001;16:55–68. Neuro 1996;100:549–54. Gaeta H, Friedman D, Ritter W, Hunt G. Age-related changes in neural trace Escera C, Yago E, Polo MD, Grau C. The individual replicability of mismatch generation of rule-based auditory features. Neurobiol Aging 2002;23:443–55. negativity at short and long inter-stimulus intervals. Clin Neurophysiol Ge Y, Wu J, Sun X, Zhang K. Enhanced mismatch negativity in adolescents with 2000b;111:546–51. posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Int J Psychophysiol 2011;79:231–5. Fabiani M, Low KA, Wee E, Sable JJ, Gratton G. Reduced suppression or labile Gene-Cos N, Pottinger R, Barrett G, Trimble M, Ring HA. A comparative study of memory? Mechanisms of inefficient filtering of irrelevant information in older mismatch negativity (MMN) in epilepsy and non-epilectic seizures. Epileptic adults. J Cognit Neurosci 2006;18:637–50. Disord 2005;7:363–72. 448 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Giard MH, Perrin F, Pernier J, Bouchet P. Brain generators implicated in processing of Hall M-H, Schulze K, Rijsdijk F, Kalitindi S, McDonald C, Bramon E, et al. Are auditory auditory stimulus deviance: a topographic event-related potential study. P300 and duration MMN heritable and putative endophenotypes of psychotic Psychophysiology 1990;27:627–40. bipolar disorder? A Maudsley bipolar twin and family study. Psychol Med Giese-Davis JE, Miller GA, Knight RA. Memory template comparison processes in 2009;39:1277–87. anhedonia and dysthymia. Psychophysiology 1993;30:646–56. Hall M-H, Rijsdijk F, Kalitinidi S, Schulze K, Kravariti E, Kane F, et al. Genetic overlap Gil R, Zai L, Neau JP, Jonveaux T, Agbo C, Rosolacc T, et al. Event-related auditory between bipolar illness and event-related potentials. Psychol Med evoked potentials and multiple sclerosis. Electroen Clin Neuro 1993;88:182–7. 2007;37:667–78. Glass E, Sachse S, von Suchodoletz W. Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-old Hall MH, Schulze K, Rijsdijk F, Piccioni M, Ettinger U, Bramon E, et al. Heritability children: an event-related potential study. NeuroReport 2008a;19:569–73. and reliability of P300, and duration mismatch negativity. Behav Genet Glass E, Sachse S, von Suchodoletz W. Development of auditory sensory memory 2006;36:845–57. from 2 to 6 years: an MMN study. J Neural Trans 2008b;115:1221–9. Hämäläinen H, Kujala T, Kekoni J, Hurskainen H, Pirilä J, Wikström H, et al. Effects of Gomes H, Molholm S, Ritter W, Kurtzberg D, Cowan N, Vaughan Jr HG. Mismatch unilateral hippocampus – amygdala – partial temporal lobe resection on negativity in children and adults, and effects of an attended task. auditory EEG/MEG responses: a case study. Scand J Psychol 2007;48:367–73. Psychophysiology 2000;37:807–16. Hämäläinen JA, Leppänen PHT, Guttorm TK, Lyyytinen H. Event-related potentials to Gomes H, Sussman E, Ritter W, Kurtzberg D, Cowan N, Vaughan Jr HG. pitch and rise time change in children with reading disabilities and typically Electrophysiological evidence of developmental changes in the duration of reading children. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:100–15. auditory sensory memory. Dev Psychol 1999;35:294–302. Hämäläinen M, Hari R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Knuutila J, Lounasmaa OV. Gomot M, Bruneau N, Laurent J-P, Barthélémy C, Saliba E. Left temporal impairment Magnetoencephalography – theory, instrumentation, and applications to of auditory information processing in prematurely born 9-year-old children: an noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Rev Mod Phys electrophysiological study. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;64:123–9. 1993;65:413–97. Gomot M, Giard M-H, Adrien J-L, Barthélémy C, Bruneau N. Hypersensitivity to Hanagasi HA, Gurvit IH, Ermutlu N, Kaptanoglu G, Karamursel S, Idrisoglu HA, et al. acoustic change in children with autism: electrophysiological evidence of left Cognitive impairment in amyotropic lateral sclerosis: evidence from frontal cortex dysfunctioning. Psychophysiology 2002;39:577–84. neuropsychological investigation and event-related potentials. Cognit Brain Gomot M, Giard M-H, Roux S, Barthélémy C, Bruneau N. Maturation of frontal and Res 2002;14:234–44. temporal components of mismatch negativity (MMN) in children. NeuroReport Hansenne M, Pinto E, Scantamburlo G, Couvreur A, Reggers J, Fuchs S, et al. 2000;11:3109–12. Mismatch negativity is not correlated with neuroendocrine indicators of Gosselin N, Mathieu A, Mazza S, Petit D, Malo J, Montplaisir J. Attentional deficits in catecholaminergic activity in healthy subjects. Hum Psychopharm Clin patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: an event-related potential 2003;18:201–5. study. Clin Neurophysiol 2006;117:2228–35. Hari R, Hämäläinen M, Ilmoniemi R, Kaukoranta E, Reinikainen K, Salminen J, et al. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and Responses of the primary auditory cortex to pitch changes in a sequence of tone strategic intentions. Am J Psychiat 2003;160:636–45. pips: neuromagnetic recordings in man. Neurosci Lett 1984;50:127–32. Gottselig JM, Brandeis D, Hofer-Tinguely G, Borbély AA, Achermann P. Human Harkrider AW, Hedrick MS. Acute effect of nicotine on auditory gating in smokers central auditory plasticity associated with tone sequence learning. Learn and non-smokers. Hearing Res 2005;202:114–28. Memory 2004;11:162–71. Hawkins HL, Presson JC. Auditory information processing. In: Boff KR, Kaufman L, Grau C, Escera C, Yago E, Polo MD. Mismatch negativity and auditory sensory Thomas JP. (Eds.) Handbook of perception and human performance, vol 2. New memory evaluation: a new faster paradigm. NeuroReport 1998;9:2451–6. York: Wiley; 1986. p. 261–4. Grau C, Polo MD, Yago E, Gual A, Escera C. Auditory sensory memory as indicated by He W, Chai H, Zheng L, Yu W, Chen W, Li J, et al. Mismatch negativity in treatment- mismatch negativity in chronic alcoholism. Clin Neurophysiol resistant depression and borderline personality disorder. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 2001;112:728–31. 2010;34:366–71. Grimm S, Schröger E, Bendixen A, Bäss P, Roye A, Deouell LY. Optimizing the Heaton P, Pring L, Hermelin B. Musical processing in high functioning children with auditory distraction paradigm: behavioural and event-related potential effects autism. Ann NY Acad Sci 2001;930:443–4. in a laterized multi-deviant approach. Clin Neurophysol 2008;119:934–47. Heekeren K, Daumann J, Neukirch A, Stock C, Kawohl W, Norra C, et al. Mismatch

Groenen P, Snik A, van der Broek P. On the clinical relevance of mismatch negativity generation in the human 5HT2A agonist and NMDA antagonist model negativity: results from subjects with normal hearing and cochlear implant of psychosis. Psychopharmacology 2008;199:77–88. users. Audiol Neuro-Otology 1996;1:112–4. Heim S, Eulitz C, Kaufmann J, Füchter I, Pantev C, Lamprecht-Dinnesen A, et al. Grossheinrich N, Kademan S, Bruder J, Bartling J, von Suchodoletz W. Auditory Atypical organisation of the auditory cortex in dyslexia as revealed by MEG. sensory memory and language abilities in former late talkers: a mismatch Neuropsychologia 2000;38:1749–59. negativity study. Psychophysiology 2010;47:822–30. Heinke W, Kenntner R, Gunter TC, Sammler D, Olthoff D, Koelsch S. Sequential Grzella I, Müller BW, Oades RD, Bender S, Schall U, Zerbin D, et al. Novelty elicited effects of increasing propofol sedation on frontal and temporal cortices as mismatch negativity in patients with schizophrenia on admission and indexed by auditory event-related potentials. Anesthesiology discharge. J Psychiatr Neurosci 2001;26:235–46. 2004;100:617–25. Guérit J-M, Verougstraete D, de Tourtchaninoff M, Debatisse D, Witdoeckt C. ERPs Heinke W, Koelsch S. The effects of anesthetics on brain activity and cognitive obtained with the auditory oddball paradigm in coma and altered states of function. Curr Opin Anaesthesiology 2005;18:625–31. consciousness: clinical relationships, prognostic value, and origin of Hermens DF, Ward PB, Redoblado Hodge MA, Kaur M, Naismith SL, Hickie IB. components. Clin Neurophysiol 1999;110:1260–9. Impaired MMN/P3a complex in first-episode psychosis: cognitive and Gumenyuk V, Korzyukov O, Alho K, Escera C, Näätänen R. Effects of auditory psychosocial associations. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 2010;34:822–9. distraction on electrophysiological brain activity and performance in children Heslenfeld DJ. Visual mismatch negativity. in: Polich J. (Ed.). Detection of change: aged 8–13 years. Psychophysiology 2004;41:30–6. event-related potential and fMRI findings. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers; Gumenyuk V, Korzyukov O, Escera C, Hämäläinen M, Huotilainen M, Häyrinen T, 2003. p. 41–60. et al. Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHD Hirayasu Y, McCarley RW, Salisbury DF, Tanaka S, Kwon JS, Frumin M, et al. Planum children. Neurosci Lett 2005;374:212–7. temporale and Heschl gyrus volume reduction in schizophrenia: a magnetic Gumenyuk V, Roth T, Korzyukov O, Jefferson C, Kick A, Spear L, et al. Shift work sleep resonance imaging study of first-episode patients. Arch Gen Psychiat disorder is associated with an attenuated brain response of sensory memory 2000;57:692–9. and increased brain response to novelty: an ERP study. Sleep 2010;33:703–13. Hirayasu Y, Potts GF, O’Donnell BF, Kwon JS, Arakaki H, Akdag SJ, et al. Auditory Gunter TC, Jackson JL, Mulder G. Focussing on aging: an electrophysiological mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: topographic evaluation with a high- exploration of spatial and attentional processing during reading. Biol Psychol density recording montage. Am J Psychiat 1998;155:1281–4. 1996;43:103–45. Hirayasu Y, Tanaka S, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, DeSantis MA, Levitt JJ, et al. Guttorm TK, Leppänen PHT, Poikkeus A-M, Eklund KM, Lyytinen P, Lyytinen H. Brain Prefrontal gray matter volume reduction in first episode schizophrenia. Cereb event-related potentials (ERPs) measured at birth predict later language Cortex 2001;11:374–81. development in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Cortex Hiruma T, Yabe H, Sato Y, Sutho T, Kaneko S. Differential effects of the hiba odor on 2005;41:291–303. CNV and MMN. Biol Psychol 2002;61:321–31.

Guttorm TK, Leppänen PHT, Richardson U, Lyytinen H. Event-related potentials and Hirvonen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Näätänen R, Sillanaukee P. Adenosine A1/A2a receptors consonant differentiation in newborns with familial risk for dyslexia. J Learn mediate suppression of mismatch negativity by ethanol in humans. Neurosci Disabil 2001;34:534–44. Lett 2000;278:57–60. Halgren E, Baudena P, Clarke JM, Heit G, Liegeois C, Chauvel P, et al. Intracerebral Holguin RS, Corral M, Cadaveira F. Mismatch negativity in young children of potentials to rare target and distracter auditory and visual-stimuli .1. Superior alcoholics from high-density families. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998;22:1363–8. temporal plane and parietal lobe. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Holopainen IE, Korpilahti P, Juottonen K, Lang H Sillanpää M. Attenuated auditory 1995a;94:191–220. event-related potential (mismatch negativity) in children with developmental Halgren E, Baudena P, Clarke JM, Heit G, Marinkovic K, Devaux B, et al. Intracerebral dysphasia. Neuropediatrics 1997;28:253–6. potentials to rare target and distracter auditory and visual-stimuli. 2. Medial, Holopainen IE, Korpilahti P, Juottonen K, Lang H, Sillanpää M. Abnormal frequency lateral and posterior temporal-lobe. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol mismatch negativity in mentally retarded children and in children with 1995b;94:229–50. developmental dysphasia. J Child Neurol 1998;13:178–83. Halgren E, Marinkovic K, Chauvel P. Generators of the late cognitive potentials in Holst M, Eswaran H, Lowery C, Murphy P, Norton J, Preissl H. Development of auditory and visual oddball tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol auditory evoked fields in human fetuses and newborns: a longitudinal MEG 1998;106:156–64. study. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:1949–55. R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 449

Honbolygó F, Csépe V, Fekésházy A, Emri M, Márián T, Sárközy G, et al. Covering Jamieson GA, Dwivedi P, Gruzelier JH. Changes in mismatch negativity across pre- evidences on language impairment in Landau–Kleffner Syndrome revealed by hypnosis, hypnosis and post-hypnosis conditions distinguish high from low behavioural and brain activity measures: a case study. Clin Neurophysiol hypnotic susceptibility groups. Brain Res Bull 2005;67:298–303. 2006;117:295–305. Jankowiak S, Berti S. Behavioral and event-related potential distraction effects with Horimoto R, Inagaki M, Yano T, Sata Y, Kaga M. Mismatch negativity of the color regularly occurring auditory deviants. Psychophysiology 2007;44:79–85. modality during a selective attention task to auditory stimuli in children with Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ceponiene R, Kielinen M, Suominen K, Linna S-L, Moilanen I, mental retardation. Brain Dev JPN 2002;24:703–9. et al. Deficient auditory processing in children with Asperger syndrome, as Horton J, Millar A, Labelle A, Knott VJ. MMN responsivity to manipulations of indexed by event-related potentials. Neurosci Lett 2003a;338:197–200. frequency and duration deviants in chronic, clozapine-treated schizophrenia Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ceponiene R, Valkama M, Vainionpää L, Laitakari K, Alku P, patients. Schizophr Res 2011;126:202–11. et al. Deficient speech-sound processing, as shown by the electrophysiologic Horváth J, Czigler I, Winkler I, Teder-Sälejärvi WA. The temporal window of brain mismatch negativity response, and naming ability in prematurely born integration in elderly and young adults. Neurobiol Aging 2007;28:964–75. children. Neurosci Lett 2003b;348:5–8. Hosák L, Kremlácˇek J, Kuba M, Libiger J, Cˇizˇek J. Mismatch negativity in Jansson-Verkasalo E, Korpilahti P, Jäntti V, Valkama M, Vainionpää L, Alku P, et al. methamphetamine dependence. A pilot study. Acta Neurobiol Exp Neurophysiologic correlates of deficient phonological representations and 2008;68:7–102. object naming in prematurely born children. Clin Neurophysiol Hosoi H, Imaizumi S, Sakaguchi T, Tonoike M, Murata K. Activation of the auditory 2004;115:179–87. cortex by ultrasound. Lancet 1998;351:496–7. Jansson-Verkasalo E, Kujala T, Jussila K, Mattila ML, Moilanen I, Näätänen R, et al. Hugdahl K, Calhoun VD. An update on neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia Similarities in the phenotype of the auditory neural substrate in children with and depression. Front Hum Neurosci 2010;4:4. Asperger syndrome and their parents. Eur J Neurosci 2005;22:986–90. Huotilainen M, Kujala A, Hotakainen M, Parkkonen L, Taulu S, Simola J, et al. Short- Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ruusuvirta T, Huotilainen M, Alku P, Kushnerenko E, Suominen term memory functions of the human fetus recorded with K, et al. Atypical perceptual narrowing in prematurely born infants is associated magnetoencephalography. NeuroReport 2005;16:81–4. with compromised language acquisition at 2 years of age. BMC Neurosci Huotilainen M, Lovio R, Kujala T, Tommiska V, Karma K, Fellman V. Could 2010;11:88. audiovisual training be used to improve cognition in extremely low birth Järvelä LS, Hurme S, Holopainen IE, Leino M, Hatanpää A-M, Mikola H, et al. weight children? Acta Paediatr 2011. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02345.x. Auditory event related potentials as tools to reveal cognitive late effects in Huotilainen M, Shestakova A, Hukki J. Using magnetoencephalography in assessing childhood cancer patients. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;122:62–72. auditory skills in infants and children. Int J Psychophysiol 2008;68:123–9. Javitt DC. Neurophysiological approaches to analyzing brain dysfunction in Huttunen T, Halonen A, Kaartinen J, Lyytinen H. Does mismatch negativity show schizophrenia. Biol Treatment Schizophr 1993;23:144–50. differences in reading-disabled children compared to normal children and Javitt DC. Intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity dysfunction in children with attention deficit? DevNeuropsychol 2007;31:453–70. schizophrenia. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:207–15. Huttunen-Scott T, Kaartinen J, Tolvanen A, Lyytinen H. Mismatch negativity (MMN) Javitt DC. When doors of perception close: bottom-up models of disrupted elicited by duration deviations in children with reading disorder, attention cognition in schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2009a;5:249–75. deficit or both. Int J Psychophysiol 2008;69:69–77. Javitt DC. Glycine transport inhibitors for the treatment of schizophrenia: symptom Iijima, M., Osawa, M., Nageishi, Y., Ushijima, R., Iwata, M. Visual mismatch and disease modification. Curr Opin Drug Discov Dev 2009b;12:468–78. negativity (MMN) in aging. in: Ogura C, Koga Y, Shimokochi M. (Eds.), Recent Javitt DC, Shelley A, Ritter W. Associated deficits in mismatch negativity generation Advances in Event-Related Brain Potentials Research. Amsterdam: Elsevier; and tone matching in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111:1733–7. 1996. p. 804–9. Javitt DC, Schroeder CE, Steinschneider M, Arezzo JC, Vaughan Jr HG. Electroen Clin Ikeda K, Hayashi A, Hashimoto S, Kanno A. Distinctive MMN relative to sound types Neuro 1992;83:87–90. in adults with intellectual disability. NeuroReport 2004;15:1053–6. Javitt DC, Spencer KM, Thaker GK, Winterer G, Hajós M. Neurophysiological Ilvonen T-M, Kujala T, Kiesiläinen A, Salonen O, Kozou H, Pekkonen E, et al. Auditory biomarkers for drug development in schizophrenia. Nature Rev 2008;68:68–83. discrimination after left hemisphere stroke: an MMN follow-up study. Stroke Javitt DC, Liederman E, Cienfuegos A, Shelley A-M. Panmodal processing 2003;34:1746–53. imprecision as a basis for dysfunction of transient memory storage systems in Ilvonen T-M, Kujala T, Kozou H, Kiesiläinen A, Salonen O, Alku P, et al. The schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull 1999;25:763–75. processing of speech and non-speech sounds in aphasic patients as reflected by Javitt DC, Steinschneider M, Schroeder CE, Arezzo JC. Role of cortical N-methyl-D- the mismatch negativity (MMN). Neurosci Lett 2004;366:235–40. aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativity Ilvonen T-M, Kujala T, Tervaniemi M, Salonen O, Näätänen R, Pekkonen E. The generation: implications for schizophrenia. P Natl Acad Sci USA processing of sound duration after left hemisphere stroke: event-related 1996;93:11962–7. potential and behavioral evidence. Psychophysiology 2001;38:622–8. Javitt DC, Grochowski S, Shelley AM, Ritter W. Impaired mismatch negativity Inami R, Kirino E, Inoue R, Arai H. Transdermal nicotine administration enhances (MMN) generation in schizophrenia as a function of stimulus deviance, automatic auditory processing reflected by mismatch negativity. Pharmacol probability, and interstimulus/interdeviant interval. Electroen Clin Neuro Biochem Be 2005;80:453–61. 1998;108:143–53. Inami R, Kirino E, Inoue E, Suzuki T, Arai H. Nicotine effects on mismatch negativity Jessen F, Fries T, Kucharski C, Nishimura T, Hoenig K, Maier W, et al. Amplitude in nonsmoking schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychobiology 2007;56:64–72. reduction of the mismatch negativity in first-degree relatives of patients with Iv J, Zhao L, Gong J, Chen C, Miao D. Event-related potential based evidence of schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett 2001;309:185–8. cognitive dysfunction in patients during the first episode of depression using a Jung J, Morlet D, Mercier B, Confavreux C, Fischer C. Mismatch negativity (MMN) in novelty oddball task. Psychiat Res-Neuroim 2010;182:58–66. multiple sclerosis: an event-related potentials study in 46 patients. Clin Izquidero L. The role of NMDA receptors in memory. Trends Pharmacol Sci Neurophysiol 2006;117:85–93. 1991;12:128–9. Kaga M, Inagaki M, Uno A. Auditory verbal and non-verbal mismatch negativity Jääskeläinen IP, Hirvonen J, Kujala T, Alho K, Eriksson CFP, Lehtokoski A, et al. Effects (MMN) in patients with severe motor and intellectual disabilities. Electroen Clin of naltrexone and ethanol on auditory event-related brain potentials. Alcohol Neuro 1999;49:194–8. 1998;15:105–11. Kähkönen S, Ahveninen J. Combination of magneto- and electroencephalography in Jääskeläinen IP, Lehtokoski A, Alho K, Kujala T, Pekkonen E, Sinclair JD, et al. Low studies of monoamine modulation on attention. Method Find Exp Clin dose of ethanol suppresses mismatch negativity of auditory event-related 2002;24:27–34. potentials. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995;19:607–10. Kähkönen S, Kesäniemi M, Nikouline VV, Karhu J, Ollikainen M, Holi M, et al. Ethanol Jääskeläinen IP, Näätänen R, Sillanaukee P. Effect of acute ethanol on auditory and modulates cortical activity: direct evidence with combined TMS and EEG. visual event-related potentials: a review and reinterpretation. Biol NeuroImage 2001;14:322–8. Psychiat1996a;40:284–91. Kähkönen S, Mäkinen V, Jääskeläinen I, Pennanen S, Liesivuori J, Ahveninen J. Jääskeläinen IP, Pekkonen E, Hirvonen J, Sillanaukee P, Näätänen R. Mismatch Serotonergic modulation of mismatch negativity. Psychiat Res-Neuroim negativity subcomponents and ethyl alcohol. Biol Psychol 1996b;43:13–25. 2005a;138:61–74. Jääskeläinen IP, Schröger E, Näätänen R. Electrophysiological indices of acute effects Kähkönen S, Marttinen-Rossi E, Yamashita H. Alcohol impairs auditory processing of ethanol on involuntary attention shifting. Psychopharmacology of frequency changes and novel sounds: a combined MEG and EEG study. 1999b;141:16–21. Psychopharmacology 2005b;177:366–72. Jääskeläinen IP, Varonen R, Näätänen R, Pekkonen E. Decay of cortical pre-attentive Kähkönen S, Yamashita H, Rytsälä H, Suominen K, Ahveninen J, Isometsä E. sound discrimination in middle-age. NeuroReport 1999a;10:123–6. Dysfunction in early auditory processing in major depressive disorder revealed Jacobs BJ, Schneider SL. Analysis of lexical-semantic processing and extensive by combined MEG and EEG. J Psychiat Neurosci 2007;32:316–22. neurological, electrophysiological, speech perception, and language evaluation Kaipio M-L, Alho K, Winkler I, Escera C, Surma-aho O, Näätänen R. Event-related following a unilateral left hemisphere lesion: pure word deafness? Aphasiology brain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries. 2003;17:123–41. NeuroReport 1999;10:2125–9. Jahshan C, Cadenhead KS, Rissling AJ, Kirihara K, Braff DL, Light GA. Automatic Kaipio M-L, Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Öhman J, Alku P, Näätänen R. Increased sensory information processing abnormalities across the illness course of distractibility in closed head injury as revealed by event-related potentials. schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2011. p. 1–13 [Epub ahead of print]. NeuroReport 2000;11:1463–8. Jahshan C, Heaton RK, Golshan S, Cadenhead KS. Course of neurocognitive deficits in Kaipio M-L, Novitski N, Tervaniemi M, Alho K, Öhman J, Salonen O, et al. Fast the prodrome and first episode of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology vigilance decrement in closed head injury patients as reflected by the mismatch 2010;24:109–20. negativity (MMN). NeuroReport 2001;12:1517–22. James L, Gordon E, Kraiuhin C, Meares R. Selective attention and auditory event- Kallio S, Revonsuo A, Lauerma H, Hämäläinen H, Lang H. The MMN amplitude related potentials in somatisation disorder. Compr Psychiat 1989;30:84–9. increases in hypnosis: a case study. NeuroReport 1999;10:3579–82. 450 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Kalyakin I, González N, Ivannikov A, Lyytinen H. Extraction of the mismatch Kiang M, Braff DL, Sprock J, Light GA. The relationship between preattentive sensory negativity elicited by sound duration decrements: a comparison of three processing deficits and age in schizophrenia patients. Clin Neurophysiol procedures. Data Knowl Eng 2009;68:1411–26. 2009;120:1949–57. Kalyakin I, González N, Joutsensalo J, Huttunen T, Kaartinen J, Lyytinen H. Optimal Kiang M, Light GA, Prugh J, Coulson S, Braff DL, Kutas M. Cognitive, digital filtering versus difference waves on the mismatch negativity in an neurophysiological, and functional correlates of proverb interpretation uninterrupted sound paradigm. Dev Neuropsychol 2007;31:429–52. abnormalities in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsych Soc 2007;13:653–63. Kalyakin I, González N, Kärkkäinen T, Lyytinen H. Independent component analysis Kida T, Nishihira Y, Hatta A, Fumuto M, Wasaka T. Automatic mismatch negativity on the mismatch negativity in an uninterrupted sound paradigm. J Neurosci detection in somatosensory modality and the effect of stimulus probability. Jpn Meth 2008b;174:301–12. Clin Neurophysiol (Tokyo) 2001;29:417–24 [in Japanese]. Kalyakin I, González N, Lyytinen H. Extraction of the mismatch negativity on two Kileny PR, Boerst A, Zwolan T. Cognitive evoked potentials to speech and tonal paradigms using independent component analysis. Proceedings of the CBMS stimuli in children with implants. Otolaryng Head Neck 1997;117:161–9. 2008. In: Puuronen S, Pechenizkiy M, Tsymbal A, Lee DJ. (Eds.), 21st IEEE Kilpeläinen R, Partanen J, Karhu J. Reduced mismatch negativity (MMN) suggests international symposium on computer-based medical systems, CBMS. Los deficits in preattentive auditory processing in distractible children. Alamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society Conference Publishing Services; 2008a. p. NeuroReport 1999;10:3341–5. 59–64. Kimura M, Katayama J, Ohira H, Schröger E. Visual mismatch negativity: new Kane NM, Butler SR, Simpson T. Coma outcome prediction using event-related evidence from the equiprobable paradigm. Psychophysiology 2009;46:402–9. potentials: P3 and mismatch negativity. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:186–91. Kimura M, Schröger E, Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity and its importance in Kane NM, Curry SH, Butler SR, Cummings BH. Electrophysiological indicator of visual cognitive sciences. NeuroReport 2011;22:669–73. awakening from coma. Lancet 1993;341:688. Kircher TT, Rapp A, Grodd W, Buchkremer G, Weiskopf N, Lutzenberger W, et al. Kane NM, Curry SH, Rowlands CA, Manara AR, Lewis T, Moss T, et al. Event-related Mismatch negativity responses in schizophrenia: a combined fMRI and whole- potentials – neurophysiological tools for predicting emergence and early head MEG study. Am J Psychiat 2004;161:294–304. outcome from traumatic coma. Intens Care Med 1996;22:39–46. Kisley MA, Davalos DB, Engleman LL, Guinther PM, Davis HP. Age-related change in Karayanidis F, Andrews S, Ward PB, Michie PT. ERP indexes of auditory selective neural processing of time-dependent stimulus features. Cognit Brain Res attention in aging and Parkinson’s disease. Psychophysiology 1995;32:335–50. 2005;25:913–25. Karma K. Auditory structuring in explaining dyslexia. In: McKevitt P, Nuallain SO, Kisley MA, Davalos DB, Layton HS, Pratt D, Ellis JK, Seger CA. Small changes in Mulvihill C. (Eds.), Advances in consciousness research, language, vision, and temporal deviance modulate mismatch negativity in humans. Neurosci Lett music, vol. 35. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publ. CO; 1999. p. 2004a;358:197–200. 221–9. Kisley MA, Noecker TL, Guinther PM. Comparison of sensory gating to mismatch Kasai K, Hashimoto O, Kawakubo Y, Yumoto M, Kamio S, Itoh K, et al. Delayed negativity and self-reported perceptual phenomena in healthy adults. automatic detection of change in speech sounds in adults with autism: a Psychophysiology 2004b;41:604–12. magnetoencephalographic study. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:1655–64. Kivisaari R, Lehtinen R, Autti T, Puuskari V, Jokela O, Ahveninen J, et al. Impaired Kasai K, Nakagome K, Itoh K, Koshida I, Hata A, Iwanami A, et al. Impaired cortical pre-attentive auditory processing in opioid dependence with and without network for preattentive detection of change in speech sounds in benzodiazepine co-dependence revealed by combined schizophrenia: a high-resolution event-related potential study. Am J magnetoencephalography and electroencephalography. Prog Neuro-Psychoph Psychiatry 2002a;159:546–53. 2007;31:1378–86. Kasai K, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, Hirayasu Y, Lee C-U, Ciszewski AA, et al. Knott V, Blais C, Scherling C, Camarda J, Millar A, Fisher D, et al. Neural effects of Progressive decrease of left superior temporal gyrus gray matter volume in nicotine during auditory selective attention in smokers: an event-related patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat 2003b;160:156–64. potential study. Neuropsychobiology 2006;53:115–26. Kasai K, Yamada H, Kamio S, Nakagome K, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, et al. Do high or Knott VJ, Bolton K, Heenan A, Shah D, Fisher DJ, Villeneuve C. Effects of acute low doses of anxiolytics and hypnotics affect mismatch negativity in nicotine on event-related potential and performance indices of auditory schizophrenic subjects? An EEG and MEG study. Clin Neurophysiol distraction in nonsmokers. Nicotine Tobacco Res 2009;11:519–30. 2002c;113:141–50. Koelsch S, Wittfoth M, Wolf A, Müller J, Hahne A. Music perception in cochlear Kasai K, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, Hirayasu Y, Onitsuka T, Spencer MH, et al. implant users: an event-related potential study. Clin Neurophysiol Progressive decrease of left Heschl gyrus and planum temporale gray matter 2004;115:966–72. volume in first-episode schizophrenia: a longitudinal magnetic resonance Koelsch S, Heinke W, Sammler D, Olthoff D. Auditory processing during deep imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiat 2003a;60:766–75. propofol sedation and recovery from unconsciousness. Clin Neurophysiol Kasai K, Yamada H, Kamio S, Nakagome K, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, et al. Brain 2006;117:1746–59. laterization for mismatch response to across- and within-category change of Kohlmetz C, Altenmüller E, Schuppert M, Wieringa BM, Münte T. Deficit in vowels. NeuroReport 2001;12:2467–71. automatic sound-change detection may underlie some music perception Kasai K, Yamada H, Kamio S, Nakagome K, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, et al. deficits after acute hemispheric stroke. Neuropsychologia 2001;39:1121–4. Neuromagnetic correlates of impaired automatic categorical perception of Kok A. Age-related changes in involuntary and voluntary attention as reflected in speech sounds in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2002b;59:159–72. components of the event-related potential (ERP). Biol Psychol 2000;54:107–43. Katada E, Sato K, Ojika K, Ueda R. Cognitive event-related potentials: useful clinical Korostenskaja M, Dapsys K, Siurkute A, Maciulis V, Ruksenas O, Kähkönen S. Effects information in Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 2004;1:63–9. of olanzapine on auditory P300 and mismatch negativity (MMN) in Kathmann N, Wagner M, Rendtorff N, Engel RR. Delayed peak latency of the schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 2005;29:543–8. mismatch negativity in schizophrenics and alcoholics. Biol Psychiat Korostenskaja M, Nikulin VV, Kicic D, Nikulina AV, Kähkönen S. Effects of NMDA 1995;37:754–7. receptor antagonist memantine on mismatch negativity in healthy subjects. Kaur M, Battisti RA, Ward PB, Ahmed A, Hickie IB, Hermens DF. MMN/P3a deficits in Brain Res Bull 2007;72:275–83. first episode psychosis: comparing schizophrenia-spectrum and affective- Korostenskaja M, Pardos M, Fujiwara H, Kujala TM, Horn P, Rose D, et al. spectrum subgroups. Schizoph Res 2011;130:203–9. Neuromagnetic evidence of impaired cortical auditory processing in pediatric Kawakubo Y, Kamio S, Nose T, Iwanami A, Nakagome K, Fukuda M, et al. Phonetic intractable epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 2010a;92:63–73. mismatch negativity predicts social skills acquisition in schizophrenia. Psychiat Korostenskaja M, Pardos M, Kujala T, Rose DF, Xiang J, Horn P, et al. Impaired auditory Res 2007;152:261–5. information processing during acute migraine: a magnetoencephalography Kawakubo Y, Kasai K, Kudo N, Rogers MA, Nakagome K, Itoh K, et al. Phonetic study. Int J Neurosci 2010b;121:355–65. mismatch negativity predicts verbal memory deficit in schizophrenia. Korpilahti P. Auditory discrimination and memory functions in SLI children: a NeuroReport 2006;17:1043–6. comprehensive study with neurophysiological and behavioural methods. Scand Kazmerski VA, Friedman D, Ritter W. Mismatch negativity during attend and ignore J Logoped Phoniatr 1995;20:131–9. conditions in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiat 1997;42:382–402. Korpilahti P, Lang HA. Auditory ERP components and mismatch negativity in Kekoni J, Hämäläinen H, Saarinen M, Gröhn J, Reinikainen K, Lehtokoski A, et al. Rate dysphasic children. Electroen Clin Neuro 1994;91:256–64. effect and mismatch responses in the somatosensory system: ERP-recordings in Korpilahti P, Jansson-Verkasalo E, Mattila M-L, Kuusikko S, Suominen K, Rytky S, humans. Biol Psychol 1997;46:125–42. et al. Processing of affective speech prosody is impaired in Asperger syndrome. J Kelly AS, Purdy SC, Thorne PR. Electrophysiological and speech perception measures Autism Dev Disord 2007;37:1539–49. of auditory processing in experimental adult cochlear implant users. Clin Kotchoubey B. Event-related potentials predict the outcome of the vegetative state. Neurophysiol 2005;116:1235–46. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:477–9. Kemner C, Verbaten M, Cuperus J, Camfferman G, van Engeland H. Auditory event- Kotchoubey B, Lang S, Herb E, Maurer P, Schmalohr D, Bostanov V, et al. Stimulus related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups. complexity enhances auditory discrimination in patients with extremely severe Biol Psychiat 1995;38:150–65. brain injuries. Neurosci Lett 2003;352:129–32. Kemner C, Verbaten MN, Koelega HS, Buitelaar JK, van der Gaag RJ, Camfferman G, Kotchoubey B, Lang S, Mezger G, Schmalohr D, Schneck M, Semmler A, et al. et al. Event-related potentials in children with attention-deficit and Information processing in severe disorders of consciousness: vegetative state hyperactivity disorder: effects of stimulus deviancy and task relevance in the and minimally conscious state. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;116:2441–53. visual and auditory modality. Biol Psychiat 1996;40:522–34. Kozou H, Kujala T, Shtyrov Y, Toppila E, Stark J, Alku P, et al. The effect of different Kenemans JL, Gren-’t jong T, Verbaten MN. Detection of visual change: mismatch or noise types on the speech and non-speech elicited mismatch negativity. rareness? NeuroReport 2003;14:1239–42. Hearing Res 2005;199:31–9. Kenemans JL, Helby W, van den Heuvel EHM, Grent-’T-Jong T. Moderate alcohol Kramer AF, Trejo LJ, Humphrey D. Assessment of mental workload with task- disrupts a mechanism for detection of rare events in human . J irrelevant auditory probes. Biol Psychol 1995;40:83–100. Psychopharmacol 2010;24:839–45. R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 451

Krauel K, Schott P, Sojka B, Pause BM, Ferstl R. Is there a mismatch negativity Kujala T, Myllyviita K, Tervaniemi M, Alho K, Kallio J, Näätänen R. Basic auditory analogue in the olfactory event-related potential? J Psychophysiol dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstrated by brain-activity measurements. 1999;13:49–55. Psychophysiol Special Rep 2000a;37:262–6. Kraus N, Koch D, McGee T, Nicol T, Cunningham J. Speech-sound discrimination in Kujala T, Palva JM, Salonen O, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Järvinen A, et al. The role of school-age children: psychophysical and neurophysiologic measures. J Speech blind humans’ visual cortex in auditory processing. Neurosci Lett Lang Hear Res 1999;42:1042–60. 2005b;379:127–31. Kraus N, Cheour M. Speech-sound representation in the brain: studies using Kujala T, Shtyrov Y, Winkler I, Saher M, Tervaniemi M, Sallinen M, et al. Detrimental mismatch negativity. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:140–50. effects of long-term noise exposure on the central auditory processing. Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell TD, King C, Tremblay K, Nicol N. Central auditory plasticity Psychophysiology 2004;41:875–81. associated with speech discrimination training. J Cognit Neurosci Kujala T, Alho K, Paavilainen P, Summala H, Näätänen R. Neural plasticity in 1995;7:25–32. processing of sound location by the early blind: an event-related potential Kraus N, McGee TJ, Carrell TD, Zecker SG, Nicol TG, Koch DB. Auditory study. Electroen Clin Neuro 1992;84:469–72. neurophysiologic responses and discrimination deficits in children with Kujala T, Kuuluvainen S, Saalasti S, Jansson-Verkasalo E, von Wendt L, Lepistö T. learning problems. Science 1996;273:971–3. Speech-feature discrimination in children with Asperger syndrome as Kraus N, McGee TJ, Littman T, Nicol TG, King C. Nonprimary auditory thalamic determined with the multi-feature mismatch negativity paradigm. Clin representation of acoustic change. J Neurophysiol 1994;72:1270–7. Neurophysol 2010;121:1410–9. Kraus N, McGee T, Micco A, Sharma A, Carrell T, Nicol T. Mismatch negativity in Kurtzberg D, Vaughan Jr HG, Kreuzer JA, Fliegler KZ. Developmental studies and school-age children to speech stimuli which are just perceptibly different. clinical application of mismatch negativity: problems and prospects. Ear Electroen Clin Neuro 1993a;88:123–30. Hearing 1995;16:105–17. Kraus N, McGee T, Sharma A, Carrell T, Nicol T. Mismatch negativity event-related Kushnerenko E, Ceponiene R, Balan P, Fellman V, Näätänen R. Maturation of the potential elicited by speech stimuli. Ear Hearing 1992;13:158–64. auditory change detection response in infants: a longitudinal ERP study. Kraus N, Micco A, Koch D, McGee T, Carrell T, Wiet R, et al. The mismatch negativity NeuroReport 2002;13:1843–8. cortical evoked potential elicited by speech in cochlear-implant users. Hearing Kushnerenko E, Winkler I, Horváth J, Näätänen R, Pavlov I, Fellman V, et al. Res 1993b;65:118–24. Processing acoustic change and novelty in newborn infants. Eur J Neurosci Kreitschmann-Andermahr I, Rosburg T, Demme U, Gaser E, Nowak H, Sauer H. Effect 2007;26:265–74. of ketamine on the neuromagnetic mismatch field in healthy humans. Cognit Kwon M, Huotilainen M, Shestakova A, Kujala T, Näätänen R, Hämäläinen H. No Brain Res 2001;12:109–16. effects of mobile phone use on cortical auditory change-detection in children: Kreitschmann-Andermahr I, Rosburg T, Meier T, Volz HP, Nowak H, Sauer H. an ERP study. Bioelectromagnetics 2010;31:191–9. Impaired sensory processing in male patients with schizophrenia: a Kwon M, Kujala T, Huotilainen M, Shestakova A, Näätänen R, Hämäläinen H. magnetoencephalographic study of auditory mismatch detection. Schizophr Preattentive auditory information processing under exposure to the 902 MHz Res 1999;35:121–9. GSM mobile phone electromagnetic field: a mismatch negativity (MMN) study. Kremlácˇek J, Hosák L, Kuba M, Libiger J, Cˇizˇek J. Visual information processing in Bioelectromagnetics 2009;30:241–8. recently abstaining methamphetamine-dependent individuals: evoked Lachmann T, Berti S, Kujala T, Schröger E. Diagnostic subgroups of developmental potentials study. Doc Ophthalmol 2008;117:245–55. dyslexia have different deficits in neural processing of tones and phonemes. Int Kropotov JD, Alho K, Näätänen R, Ponomarev VA, Kropotova OV, Anichkov AD, et al. J Psychophysiol 2005;56:105–20. Human auditory-cortex mechanisms of preattentive sound discrimination. Lähteenmäki P. Effects of childhood cancer on patients and their families – physical Neurosci Lett 2000;280:87–90. and physiological aspects. Annales Universitatis Turkuensis, Ser D Tom 338, Kuhl P. Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nature Rev Medica-Odontologia; 1999. 2004;5:831–43. Lalo E, Vercueil L, Bougerol T, Jouk P-S, Debû B. Late event-related potentials and Kuhl P, Coffey-Corina S, Padden D, Dawson G. Links between social and linguistic movement complexity in young adults with Down syndrome. Neurophysiol processing of speech in preschool children with autism: behavioral and Clin 2005;35:81–91. electrophysiological measures. Dev Sci 2005;8:F1–F12. Lang H, Eerola O, Korpilahti P, Holopainen IE, Salo SK, Aaltonen O. Practical issues in Kuhl PK, Williams KA, Lacerda F, Stevens KN, Lindblom B. Linguistic experience the clinical application of mismatch negativity. Ear Hearing 1995;16:118–30. alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science Lang HA, Nyrke T, Ek M, Aaltonen O, Raimo I, Näätänen R. Pitch discrimination 1991;225:606–8. performance and auditory event-related potentials. In. Brunia CHM, Gaillard Kujala A, Huotilainen M, Hotakainen M, Lennes M, Parkkonen L, Fellman V, et al. AWK, Kok A, Mulder G, Verbaten MN. (Eds.), Psychophysiological brain Speech-sound discrimination in neonates as measured with MEG. NeuroReport research, vol. 1. Tilburg, The : Tilburg University Press; 1990. p. 2004;15:2089–92. 294–8. Kujala A, Huotilainen M, Uther M, Shtyrov Y, Monto S, Ilmoniemi RJ, et al. Plastic Laureys S, Perrin F, Schnakers C, Boly M, Majerus S. Residual cognitive function in cortical changes induced by learning to communicate with non-speech sounds. comatose, vegetative and minimally conscious states. Curr Opin Neurol NeuroReport 2003;14:1683–7. 2005;18:726–33. Kujala T. The role of early discrimination deficits in language disorders. J Lavoie S, Murray MM, Deppen P, Knyazeva MG, Berk M, Boulat O, et al. Glutathione Psychophysiol 2007;21:239–50. precursor, N-acetyl-cysteine, improves mismatch negativity in schizophrenia Kujala T, Brattico E. Detrimental noise effects on brain’s speech functions. Biol patients. Neuropsychopharmacol 2007;33:2187–99. Psychol 2009;81:135–43. Leipälä JA, Partanen E, Kushnerenko E, Huotilainen M, Fellman V. Perinatal cerebral Kujala T, Näätänen R. The mismatch negativity in evaluating central auditory insults alter auditory event-related potentials. Early Hum Dev 2011;87:89–95. dysfunctions in dyslexia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2001;25:535–43. Leitman DI, Sehatpour P, Higgins BA, Foxe JJ, Silipo G, Javitt DC. Sensory deficits and Kujala T, Näätänen R. The adaptive brain: a neurophysiological perspective. Progr distributed hierarchical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat Neurobiol 2010;91:55–67. 2010;167:818–27. Kujala T, Aho E, Lepistö T, Jansson-Verkasalo E, Nieminen-von Wendt T, von Wendt Lepistö T, Kajander M, Vanhala R, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. The perception L, et al. Atypical pattern of discriminating sound features in adults with of invariant speech features in children with autism. Biol Psychol Asperger syndrome as reflected by the mismatch negativity. Biol Psychol 2008;77:25–31. 2007;75:109–14. Lepistö T, Kujala T, Vanhala R, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. The Kujala T, Alho K, Huotilainen M, Ilmoniemi RJ, Lehtokoski A, Leinonen A, et al. discrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in children Electrophysiological evidence for cross-modal plasticity in humans with early- with autism. Brain Res 2005;1066:147–57. and late-onset blindness. Psychophysiology 1997;34:213–6. Lepistö T, Nieminen-von Wendt T, von Wendt L, Näätänen R, Kujala T. Auditory Kujala T, Alho K, Kekoni J, Hämäläinen H, Reinikainen K, Salonen O, et al. Auditory cortical change detection in adults with Asperger syndrome. Neurosci Lett and somatosensory event-related brain potentials in early blind humans. Exp 2007;414:136–40. Brain Res 1995a;104:519–26. Lepistö T, Silokallio S, Nieminen-von Wendt P, Alku T, Näätänen R, Kujala T. Kujala T, Alho K, Näätänen R. Cross-modal reorganization of human cortical Auditory perception and attention as reflected by the brain event-related functions. Trends Neurosci 2000b;3:115–20. potentials in children with Asperger syndrome. Clin Neurophysiol Kujala T, Belitz S, Tervaniemi M, Näätänen R. Auditory sensory memory disorder in 2006;117:2161–71. dyslexic adults as indexed by the mismatch negativity. Eur J Neurosci Lepistö T, Soininen M, Ceponiene R, Almqvist F, Näätänen R, Aronen E. Auditory 2003;17:1323–7. event-related potential indices of increased distractibility in children with Kujala T, Huotilainen M, Sinkkonen J, Ahonen AI, Alho K, Hämäläinen MS, et al. major depression. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;115:620–7. Visual cortex activation in blind humans during sound discrimination. Neurosci Leppänen PHT, Guttorm TK, Pihko E, Takkinen S, Eklund KE, Lyytinen H, et al. Lett 1995b;183:143–6. Maturational effects on newborn ERPs measured in the mismatch negativity Kujala T, Karma K, Ceponiene R, Belitz S, Turkkila P, Tervaniemi M, et al. Plastic paradigm. Exp Neurol 2004;190:S91–S101. neural changes and reading improvement caused by audio-visual training in Leppänen PHT, Lyytinen H. Auditory event-related potentials in the study of reading-impaired children. P Natl Acad Sci USA 2001;98:10509–14. developmental language related disorders. Audiol Neuro-Otol 1997;2:308–40. Kujala T, Lepistö T, Nieminen-von Wendt T, Näätänen P, Näätänen R. Leppänen PHT, Eklund KM, Lyytinen H. Event-related brain potentials to change in Neurophysiologial evidence for cortical discrimination impairment of prosody rapidly presented acoustic stimuli in newborns. Dev Neuropsychol in Asperger syndrome. Neurosci Lett 2005a;383:260–5. 1997;13:175–204. Kujala T, Lovio R, Lepistö T, Laasonen M, Näätänen R. Evaluation of multi-attribute Leppänen PHT, Pihko E, Eklund KM, Lyytinen H. Cortical responses of infants with or auditory discrimination in dyslexia with the mismatch negativity. Clin without a genetic risk for dyslexia: II. Group effects. NeuroReport Neurophysiol 2006;117:885–93. 1999;10:969–9673. 452 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Leppänen PHT, Richardson U, Pihko E, Eklund KM, Guttorm TK, Aro M, et al. Brain Magno E, Yeap S, Thakore JH, Garavan H, De Sanctis P, Foxe JJ. Are auditory-evoked responses to changes in speech sound durations differ between infants with and frequency and duration mismatch negativity deficits endophenotypic for without familial risk for dyslexia. Dev Neuropsychol 2002;22:407–22. schizophrenia? High-density electrical mapping in clinically unaffected first- Leppänen PHT, Hämäläinen JA, Salminen HK, Eklund KM, Guttorm TK, Lohvansuu K, degree relatives and first-episode and chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat et al. Newborn brain event-related potentials revealing atypical processing of 2008;64:385–91. sound frequency and the subsequent association with later literacy skills in Magnusson KR, Brim BL, Das SR. Selective vulnerabilities of N-methyl-D-aspartate children with familial dyslexia. Cortex 2010;46:1362–76. (NMDA) receptors during brain aging. Front Aging Neurosci 2010;2:11. Lessard N, Pare M, Lepore F, Lassonde M. Early-blind human subjects localize sound Mäkelä JP, Salmelin R, Hokkanen L, Launes J, Hari R. Neuromagnetic sequelae of sources better than sighted subjects. Nature 1998;395:278–80. herpes simplex encephalitis. Electroen Clin Neuro 1998a;106:251–8.

Leung S, Croft RJ, Baldeweg T, Nathan PJ. Acute dopamine D1 and D2 receptor Mäkelä JP, Salmelin R, Kotila M, Salonen O, Laaksonen R, Hokkanen L, et al. stimulation does not modulate mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy human Modification of neuromagnetic cortical signals by thalamic infarctions. subjects. Psychopharmacology 2007;194:443–51. Electroen Clin Neuro 1998b;106:433–43. Leung S, Croft RJ, O’Neill BV, Nathan PJ. Acute high-dose glycine attenuates Mäntysalo S, Salmi T. Effect of noise on the mismatch negativity of the event-related mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy human controls. Psychopharmacology potential of the human brain. In: Bond NW, Siddle DAT. (Eds.) Psychobiology: 2008;196:451–60. issues and applications. North-Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; 1989. Levänen S, Ahonen A, Hari R, McEvoy L, Sams M. Deviant auditory stimuli activate p. 277–88. human left and right auditory cortex differently. Cerebr Cortex 1996;6:288–96. March L, Cienfuegos A, Goldbloom L, Ritter W, Cowan N, Javitt DC, et al. Normal time Levänen S, Hari R, McEvoy L, Sams M. Responses of the human auditory cortex to course of auditory recognition in schizophrenia, despite impaired precision of changes in one versus two stimulus features. Exp Brain Res 1993;97:177–83. the auditory sensory (‘‘echoic’’) memory code. J Abnorm Psychol Liasis A, Towell A, Alho K, Boyd S. Intracranial identification of an electric frontal- 1999;108:69–75. cortex response to auditory stimulus change: a case study. Cognitive Brain Res Marco-Pallares J, Grau C, Gual A, Escera C, Grau C. Mismatch negativity impairment 2001;11:227–33. associated with alcohol consumption in chronic alcoholics: a scalp current Liasis A, Towell A, Boyd S. Intracranial auditory detection and discrimination potentials density study. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;1:51–7. as substrates of echoic memory in children. Cognit Brain Res 1999;7:503–6. Marco-Pallares J, Grau C, Ruffini G. Combined ICA-LORETA analysis of mismatch Liasis A, Towell A, Boyd S. Intracranial evidence for differential encoding of negativity. NeuroImage 2005;25:471–7. frequency and duration discrimination responses. Ear Hear 2000;21:252–6. Marler JA, Chamolin CA, Gillam RB. Auditory memory for backward masking signals Liasis A, Bamiou DE, Boyd S, Towell A. Evidence for a neurophysiologic auditory in children with language impairment. Psychophysiology 2002;39:767–80. deficit in children with benign epilepsy with centro-temporal spikes. J Neural Martin LF, Davalos DB, Kisley MA. Nicotine enhances automatic temporal Trans 2006;113:939–49. processing as measured by the mismatch negativity waveform. Nicotine Light GA, Braff DL. Mismatch negativity deficits are associated with poor Tobacco Res 2009;11:698–706. functioning in schizophrenia patients. Arch Gen Psychiat 2005a;62:127–36. Martynova O, Kirjavainen J, Cheour M. Mismatch negativity and late discriminative Light GA, Braff DL. Stability of mismatch negativity deficits and their relationship to negativity in sleeping human newborns. Neurosci Lett 2003;340:75–8. functional impairment in chronic schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat Massaro DW. A comparison of forward versus backward recognition masking. J Exp 2005b;162:1741–3. Psychol 1973;100:434–6. Light GA, Swerdlow NR, Braff DL. Preattentive sensory processing as indexed by the Massaro DW. Backward recognition masking. J Acoust Soc Am 1975;58:1059–65. MMN and P3a brain responses is associated with cognitive and psychosocial Matsubayashi J, Kawakubo Y, Suga M, Takei Y, Kumano S, Fukuda M, et al. The functioning in healthy adults. J Cognit Neurosci 2007;19:1624–32. influence of gender and personality traits on individual difference in auditory Lin Y-Y, Hsiao F-J, Shih YH, Yiu C-H, Yen D-J, Kwan S-Y, et al. Plastic phase-locking mismatch: a magnetoencephalographic (MMNm) study. Brain Res and magnetic mismatch response to auditory deviants in temporal lobe 2008;1236:159–65. epilepsy. Cereb Cortex 2007;17:2516–25. Matthews N, Todd J, Budd TW, Cooper G, Michie PT. Auditory lateralization in Lipton SA, Rosenberg PA. Excitatory amino acids as a final common pathway for schizophrenia – mismatch negativity and behavioral evidence of a selective neurologic disorders. New Engl J Med 1994;330:613–22. impairment in encoding interaural time cues. Clin Neurophysiol Liu T, Shi J, Zhang Q, Zhao D, Yang J. Neural mechanism of auditory sensory 2007;118:833–44. processing in children with high intelligence. NeuroReport 2007;18:1571–5. Maurer U, Bucher K, Brem S, Brandeis D. Altered responses to tone and phoneme Lonka E, Lehtokoski A, Kujala T, Johansson R, Rimmanen S, Alho K, et al. The mismatch in kindergartners at familial dyslexia risk. NeuroReport mismatch negativity (MMN) brain response as an index of speech perception 2003;14:2245–50. recovery in cochlear-implant recipients. Audiol Neuro-otol 2004;9:160–2. Maurer U, Bucher K, Brem S, Benz R, Kranz F, Schulz E, et al. Neurophysiology in Lorenzo-Lopez L, Amenedo E, Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F. Pre-attentive detection of preschool improves behavioral prediction of reading ability throughout primary motion direction changes in normal aging. NeuroReport 2004;15:2633–6. school. Biol Psychiat 2009;66:341–8. Lovio R, Näätänen R, Kujala T. Abnormal pattern of cortical speech feature McGeer EG, McGeer PL. Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease and mild discrimination in 6-year-old children at risk for dyslexia. Brain Res cognitive impairment: a field in its infancy. J Alzheimer’s Disease 2010;1335:53–62. 2010;19:355–61. Lovio R, Pakarinen S, Huotilainen M, Alku P, Silvennoinen S, Näätänen R. Auditory Mengler ED, Hogben JH, Michie PT, Bishop DVM. Poor frequency discrimination is discrimination profiles of speech sound changes in 6-year-old children as related to oral language disorder in children: a psychoacoustic study. Dyslexia determined with the multi-feature MMN paradigm. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;11:155–73. 2009;120:916–21. Menning H, Renz A, Seifert J, Maercker A. Reduced mismatch negativity in Luauté J, Fischer C, Adelaine P, Morlet D, Tell L, Boisson D. Late auditory event- posttraumatic stress disorder: a compensatory mechanism for chronic related potentials can be useful to predict good functional outcome after coma. hyperarousal? Int J Psychophysiol 2008;68:27–34. Arch Physic Med Rehab 2005;86:917–23. Menning H, Roberts LE, Pantev C, et al. Plastic changes in the auditory cortex Luck SJ, Mathalon DH, O’Donnell BF, Hämäläinen MS, Spencer KM, Javitt DC, et al. A induced by intensive frequency discrimination training. NeuroReport roadmap for the development and validation of event-related potential 2000;11:817–22. biomarkers in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 2011;70:28–34. Mento G, Suppiej A, Altoè G, Bisiacchi PS. Functional hemispheric asymmetries in Lyytinen H, Ahonen T, Eklund K, Guttorm TK, Laakso M-L, Leinonen S, et al. humans: electrophysiological evidence from preterm infants. Eur J Neurosci Developmental pathways of children with and without familial risk for dyslexia 2010;31:565–74. during the first years of life. Dev Neuropsychol 2001;20:535–54. Mervaala E, Alhainen K, Helkala EL, Partanen J, Jousmäki V, Väyrynen M, et al.

Lyytinen H, Aro M, Eklund K, Erskinel J, Guttorm T, Laakso M-L, et al. The Electrophysiological and neuropsychological effects of a central alpha2- development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: birth to early school age. antagonist atipamezole in healthy volunteers. Behav Brain Res 1993;55:85–91. Ann Dyslexia 2004;54:184–220. Metz-Lutz M-N, Philippini M. Neuropsychological findings in rolandic epilepsy and Lyytinen H, Erskine J, Kujala J, Ojanen E, Richardson U. In search of a science-based Landau–Kleffner syndrome. Epilepsia 2006;47:S71–5. application: a learning tool for reading acquisition. Scand J Psychol Michie PT. What has MMN revealed about the auditory system in schizophrenia? 2009;50:668–75. International J Psychophysiol 2001;42:177–94. Lyytinen H, Erskine J, Tolvanen A, Torppa M. Trajectories of reading development: a Michie PT, Budd TW, Todd J, Rock D, Wichmann H, Box J, et al. Duration and follow-up from birth to school age of children with and without risk for frequency mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol dyslexia. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 2006;52:514–46. 2000;111:1054–65. Maekawa T, Goto Y, Kinukawa N, Taniwaki T, Kanba S, Tobimatsu S. Functional Michie PT, Innes-Brown H, Todd J, Jablensky AV. Duration mismatch negativity in characterization of mismatch negativity to a visual stimulus. Clin Neurophysiol biological relatives of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Biol 2005;116:2392–402. Psychiat 2002;52:749–58. Maekawa T, Tobimatsu S, Inada N, Oribe N, Onitsuka T, Kanba S, et al. Top-down and Mikkola K, Kushnerenko E, Partanen E, Serenius-Sirve S, Leipälä J, Huotilainen M, bottom-up visual information processing of non-social stimuli in high- et al. Auditory event-related potentials and cognitive function of preterm functioning autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord 2011;5:201–9. children at five years of age. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:1494–502. Maekawa T, Tobimatsu S, Ogata K, Onitsuka T, Kanba S. Preattentive visual change Mikkola K, Wetzel N, Leipälä J, Serenius-Sirve S, Schröger E, Huotilainen M, et al. detection as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) – evidence for a Behavioral and evoked potential measures of distraction in 5-year-old children memory-based process. Neurosci Res 2009;65:107–12. born preterm. Int J Psychophysiol 2010;77:8–12. Mager R, Falkenstein M, Störmer R, Brand S, Müller-Spahn F, Bullinger AH. Auditory Minami Y, Kirino E. Schizophrenic patients are impaired in memory reinstatement distraction in young and middle-aged adults: a behavioural and event-related underlying mismatch negativity system. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:120–8. potential study. J Neural Trans 2005;112:1165–76. R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 453

Missioner P, Ragot R, Derouesné C, Guez D, Renault B. Automatic attentional shifts Nager W, Münte TF, Bohrer I, Lenarz T, Dengler R, Möbes J, et al. Automatic and induced by a noradrenergic drug in Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from evoked attentive processing of sounds in cochlear implant patients – potentials. Int J Psychophysiol 1999;33:243–51. electrophysiological evidence. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2007;25:391–6. Miyajima M, Ohta K, Hara K, Iino H, Maehara T, Hara M, et al. Abnormal mismatch Nakagawa K, Shoji H, Katada A, Ozaki H. How regularities in stimulus series and in negativity for pure-tone sounds in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res ISI affect on MMN in persons with intellectual disabilities. Int Congr Ser 2011;94:149–57. 2002;1232:363–6. Moberget T, Karns CM, Deouell LY, Lindgren M, Knight RT, Ivry RB. Detecting Nakagome K, Ichikawa I, Kanno O, Akaho R, Suzuki M, Takazawa S, et al. Overnight violations of sensory expectations following cerebellar degeneration: a effects of triazolam on cognitive function: an event-related potentials study. mismatch negativity study. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:2569–79. Neuropsychobiology 1998;38:232–40. Molholm S, Gomes H, Lobosco J, Deacon D, Ritter W. Feature versus gestalt Nashida T, Yabe H, Sato T, Sutoh T, Shinosaki N, Kaneko S. Automatic auditory representation of stimuli in the mismatch negativity system of 7- to 9-year-old processing in sleep. Sleep 2000;23:1–8. children. Psychophysiology 2004;41:385–93. Naumann A, Bierbrauer J, Przuntek H, Daum I. Attentive and preattentive processing Molholm S, Martinez A, Ritter W, Javitt DC, Foxe JJ. The neural circuitry of pre- in narcolepsy as revealed by event-related potentials (ERPs). NeuroReport attentive auditory change-detection: an fMRI study of pitch and duration 2001;12:2807–11. mismatch negativity generators. Cerebr Cortex 2005;15:545–51. Neumann N, Kotchoubey B. Assessment of cognitive functions in severely paralysed Moreau P, Jolicoeur P, Peretz I. Automatic brain responses to pitch changes in and severely brain-damaged patients: neuropsychological and congenital amusia. Ann NY Acad Sci 2009;1169:191–4. electrophysiological methods. Brain Res Protoc 2004;14:25–36. Morgan III CA, Grillon C. Abnormal mismatch negativity in women with sexual Newcomer JW, Farber NB, Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Selke G, Melson AK, Hersey T, et al. assault-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiat 1999;45:827–32. Ketamine-induced NMDA-receptor hypofunction as a model of memory Morlet D, Bouchet P, Fischer C. Mismatch negativity and N100 monitoring: potential impairment and psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacol 1998;20:106–18. clinical value and methodological advances. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:198–206. Nittono H, Momose D, Hori T. The vanishing point of the mismatch negativity at Murakami T, Nakagome K, Kamio S, Kasai K, Iwanami A, Hiramatsu K-I, et al. The sleep onset. Clin Neurophysiol 2001;112:732–9. effects of benzodiazepines on event-related potential indices of automatic and Niznikiewicz MA, Spencer KM, Dickey C, Volgmaier M, Seidman LJ, Shenton ME, controlled processing in schizophrenia. A preliminary report. Prog Neuro- et al. Abnormal pitch mismatch negativity in individuals with schizotypal Psychoph 2002;26:651–61. personality disorder. Schizophr Res 2009;110:188–93. Müller BW, Jüptner M, Jentzen W, Müller SP. Cortical activation to auditory Nordby H, Brønnick KS, Hugdahl K. Processing of deviant visual events reflected by mismatch elicited by frequency deviant and complex novel sounds: a PET study. event-related brain potentials. In: Ogura C, Koga Y, Shimokochi M. Recent NeuroImage 2002;17:231–9. advances in event-related brain potential research. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1996. Näätänen R. The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by p. 99–104. event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function. Behav Novitski N, Huotilainen M, Tervaniemi M, Näätänen R, Fellman V. Neonatal Brain Sci 1990;13:201–88. frequency discrimination in 250–4000 Hz frequency range: Näätänen R. Attention and brain function. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1992. electrophysiological evidence. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:417–9. Näätänen R. The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the Oades RD, Dittman-Balcar A, Schepker R, Eggers C, Zerbin D. Auditory event-related mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent MMNm. potentials (ERPs) and mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy children and those Psychophysiology (Presidential Address, 1999) 2001;38:1–21. with attention-deficit or Tourette/tic symptoms. Biol Psychol 1996;43:163–85. Näätänen R. Somatosensory mismatch negativity: a new clinical tool for Oades RD, Zerbin DA, Dittmann-Balcar A, Eggers C. Auditory event-related potential developmental neurological research? Dev Med Child Neurol 2009;51:930–1. (ERP) and difference-wave topography in schizophrenic patients with/without Näätänen R, Gaillard AWK. The orienting reflex and the N2 deflection of the event- active hallucinations and delusions: a comparison with young obsessive- related potential (ERP). In: Gaillard AWK, Ritter W. (Eds.), Tutorials in event compulsive disorder (OCD) and healthy subjects. Int J Psychophysiol related potential research: endogenous components. North Holland: 1996;22:185–214. Amsterdam; 1983. p. 119–41. Oades RD, Wild-Wall N, Juran SA, Sachsse J, Oknina LB, Röpcke B. Auditory change Näätänen R, Kähkönen S. Central auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia as revealed detection in schizophrenia: sources of activity, related neuropsychological by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent MMNm – a function and symptoms in patients with a first episode in adolescence, and review. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009;12:125–35. patients 14 years after an adolescent illness-onset. BMC Psychiatry 2006;6:7. Näätänen R, Kreegipuu K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) as an index of different Oates PA, Kurtzberg D, Stapells DR. Effects of sensorineural hearing loss on cortical forms of memory in audition. In: Bäckman L, Nyberg L, editors. Memory, aging, event-related potential and behavioural measures of speech-sound processing. and brain. a festschrift in hounour of Lars-Göarn Nilsson. Psychology Press; Ear Hearing 2002;23:399–415. 2011. p. 287–99. Ogura C, Nageishi Y, Fukao K, Shimoji Y, Hirano K, Hokama H, et al. Deviate Näätänen R, Kujala T. The mismatch negativity and its magnetic equivalent: an component of event-related potentials in Shuchaku–Seikaku, a premorbid index of language impairment or more general cognitive decline in autism? Biol personality of depression. Jpn J Psychiat Neurol 1991;45:641–51.

Psychiat 2011;70:212–3. Ogura C, Nageishi Y, Omura F, Fukao K, Ohta H, Kishimoto A, et al. N200 component Näätänen R, Michie PT. Early selective attention effects on the evoked potential. A of event-related potentials in depression. Biol Psychiat 1993;33:720–6. critical review and reinterpretation. Biol Psychol 1979;8:81–136. Oknina LB, Wild-Wall N, Oades RD, Juran SA, Röpcke B, Pfueller U, et al. Frontal and Näätänen R, Summala H. Road-user behavior and traffic accidents. North-Holland, temporal sources of mismatch negativity in healthy controls, patients at onset New York: Amsterdam and American Elsevier; 1976. 270 p. of schizophrenia in adolescence and others at 15 years after onset. Scizophr Res Näätänen R, Winkler I. The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive 2005;76:25–41. neuroscience. Psychol Bull 1999;6:826–59. Olney JW, Farber NB. Glutamate receptor dysfunction and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Näätänen R, Gaillard AWK, Mäntysalo S. Early selective-attention effect on evoked Psychiat 1995;52:998–1007. potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol 1978;42:313–29. Opitz B, Rinne T, Mecklinger A, von Cramon DY, Schröger E. Differential contribution Näätänen R, Kujala T, Kreegipuu K, Carlson S, Escera C, Baldeweg T, et al. The of frontal and temporal cortices to auditory change detection: fMRI and ERP mismatch negativity: an index of cognitive decline in neuropsychiatric and results. NeuroImage 2002;15:165–74. neurological diseases and in aging. Brain 2011. doi:10.1093/brain/awr064. Oram Cardy JE, Flagg EJ, Roberts W, Brian J, Roberts TPL. Magnetoencephalography Näätänen R, Kujala T, Winkler I. Auditory processing that leads to conscious identifies rapid temporal processing deficit in autism and language impairment. perception: a unique window to central auditory processing opened by the NeuroReport 2005a;16:329–32. mismatch negativity and related responses. Psychophysiology 2011b;48:4–22. Oram Cardy JE, Flagg EJ, Roberts W, Roberts TPL. Delayed mismatch field for speech Näätänen R, Lehtokoski A, Lennes M, Cheour M, Huotilainen M, Iivonen A, et al. and non-speech sounds in children with autism. NeuroReport 2005b;16:521–5. Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic Oranje B, Jensen K, Wienberg M, Glenthøj BY. Divergent effects of increased brain responses. Nature 1997;385:432–4. serotonergic activity on psychophysiological parameters of human attention. Näätänen R, Tervaniemi M, Sussman E, Paavilainen P, Winkler I. Primitive Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;11:453–63. intelligence in the auditory cortex. Trends Neurosci 2001;24:283–8. Osawa M, Iijima M, Nageishi Y, Ushijima R, Iwata M. Automatic and controlled Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Rinne T, Alho K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic processing in normal aging and dementia. In: Ogura C, Koga Y, Shimokochi M, research of central auditory processing: a review. Clin Neurophysiol editors. Recent advances in event-related brain potential research 1996;Vol. 2007;118:2544–90. 1099. p. 743–50. Näätänen R, Schröger E, Alho K. Electrophysiology of Attention. In Wixted J. (Ed.), Paavilainen P, Alho K, Reinikainen K, Sams M, Näätänen R. Right-hemisphere Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology, 3rd ed. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; dominance of different mismatch negativities. Electroencephalogr Clin 2002. p. 601–53. Neurophysio 1991;78:466–79. Näätänen R, Schröger E, Karakas S, Tervaniemi M, Paavilainen P. Development of a Pakarinen S, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. The mismatch negativity (MMN) with no memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. NeuroReport standard stimulus. Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:1043–50. 1993;4:503–6. Pakarinen S, Lovio R, Huotilainen M, Alku P, Näätänen R, Kujala T. Fast multi-feature Näätänen R, Pakarinen S, Rinne T, Takegata R. The mismatch negativity (MMN) – paradigm for recording several mismatch negativities (MMNs) to phonetic and towards the optimal paradigm. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;115:140–4. acoustic changes in speech sounds. Biol Psychol 2009;82:219–26. Näätänen R, Simpson M, Loveless NE. Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials. Biol Pakarinen S, Takegata R, Rinne T, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. Measurement of Psychol 1982;14:53–98. existensive auditory discrimination profiles using the mismatch negativity Naccache L, Puybasset L, Gaillard R, Serve E, Willer J. Auditory mismatch negativity (MMN) of the auditory event-related potential (ERP). Clin Neurophysiol is a good predictor of awakening in comatose patients: a fast and reliable 2007;118:177–85. procedure. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:988–9. 454 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Pang EW, Fowler B. Dissociation of the mismatch negativity and processing Pihko E, Kujala T, Mickos A, Alku P, Byring R, Korkman M. Language impairment is negativity attentional waveforms with nitrous oxide. Psychophysiology reflected in auditory evoked fields. Int J Psychophysiol 2008;68:161–9. 1999;36:552–8. Pihko E, Mickos A, Kujala T, Pihlgren A, Westman M, Alku P, et al. Group Pang EW, Edmonds GE, Desjardins R, Khan SC, Trainor LJ, Taylor MJ. Mismatch intervention changes brain activity in bilingual language-impaired children. negativity to speech stimuli in 8-month-old infants and adults. Int J Cereb Cortex 2007;17:849–58. Psychophysiol 1998;29:227–36. Pincze Z, Lakatos P, Rajkai C, Ulbert I, Karmos G. Separation of mismatch negativity Park H-J, Kwon JS, Youn T, Pae JS, Kim J-J, Kim M-S, et al. Statistical parametric and the N1 wave in the auditory cortex of the cat: a topographic study. Clin mapping of LORETA using high density EEG and individual MRI: application to Neurophysiol 2001;112:778–84. mismatch negativities in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp 2002;17:168–78. Pincze Z, Lakatos P, Rajkai C, Ulbert I, Karmos G. Effect of deviant probability and Paul I, Bott C, Heim S, Eulitz C, Elbert T. Reduced hemispheric asymmetry of the interstimulus/interdeviant interval on the auditory N1 and mismatch negativity auditory N260m in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 2006a;44:785–94. in the cat auditory cortex. Brain Res Cognitive Brain Res 2002;13:249–53. Paul I, Bott C, Heim S, Wienbruch C, Elbert T. Phonological but not auditory Polo MD, Escera C, Gual A, Grau C. Mismatch negativity and auditory sensory discrimination is impaired in dyslexia. Eur J Neurosci 2006b;24:2945–53. memory in chronic alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1999;23:1744–50. Pause BM, Krauel K. Chemosensory event-related potentials (CSERP) as a key to the Polo MD, Escera C, Yago E, Alho K, Gual A, Grau C. Electrophysiological evidence of psychology of odors. Int J Psychophysiol 2000;36:105–22. abnormal activation of the cerebral network of involuntary attention in Pazo-Alvarez P, Amenedo E, Cadaveira F. Automatic detection of motion direction alcoholism. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:134–46. changes in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 2004;19:1978–86. Polo MD, Newton P, Rogers D, Escera C, Butler S. ERPs and behavioural indices of Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F, Amenedo E. MMN in the visual modality: a review. Biol long-term preattentive and attentive deficits after closed head injury. Psychol 2003;63:199–236. Neuropsychologia 2002;40:2350–9. Peach RK, Newhoff M, Rubin SS. Attention in aphasia as revealed by event-related Ponton CW, Don M. The mismatch negativity in cochlear implant users. Ear Hearing potentials: a preliminary investigation. In: Lemmer ML. (Ed.) Clinical 1995;16:131–46. aphasiology, vol. 21. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1992. p. 323–33. Ponton CW, Don M. Cortically-evoked activity recorded from cochlear implant Peach RK, Newhoff M, Rubin SS. Attention in aphasia as revealed by event-related users. Methods and Applications. In: Cullington H. (Ed.), Cochlear implants potentials: a preliminary investigation. Clin Aphasiol 1993;21:323–33. objective measures. London: Whurr Publishers Ltd; 2003. p. 187–230. Peach RK, Rubin SS, Newhoff M. A topographic event-related potential analysis of Ponton CW, Eggermont JJ. Of kittens and kids: altered cortical maturation following the attention deficit for auditory processing in Aphasia. Clin Aphasiol profound deafness and cochlear implant use. Audiol Neuro-Otol 1994;22:81–96. 2001;6:363–3680. Pekkonen E. Mismatch negativity in aging and in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s Ponton CW, Eggermont JJ. Electrophysiological measures of human auditory system diseases. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:216–24. maturation: Relationship with neuroanatomy and behavior. in: Burkhard RF, Pekkonen E, Ahveninen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Seppä K, Näätänen R, Sillanaukee P. Don M, Eggermont JJ. (Eds.), Auditory evoked potentials: basic principles and Selective acceleration of auditory processing in chronic alcoholics. Alcohol Clin clinical application. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2007. p. 358– Exp Res 1998;22:605–9. 402. Pekkonen E, Hirvonen J, Ahveninen J, Kähkönen S, Kaakkola S, Huttunen J, et al. Ponton CW, Bernstein LE, Auer ET. Mismatch negativity with visual-only and Memory-based comparison process not attenuated by haloperiodol: a audiovisual speech. Brain Topogr 2009;21:207–15. combined MEG and EEG study. NeuroReport 2002;13:177–81. Ponton CW, Don M, Eggermont JJ, Kwong B. The integrated mismatch negativity Pekkonen E, Hirvonen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Kaakkola S, Huttunen J. Auditory sensory (MMNi): a noise-free representation of evoked responses allowing single-point memory and the cholinergic system: implication for Alzheimer’s disease. distribution-free statistical tests. Electroen Clin Neuro 1997;104:143–50. NeuroImage 2001a;14:376–82. Ponton CW, Eggermont JJ, Don M, Waring MD, Kwong B, Cunningham J, et al. Pekkonen E, Huotilainen M, Virtanen J, Näätänen R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Erkinjuntti T. Maturation of the mismatch negativity: effects of profound deafness and Alzheimer’s disease affects parallel processing between the auditory cortices. cochlear implant use. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:167–85. NeuroReport 1996b;7:1363–8. Potts GF, Hirayasu Y, O’Donnell BF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. High-density Pekkonen E, Huotilainen M, Virtanen J, Sinkkonen J, Rinne T, Ilmoniemi R, et al. Age- recording and topographic analysis of the auditory oddball event-related related functional differences between auditory cortices: a whole-head MEG- potential in patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 1998;44:982–9. study. NeuroReport 1995a;6:1803–6. Price GW, Michie PT, Johnston J, Innes-Brown H, Kent A, Clissa P, et al. A Pekkonen E, Jääskeläinen IP, Erkinjuntti T, Hietanen M, Huotilainen M, Ilmoniemi RJ, multivariate electrophysiological endophenotype, from a unitary cohort, shows et al. Preserved stimulus deviance detection in Alzheimer’s disease. greater research utility than any single feature in the Western Australian family NeuroReport 2001b;12:1649–52. study of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 2006;60:1–10. Pekkonen E, Jääskeläinen IP, Hietanen M, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Pritchard W, Sokhadze E, Houliman M. Effects of nicotine and smoking on event- et al. Impaired preconscious auditory processing and cognitive function in related potentials: a review. Nicotine Tobacco Res 2004;6:961–84. Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol 1999;110:1942–7. Pulvermuller F, Shtyrov Y, Kujala T, Naatanen R. Word-specific cortical activity as Pekkonen E, Jääskeläinen IP, Kaakkola S, Ahveninen J. Cholinergic modulation of revealed by the mismatch negativity. Psychophysiol 2004;41:106–12. preattentive auditory processing in aging. NeuroImage 2005;27:387–92. Qiu X, Yang X, Qiao Z, Wang L, Ning N, Shi J, et al. Impairment in processing visual Pekkonen E, Jousmäki V, Könönen M, Reinikainen K, Partanen J. Auditory sensory information at the pre-attentive stage in patients with a major depressive memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: an event-related potential study. disorder: a visual mismatch negativity study. Neurosci Lett 2011;491:53–7. NeuroReport 1994;5:2537–40. Rabinowicz EF, Silipo GMA, Goldman R, Javitt DC. Auditory sensory dysfunction in Pekkonen E, Jousmäki V, Reinikainen K, Partanen J. Automatic auditory schizophrenia: imprecision or distractibility. Arch Gen Psychiat discrimination is impaired in Parkinson’s disease. Electroen Clin Neuro 2000;57:1149–55. 1995c;95:47–52. Ragazzoni A, Grippo A, Tozzi F, Zaccara G. Event-related potentials in patients with Pekkonen E, Osipova D, Laaksovirta H. Magnetoencephalographic evidence of total locked-in state due to fulminant Guillain–Barré syndrome. Int J abnormal auditory processing in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with bulbar Psychophysiol 2000;37:99–109. signs. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;115:309–15. Raggi A, Consonni M, Iannaccone S, Perani D, Zamboni M, Sferrazza B, et al. Auditory Pekkonen E, Rinne T, Näätänen R. Variability and replicability of the mismatch event-related potentials in non-demented patients with sporadic amyotrophic negativity. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol 1995b;96:546–54. lateral sclerosis. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:342–50. Pekkonen E, Rinne T, Reinikainen K, Kujala T, Alho K, Näätänen R. Aging effects on Rahne T, von Specht H, Mühler R. Sorted averaging-application to auditory event- auditory processing: an event-related potential study. Exp Aging Res related responses. J Neurosci Meth 2008;172:74–8. 1996a;22:171–84. Rasser PE, Schall U, Todd J, Michie PT, Ward PB, Johnston P, et al. Gray matter Peretz I, Brattico E, Järvenpää M, Tervaniemi M. The amusic brain: in tune, out of deficits, mismatch negativity, and outcomes in Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia key, and unaware. Brain 2009;132:1277–86. Bull 2011;37:131–40. Pettigrew CM, Murdoch BE, Chenery HJ, Kei J. The relationship between the Raz A, Deouell LY, Bentin S. Is pre-attentive processing compromised by prolonged mismatch negativity (MMN) and psycholinguistic models of spoken word wakefulness? Effects of total sleep deprivation on the mismatch negativity. processing. Aphasiology 2004a;18:3–28. Psychophysiology 2001;38:787–95. Pettigrew CM, Murdoch BE, Ponton CW, Finnigan S, Alku P, Sockalingam R, et al. Realmuto G, Begleiter H, Odencrantz J, Porjesz B. Event-related potential evidence of Automatic auditory processing of English words as indexed by the mismatch dysfunction in automatic processing in abstinent alcoholics. Bioll Psychiat negativity, using a multiple deviant paradigm. Ear Hearing 2004b;25:284–301. 1993;33:594–601. Pettigrew CM, Murdoch BE, Kei J, Ponton CW, Alku P, Chenery HJ. The mismatch Reinvang I, Nordby H, Nielsen CS. Information processing deficits in head injury negativity (MMN) response to complex tones and spoken words in individuals assessed with ERPs reflecting early and late processing stages. with aphasia. Aphasiology 2005;19:131–63. Neuropsychologia 2000;38:995–1005. Pietrowsky R, Fehm HL, Born J. An analysis of the effects of corticotropin-releasing Reite M, Teale P, Rojas DC, Asherin R, Hernandez O. MEG auditory evoked fields hormone (h-CRH), adenocorticotropin (ACTH) and b-endorphin on ERPs in suggest altered structural/functional asymmetry in primary but not secondary humans. In: Brunia CHM, Gaillard AWK, Kok A. (Eds.), Psychol Brain Res auditory cortex in bipolar disorder. Bipol Disord 2009;11:371–81. 1990;2:106–10 [Tilbury University Press]. Renvall H, Hari R. Diminished auditory mismatch fields in dyslexic adults. Ann Pihko E, Kujala T, Mickos A, Antell H, Alku P, Byring R, et al. Magnetic fields evoked Neurol 2003;53:551–7. by speech sounds in pre-school children. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:112–9. Restuccia D, Della Marca C, Valeriani M, Leggio MG, Molinari M. Cerebellar damage Pihko E, Leppänen PHT, Eklund KM, Cheour M, Guttorm TK, Lyytinen H. Cortical impairs detection of somatosensory input changes. A somatosensory mismatch- responses of infants with and without a genetic risk for dyslexia: I. Age effects. negativity study. Brain 2007;130:276–87. NeuroReport 1999;10:901–5. R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 455

Restuccia D, Zanini S, Cazzagon M, Del Piero I, Martucci L, Della Marca G. Sallinen M, Kaartinen J, Lyytinen H. Is the appearance of mismatch negativity during Somatosensory mismatch negativity in healthy children. Dev Med Child stage 2 sleep related to the elicitation of K-complex. Electroen Clin Neuro Neurol 2009;51:991–8. 1994;91:140–8. Richardson U, Leppänen PHT, Leiwo M, Lyytinen H. Speech perception of infants Sallinen M, Kaartinen J, Lyytinen H. Processing of auditory stimuli during tonic and with high familial risk for dyslexia differs at the age of 6 months. Dev phasic periods of REM sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials. J Neuropsychol 2003;23:387–97. Sleep Res 1996;5:220–8. Riekkinen Jr P, Pääkkönen A, Karhu J, Partanen J, Soininen H, Laakso M, et al. THA Sallinen M, Lyytinen H. Mismatch negativity during objective and subjective disrupts mismatch negativity in Alzheimer disease. Psychopharmacology sleepiness. Psychophysiology 1997;34:694–702. 1997;133:203–6. Salmi J, Huotilainen M, Pakarinen S, Siren T, Alho K, Aronen ET. Does sleep qualify Rinker T, Kohls G, Richter C, Maas V, Schulz E, Schecker M. Abnormal frequency affect involuntary attention switching system? Neurosci Lett 2005;390:150–5. discrimination in children with SLI as indexed by mismatch negativity (MMN). Salo S, Peltola MS, Aaltonen O, Johansson R, Lang AH, Laurikainen E. Stability of Neurosci Lett 2007;413:99–104. memory traces for speech sounds in cochlear implant patients. Logoped Rinne T, Alho K, Ilmoniemi RJ, Virtanen J, Näätänen R. Separate time behaviors of the Phoniatr Vocol 2002;27:132–8. temporal and frontal mismatch negativity sources. NeuroImage 2000;12:14–9. Sams M, Hämäläinen M, Antervo A, Kaukoranta E, Reinikainen K, Hari R. Cerebral Rinne T, Degerman A, Alho K. Superior temporal and inferior frontal cortices are neuromagnetic responses evoked by short auditory stimuli. Electroen Clin activated by infrequent sound duration decrements: an fMRI study. Neuro 1985b;61:254–66. NeuroImage 2005;26:66–72. Sams M, Paavilainen P, Alho K, Näätänen R. Auditory frequency discrimination and Rinne T, Gratton G, Fabiani M, Cowan N, Maclin E, Stinard A, et al. Scalp-recorded event-related potentials. Electroen Clin Neuro 1985a;62:437–48. optical signals make sound processing in the auditory cortex visible. Sams M, Hari R, Rif J, Knuuttila J. The human auditory sensory memory trace NeuroImage 1999;10:620–4. persists about 10 s: neuromagnetic evidence. J Cognitive Neurosci Rinne T, Särkkä A, Degerman A, Schröger E, Alho K. Two separate mechanisms 1993;5:363–70. underlie auditory change detection and involuntary control of attention. Brain Sandmann P, Kegel A, Eichele T, Dillier N, Lai W, Bendixen A, et al. Res 2006;1077:135–43. Neurophysiological evidence of impaired musical sound perception in Rissling AJ, Makeig S, Braff DL, Light GA. Neurophysiologic markers of abnormal cochlear-implant users. Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:2070–82. brain activity in schizophrenia. Curr Psychiat Reports 2010;12:572–8. Santos MAR, Munhoz MSL, Peixoto MAL, Haase VG, Rodrigues JL, Resende LM. Roberts TPL, Cannon KM, Tavabi K, Blaskey L, Khan SY, Monroe JF, et al. Auditory Mismatch negativity contribution in multiple sclerosis patients. Revi Bras magnetic mismatch field latency: a biomarker for language impairment in Otorrinolaringol 2006;72:800–7. autism. Biol Psychiat 2011;70:263–9. Särkämö T, Pihko E, Laitinen S, Forsblom A, Soinila S, Mikkonen M, et al. Music and Roberts TPL, Schmidt GL, Egeth M, Blaskey L, Rey MM, Edgar C, et al. speech listening enhance the recovery of early sensory processing after stroke. J Electrophysiological signature: magnetoencephalographic studies of the Cognitive Neurosci 2010a;22:2716–27. neural correlates of language impairment in autism spectrum disorders. Int J Särkämö T, Tervaniemi M, Laitinen S, Forsblom A, Soinila S, Mikkonen M, et al. Psychophysiol 2008;68:149–60. Music listening enhances cognitive recovery and mood after middle cerebral Röder B, Teder-Sälejärvi W, Sterr A, Rösler F, Hillyard SA, Neville HJ. Improved artery stroke. Brain 2008;131:866–76. auditory spatial tuning in blind humans. Nature 1999;400:162–6. Särkämö T, Tervaniemi M, Soinila S, Autti T, Silvennoinen HM, Laine M, et al. Rodriguez HS, Corral M, Cadaveira F. Mismatch negativity in young children of Cognitive deficits associated with acquired amusia after stroke: a alcoholics from high-density families. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998;22:1363–8. neuropsychological follow-up study. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:2642–51. Roeske D, Ludwig KU, Neuhoff N, Becker J, Bartling J, Bruder J, et al. First-genome- Särkämö T, Tervaniemi M, Soinila S, Autti T, Silvennoinen HM, Laine M, et al. wide association scan on neurophysiological endophenotypes points to trans- Auditory and cognitive deficits associated with acquired amusia in stroke: a regulation effects on SLC2A3 in dyslexic children. Mol Psychiatr magnetoencephalography and neuropsychological follow-up study. PLoS ONE 2011;16:97–107. 2010b;5:e15157. Roger C, Hasbroucq T, Rabat A, Vidal F. Neurophysics of temporal discrimination in Sasaki T, Campbell KB, Bazana PG, Stelmack RM. Individual differences in mismatch the rat: a mismatch negativity study. Psychophysiol 2009;46:1028–32. negativity measures of involuntary attention shift. Clin Neurophysiolo Roman S, Canévet G, Marquis P, Triglia J-M, Liégeois-Chauvel C. Relationship 2000;111:1553–60. between auditory perception skills and mismatch negativity recorded in free Sato Y, Yabe H, Todd J, Michie P, Shinozaki N, Sutoh T, et al. Impairment in activation field in cochlear-implant users. Hearing Res 2005;201:10–20. of a frontal attention-switch mechanism in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychol Rosburg T. Left hemispheric dipole locations of the neuromagnetic mismatch 2002;62:49–63. negativity to frequency, intensity and duration deviants. Cogn Brain Res Sato Y, Yabe H, Hiruma T, Sutoh T, Shinozaki N, Nashida T, et al. Do changes in 2003;16:83–90. probability on the MMN have a different effect in schizophrenic patients. In: Rosburg T, Marinou V, Haueisen J, Smesny S, Sauer H. Effects of lorazepam on the Hashimoto I, Kakigi R. (Eds.), Recent advances in human neurophysiology. neuromagnetic mismatch negativity (MMNm) and auditory evoked field Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1998. p. 803–8. component N100m. Neuropsychopharmacol 2004;29:1723–33. Satterfield JH, Schell AM, Nicholas T, Backs RW. Topographic study of auditory Rosburg T, Trautner P, Dietl T, Korzyukov OA, Boutros NN, Schaller C, et al. Subdural event-related potentials in normal boys and boys with attention deficit disorder recordings of the mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with focal epilepsy. with hyperactivity. Psychophysiology 1988;25:591–606. Brain 2005;128:819–28. Sauer H, Volz HP. Functional magnetic resonance imaging and magnetic resonance Rosburg T, Trautner P, Ludowig E, Schaller C, Kurthen M, Elger CE, et al. spectroscopy in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiat 2000;13:21–6. Hippocampal event-related potentials to tone duration deviance in a passive Sawada M, Iida J, Ota T, Negoro H, Tanaka S, Sadamatsu M, Kishimoto T. Effects of oddball paradigm in humans. NeuroImage 2007;37:274–81. osmotic-release methylphenidate in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder as Rothenberger, A. Electrical brain activity in children with hyperkinetic syndrome: measured by event-related potentials. Psychiat Clin Neuros 2010;64:491–8. evidence of frontal cortical dysfunction. In: Sergeant J. (Ed.), European Schall U, Catts SV, Chaturvedi S, Liebert B, Redenbach J, Karayanidis F, et al. The approaches to hyperkinetic disorder. Zürich: Trümpi; 1995. p. 255–270. effect of clozapine therapy on frontal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia: Rothenberger A, Banaschewski T, Heinrich H, Moll GH, Schmidt MH, van’t Klooster neuropsychology and event-related potential measures. Int J B. Comorbidity in ADHD-children: effects of coexisting conduct disorder or tic Neuropsychopharmacol 1998;1:19–29. disorder on event-related brain potentials in an auditory selective-attention Schall U, Catts SV, Karayanidis F, Ward PB. Auditory event-related potential indices task. Eur Arch Psychiat Clin Neurosci 2000;250:101–10. of fronto-temporal information processing in schizophrenia syndromes: valid Rowland LP, Shneider NA. Medical progress: amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. New outcome prediction of clozapine therapy in a three-year follow-up. Int J Engl J Med 2001;344:1688–700. Neuropsychopharmacol 1999;2:83–93. Ruby P, Caclin A, Boulet S, Delpuech C, Morlet D. Odd sound processing in the Schall U, Johnston P, Todd J, Ward PB, Michie PT. Functional neuroanatomy of sleeping brain. J Cognitive Neurosci 2008;20:296–311. auditory mismatch processing: an event-related fMRI study of duration-deviant Rudner M, Foo C, Sundewall-Thorén E, Lunner T, Rönnberg J. Phonological mismatch oddballs. NeuroImage 2003;20:729–36. and explicit cognitive processing in a sample of 102 hearing-aid users. Int J Schiff S, Valenti P, Andrea P, Lot M, Bisiacchi P, Gatta A, et al. The effect of aging on Audiol 2008;47:S163–70. auditory components of event-related brain potentials. Clin Neurophysiol Ruusuvirta T, Huotilainen M, Fellman V, Näätänen R. Numerical discrimination in 2008;119:1795–802. newborn infants as revealed by event-related potentials to tone sequences. Eur J Schreiber H, Stolz-Born G, Kornhuber H, Born J. Event-related potential correlates of Neurosci 2009;30:620–4. impaired selective attention in children at high risk for schizophrenia. Biol Ruusuvirta T, Koivisto K, Wikgren J, Astikainen P. Processing of melodic contours in Psychiat 1992;32:634–51. urethane-anaesthetized rats. Eur J Neurosci 2007;26:701–3. Schreiber H, Stolz-Born G, Pietrowsky R, Kornhuber HH, Fehm HL, Born J. Improved Ruusuvirta T, Penttonen M, Korhonen T. Auditory cortical event-related potentials event-related potential signs of selective attention after the administration of to pitch deviances in rats. Neurosci Lett 1998;248:45–8. the cholecystokinin analog ceruletide in healthy persons. Biol Psychiat Salisbury DF, Kuroki N, Kasai K, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Progressive and 1995;37:702–12. interrelated functional and structural evidence of post-onset brain reduction in Schroeder MM, Ritter W, Vaughan Jr HG. The mismatch negativity to novel stimuli schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat 2007;64:521–9. reflects cognitive decline. Ann NY Acad Sci 1995;769:399–401. Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, Griggs CB, Bonner-Jackson A, McCarley RW. Mismatch Schroeder MM, Handelsman L, Torres L, Dorfman D, Rinaldi P, Jacobsen J, et al. Early negativity in chronic schizophrenia and first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen and late cognitive event-related potentials mark stages of HIV-1 infection in the Psychiat 2002;59:686–94. drug-user risk group. Biol Psychiat 1994;35:54–69. 456 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Schröger E. On the detection of auditory deviants: a pre-attentive activation Smolnik R, Molle M, Fehm HL, Born J. Brain potentials and attention after acute and model. Psychophysiol 1997;34:245–2457. subchronic intranasal administration of ACTH 4-10 and desacetyl-alpha-MSH in Schröger E, Tervaniemi M, Wolff C, Näätänen R. Preattentive periodicity detection in humans. Neuroendocrinology 1999;70:63–72. auditory patterns as governed by time and intensity information. Cognit Brain Smolnik R, Pietrowsky R, Fehm HL, Born J. Enhanced selective attention after low- Res 1996;4:145–8. dose administration of the benzodiazepine antagonist flumazenil. J Clin Schulte-Körne G, Bruder J. Clinical neurophysiology of visual and auditory Psychopharm 1998;18:241–7. processing in dyslexia: a review. Clinl Neurophysiol 2010;121:1794–809. Sorokin A, Alku P, Kujala T. Change and novelty detection in speech and non-speech Schulte-Körne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, Remschmidt H. Auditory processing and sound streams. Brain Res 2010;1327:77–90. dyslexia: evidence for a specific speech processing deficit. NeuroReport Spackman LA, Towell A, Boyd SG. Somatosensory discrimination: an intracranial 1998;9:337–40. event-related potential study of children with refractory epilepsy. Brain Res Schulte-Körne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, Remschmidt H. Pre-attentive processing of 2010;1310:68–76. auditory patterns in dyslexic human subjects. Neurosci Lett 1999;276:41–4. Spackman LA, Boyd SG, Towell A. Effects of stimulus frequency and duration on Schulte-Körne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, Remschmidt H. Speech perception deficit in somatosensory discrimination responses. Exp Brain Res 2007;117:21–30. dyslexic adults as measured by mismatch negativity (MMN). Int J Psychophysiol Squires KC, Squires NK, Hillyard SA. Decision-related cortical potentials during an 2001;40:77–87. auditory signal detection task with cued observation intervals. J Exp Psychol Sculthorpe LD, Ouellet DR, Campbell KB. MMN elicitation during natural sleep to Human 1975;1:268–79. violations of an auditory pattern. Brain Res 2009;1290:52–62. Srinivasan N, Baijal S. Concentrative meditation enhances preattentive processing: a Schwieler L, Linderholm KR, Nilsson-Todd LK, Erhardt S, Engberg G. Clozapine mismatch negativity study. NeuroReport 2007;18:1709–12. interacts with the glycine site of the NMDA receptor: electrophysiological studies Stagg C, Hindley P, Tales A, Butler S. Visual mismatch negativity: the detection of of dopamine neurons in the rat ventral tegmental area. Life Sci 2008;83:170–5. stimulus change. NeuroReport 2004;15:659–63. Sebastian C, Yasin I. Speech versus tone processing in compensated dyslexia: Stapells DR. Cortical event related potentials to auditory stimuli. In: Katz J. (Ed.), discrimination and lateralization with a dichotic mismatch negativity (MMN) Handbook of clinical audiology, 5th ed. Baltimore, MD: Lippincott & Wilkins; paradigm. Int J Psychophysiol 2008;70:115–26. 2002. p. 378–406. Seri S, Cerquiglini A, Pisani F, Curatolo P. Autism in tuberous sclerosis: evoked Stefanics G, Kimura M, Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity reveals automatic potential evidence for a deficit in auditory sensory processing. Clin detection of sequential regularity violation. Front Hum Neurosci 2011;5:46. Neurophysiol 1999;110:1825–30. Stix G. How to build a better learner. Sci Am 2011;305:50–7. Seri S, Pisani F, Thai JN, Cerquiglini A. Pre-attentive sensory processing in autistic Stone JM, Bramon E, Pauls A, Sumich A, McGuire PK. Thalamic neurochemical spectrum disorder. Are electromagnetic measurements telling us a coherent abnormalities in individuals with prodromal symptoms of schizophrenia – story. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;63:159–63. relationship to auditory event-related potentials. Psychiat Res-Neuroim Serra JM, Escera C, Sánchez-Turet M, Sánchez-Sastre J, Grau C. The H1-receptor 2010;183:174–6. antagonist chlorpheniramine decreases the ending phase of the mismatch Stoodley CJ, Hill R, Stein JF, Bishop DVM. Auditory event-related potentials differ in negativity of the human auditory event-related potentials. Neurosci Lett dyslexic even when auditory psychophysical performance is normal. Brain Res 1996;203:77–80. 2006;1121:190–9. Shafer VL, Morr ML, Datta H, Kurzberg D, Schwartz RG. Neurophysiological indexes Sullivan EV, Shear PK, Zipursky RB, Sagar HJ, Pfefferbaum A. A deficit profile of of speech processing deficits in children with specific language impairment. J executive, memory, and motor functions in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat Cognitive Neurosci 2005;17:1168–80. 1995;36:641–53. Shafer VL, Ponton C, Datta H, Morr ML, Schwartz RG. Neurophysiological indices of Sysoeva OV, Maluchenko NV, Smirnov KS, Shleptsova VA, Ivanitsky AM, Tonevitsky attention to speech in children with specific language impairment. Clin AG. Pecularities of brain information processing in persons with different Neurophysiol 2007;118:1230–43. serotonin transporter gene variants. B Exp Biol Med 2009;148:731–4. Shankarnarayan VC, Maruthy S. Mismatch negativity in children with dyslexia Takei Y, Kumano S, Hattori S, Uehara T, Kawakubo Y, Kasai K, et al. Preattentive speaking Indian languages. Behav Brain Funct 2007;3:36. dysfunction in major depression: a magnetoencephalography study using Sharma M, Purdy SC, Newall P, Wheldall K, Beaman R, Dillon H. Electrophysiological auditory mismatch negativity. Psychophysiology 2009;46:52–61. and behavioral evidence of auditory processing deficits in children with reading Takei Y, Kumano S, Maki Y, Hattori S, Kawakubo Y, Kasai K, et al. Preattentive disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;117:1130–44. dysfunction in bipolar disorder: a MEG study using mismatch negativity. Prog Sharma M, Purdy SC, Newall P, Wheldall K, Beaman R. Refractory effects on Neuro-Psychoph 2010;34:903–12. auditory-evoked responses in children with reading disorders. NeuroReport Tales A, Butler S. Visual mismatch negativity highlights abnormal preattentive 2007;18:133–6. visual processing in Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroReport 2006;17:887–90. Sharma A, Purdy SC, Newall P, Wheldall K, Beaman R, Dillon H. Electrophysiological Tales A, Haworth J, Wilcock G, Newton P, Butler S. Visual mismatch negativity and behavioral evidence of auditory processing deficits in children with reading highlights abnormal pre-attentive visual processing in mild cognitive disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 2006;117:1130–44. impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:1224–32. Sharma M, Purdy AS, Kelly SC. Comorbidity of auditory processing, language, and Tales A, Muir J L, Bayer A, Jones R, Snowden RJ. Phasic visual alertness in Alzheimer’s reading disorders. J Speech Lang Hear R 2009;52:706–22. disease and ageing. NeuroReport 2002a;13:2557–60. Shelley AM, Ward PB, Catts SV, Michie PT, Andrews S, McConaghy N. Mismatch Tales A, Newton P, Troscianko T, Butler S. Mismatch negativity in the visual negativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizophrenia. Biol modality. NeuroReport 1999;110:3363–7. Psychiat 1991;203:77–80. Tales A, Troscianko T, Wilcock GK, Newton P, Butler SR. Age-related changes in the Shestakova A, Brattico E, Huotilainen M, Galunov V, Soloviev A, Sams M, et al. preattentional detection of visual change. NeuroReport 2002b;13:969–72. Abstract phoneme representations in the left temporal cortex: magnetic Tanaka M, Okubo O, Fuchigami T, Harada K. A study of mismatch negativity in mismatch negativity study. NeuroReport 2002;13:1813–6. newborns. Pediatr Int 2001;43:281–6. Shestakova A, Huotilainen M, Ceponiene R, Cheour M. Event-related potentials Tarkka IM, Luukainen-Markkula R, Pitkänen K, Hämäläinen H. Alterations in visual associated with second language learning in children. Clin Neurophysiol and auditory processing in hemispatial neglect: an evoked potential follow-up 2003;114:1507–12. study. Int J Psychophysiol 2011;79:272–9. Shin KS, Kim JS, Kang DH, Koh Y, Choi JS, O’Donnell BF, et al. Pre-attentive auditory Taylor MJ, Baldeweg T. Application of EEG, ERP and intracranial recordings to the processing in ultra-high-risk for schizophrenia with magnetoencephalography. investigation of cognitive functions in children. Dev Sci 2002;5:318–34. Biol Psychiat 2009;65:1071–8. Tervaniemi M, Winkler I, Näätänen R. Pre-attentive categorization of sounds by Shinozaki N, Yabe H, Sato Y, Hiruma T, Sutoh T, Nashida T, et al. The difference in timbre as revealed by event-related potentials. NeuroReport 1997;8:2571–4. mismatch negativity between the acute and post-acute phase of schizophrenia. Tervaniemi M, Kujala A, Alho K, Virtanen J, Ilmoniemi RJ, Näätänen R. Functional Biol Psychol 2002;59:105–19. specialization of the human auditory cortex in processing phonetic and Shinozaki N, Yabe H, Sutoh T, Hiruma T, Kaneko S. Somatosensory automatic musical sounds: a magnetoencephalographic (MEG) study. NeuroImage 1999; responses to deviant stimuli. Cognit Brain Res 1998;7:165–71. 9:330–6. Shtyrov Y, Pulvermüller F. Language in the mismatch negativity design: Tervaniemi M, Medvedev SV, Alho K, Pakhomov SV, Roudas MS, van Zuijen TL, et al. motivations, benefits, and prospects. J Psychophysiol 2007;21:176–87. Lateralized automatic auditory processing of phonetic versus musical Shtyrov Y, Kujala T, Ahveninen J, Tervaniemi M, Alku P, Ilmoniemi RJ, et al. information: a PET study. Hum Brain Mapp 2000;10:74–9. Background acoustic noise and the hemispheric lateralization of speech Thönnesen H, Zvyagintsev M, Harke KC, Boers F, Dammers J, Norra C, et al. processing in the human brain: magnetic mismatch negativity study. Optimized mismatch negativity paradigm reflects deficits in schizophrenia Neurosci Lett 1998;251:141–4. patients. A combined EEG and MEG study. Biol Psychol 2008;77:205–16. Shtyrov Y, Kujala T, Ilmoniemi RJ, Näätänen R. Noise affects speech-signal Tikhonravov D, Neuvonen T, Pertovaara A, Savioja K, Ruusuvirta T, Näätänen R, et al. processing differently in the cerebral hemispheres. NeuroReport Effects of an NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 on an MMN-like response 1999;10:2189–92. recorded in anesthetized rats. Brain Res 2008;1203:97–102. Siegal M, Blades M. Language and auditory processing in autism. Trends Cogn Sci Tikhonravov D, Neuvonen T, Pertovaara A, Savioja K, Ruusuvirta T, Näätänen R, et al. 2003;7:378–80. Dose-related effects of memantine on a mismatch negativity-like response in Simoens VL, Istók E, Hyttinen S, Hirvonen A, Näätänen R, Tervaniemi M. anesthetized rats. Neuroscience 2010;167:1175–82. Psychosocial stress attenuates general sound processing and duration change Todd J, Michie PT, Jablensky AV. Association between reduced duration mismatch detection. Psychophysiology 2007;44:30–8. negativity (MMN) and raised temporal discrimination thresholds in Simpson TP, Manara AR, Kane NM, Barton RL, Rowlands CA, Butler SR. Effect of schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:2061–70. propofol anaesthesia on the event-related potential mismatch negativity and Todd J, Michie PT, Schall U, Karayanidis F, Yabe H, Näätänen R. Deviant matters: the auditory-evoked potential N1. Britt J Anaesthesia 2002;89:382–8. duration, frequency, and intensity deviants reveal different patterns of R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 457

mismatch negativity reduction in early and late schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat van Hooff JC, de Beer NAM, Brunia CHM, Cluitmans PJM, Kortsen HHM, Tavilla G, 2008;63:58–64. et al. Information processing during cardiac surgery: an event related potential Todd, J, Michie PT, Schall U, Ward P, Catts S. Mismatch negativity (MMN) reduction study. Electroen Clin Neuro 1995;96:433–52. in schizophrenia -impaired prediction-error generation, estimation or salience. Vanhanen M, Karhu J, Koivisto K, Pääkkönen A, Partanen J, Laakso M, et al. ERPs Int J Psychophysiol, in press. reveal deficits in automatic cerebral stimulus processing in patients with Toiviainen P, Tervaniemi M, Louhivuori J, Saher M, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. NIDDM. NeuroReport 1996;7:2767–71. Timbre similarity: convergence of neural, behavioral, and computational Vanhaudenhuyse A, Laureys S, Perrin F. Cognitive event-related potentials in approaches. Music Percept 1998;16:223–41. comatose and post-comatose states. Neurocrit Care 2008;8:262–70. Towey JP, Tenke CE, Bruder GE, Leite P, Friedman D, Liebowitz M, et al. Brain event- van Leeuwen T, Been P, Kuijpers C, Zwarts F, Maassen B, van der Leij A. Mismatch related potential correlates of overfocused attention in obsessive-compulsive response is absent in 2-month-old infants at risk for dyslexia. NeuroReport disorder. Psychophysiology 1994;31:535–43. 2006;17:351–5. Toyomaki A, Kusumi I, Matsuyama T, Kako Y, Ito K, Koyama T. Tone duration van Woerkom TC, Fortgens C, Rompel-Martens CM, van de Wetering BJ. Auditory mismatch negativity deficits predict impairment of executive function in event-related potentials in adult patients with Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome schizophrenia. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 2008;32:95–9. in the oddball paradigm. Electroen Clin Neuro 1988;71:443–9. Trainor L, McFadden M, Hodgson L, Darragh L, Barlow J, Matsos L, et al. Changes in van Woerkom TCAM, Roos RAC, van Dijk JG. Altered attentional processing of auditory cortex and the development of mismatch negativity between 2 and 6 background stimuli in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: a study in auditory months of age. Int J Psychophysiol 2003;51:5–15. event-related potentials evoked in an oddball paradigm. Acta Neurol Scand Trainor L, Samuel SS, Desjardins RN, Sonnadara RR. Measuring temporal resolution 1994;90:116–23. in infants using mismatch negativity. NeuroReport 2001;12:2443–8. Vaz Pato M, Jones SJ, Perez N, Sprague LC. Mismatch negativity to single and Trautwein PG, Ponton CW, Kwong B, Waring MD. Neuropsysiological and multiple pitch-deviant tones in regular and pseudo-random complex tone psychophysical measures of duration discrimination in normal-hearing adults sequences. Neurophysiology 2002;113:519–27. and adults with cochlear implants. In: Proceedings 16th international congress Verbaten MN, van Engeland H. (Neuro) physiologic similarities between childhood on acoustics and 135th meeting Acoustic Society of America, 1998. p. 877-8. developmental disorders and schizophrenia. In: Den Boer JA, Westenberg HGM, Tremblay K, Kraus N, Carrel TD, McGee T. Central auditory system plasticity: Praag HM. (Eds.), Advances in the neurobiology of schizophrenia. John Wiley & Generalization to novel stimuli following listening training. J Acoust Soc Am Sons Ltd; 1995. p. 369–400. 1997;102:3762–73. Verbaten MN, Overtoom CCE, Koelega HS, Swaab-Barneveld H, van der Gaag RJ, Tremblay K, Kraus N, McGee T. The time course of auditory perceptual learning: Buitelaar J, et al. Methylphenidate influences on both early and late ERP waves neurophysiological changes during speech-sound training. NeuroReport of ADHD children in a continuous performance test. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1998;9:3557–60. 1994;22:561–78. Troche SJ, Houlihan ME, Stelmack RM, Rammsayer TH. Mental ability and the Verleger R, Neukäter W, Kömpf D, Viereggem P. On the reasons for the delay of P3 discrimination of auditory frequency and duration change without focused latency in healthy elderly subjects. Electroen Clin Neuro 1991;79:488–502. attention: an analysis of mismatch negativity. Pers Individ Differ Vieregger P, Verleger R, Wascher E, Stüven F, Kömpf D. Auditory selective attention 2010;49:228–33. is impaired in Parkinson’s disease – event-related evidence from EEG potentials. Troche SJ, Houlihan ME, Stelmack RM, Rammsayer TH. Mental ability, P300, and Cognit Brain Res 1994;2:117–29. mismatch negativity: analysis of frequency and duration discrimination. Vuust P, Brattico E, Glerean E, Seppänen M, Pakarinen S, Tervaniemi M, et al. New Intelligence 2009;37:365–73. fast mismatch negativity paradigm for determining the neural prerequisites for Tse C-Y, Penney TB. On the functional role of temporal and frontal cortex activation musical ability. Cortex 2011;47:1091–9. in passive detection of auditory deviance. NeuroImage 2008;41:1462–70. Wable J, van der Abbeele T, Gallégo S, Frachet B. Mismatch negativity: a tool for the Turetsky BI, Calkins ME, Light GA, Olincy A, Radant AD, Swerdlow NR. assessment of stimuli discrimination in cochlear implant subjects. Clin Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected Neurophysiol 2000;111:743–51. candidate measures. Schizophrenia Bull 2007;33:69–94. Wang W, Zhu SZ, Pan LC, Hu AH, Wang YH. Mismatch negativity and personality Turetsky BI, Bilker WE, Siegel SJ, Kohler CG, Gur RE. Profile of auditory information- traits in chronic primary insomniacs. Funct Neurol 2001;16:3–10. processing deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiat Res 2009;165:27–37. Weber C, Hahne A, Friedrich M, Friederici AD. Reduced stress pattern discrimination Umbricht D, Krljes S. Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. in 5-month-olds as a marker of risk for later language impairment: Schizophr Res 2005;76:1–23. neurophysiological evidence. Cognit Brain Res 2005;25:180–7. Umbricht DS, Bates JA, Lieberman JA, Kane JM, Javitt DC. Electrophysiological indices Wei J-H, Chan T-C, Luo Y-J. A modified oddball paradigm ‘‘cross-modal delayed of automatic and controlled auditory information processing in first-episode, response’’ and the research on mismatch negativity. Brain Res Bull recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 2006;59:762–72. 2002;57:221–30. Umbricht D, Javitt D, Novak G, Bates J, Pollack S, Lieberman J, et al. Effects of Wertz RT, Auther LL, Burch-Sims GP, Abou-Khalil R, Kirshner HS, Duncan GW. A clozapine on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat comparison of the mismatch negativity (MMN) event-related potential to tone 1998;44:716–25. and speech stimuli in normal and aphasic adults. Aphasiology 1998;12:499–507. Umbricht D, Javitt D, Novak G, Bates J, Pollack S, Lieberman J, et al. Effects of Wible CG, Kubicki M, Yoo S-S, Kacher DF, Salisbury DF, Anderson MC, et al. A risperidone on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia. Int J functional magnetic resonance imaging study of auditory mismatch in Neuropsychopharmacol 1999;2:299–304. schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat 2001;158:938–43. Umbricht D, Koller R, Schmid L, Skrabo A, Grübel C, Huber T, et al. How specific are Wienberg M, Glenthoj BY, Jensen KS, Oranje B. A single high dose of escitalopram deficits in mismatch negativity generation to schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat increases mismatch negativity without affecting processing negativity or P300 2003;53:1120–31. amplitude in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 2010;24:1183–92. Umbricht D, Koller R, Vollenweider FX, Schmid L. Mismatch negativity predicts Wijnen VJM, van Boxtel GJM, Eilander HJ, de Gelder B. Mismatch negativity predicts psychotic experiences induced by NMDA receptor antagonist in healthy recovery from the vegetative state. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:597–605. volunteers. Biol Psychiat 2002;51:400–6. Wild-Wall N, Oades RD, Juran SA. Maturation processes in automatic change Umbricht D, Schmid L, Koller R, Vollenweider FX, Hell D, Javitt DC. Ketamine- detection as revealed by event-related brain potentials and dipole source induced deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in healthy localization: significance for adult AD/HD. Int J Psychophysiol 2005;58: volunteers: implications for models of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Arch 34–46. Gen Psychiat 2000;57:1139–47. Winkler I. Interpreting the mismatch negativity. J Psychophysiol 2007;21:147–63. Umbricht D, Vyssotki D, Latanov A, Nitsch R, Lipp HP. Deviant-related Winkler I, Näätänen R. The effects of auditory backward masking on event-related electrophysiological activity in mice: is there mismatch negativity in mice. brain potentials. Electroen Clin Neuro 1994;44:185–9. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:353–63. Winkler I, Reinikainen K, Näätänen R. Event-related brain potentials reflect echoic Urban A, Kremlácek J, Masopust J, Libiger J. Visual mismatch negativity among memory in humans. Percept Psychophys 1993;53:443–9. patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2008;102:320–8. Winkler I, Kujala T, Tiitinen H, Sivonen P, Alku P, Lehtokoski A, et al. Brain responses Uwer R, Albrecht R, von Suchodoletz W. Automatic processing of tones and speech reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes. Psychophysiology stimuli in children with specific language impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol 1999c;36:638–42. 2002;44:527–32. Winsberg BG, Javitt DC, Silippo GS. Electrophysiological indices of information Uwer R, von Suchodoletz W. Stability of mismatch negativities in children. Clin processing in methylphenidate responders. Biol Psychiat 1997;42:434–45. Neurophysiol 2000;111:45–52. Winsberg BG, Javitt DC, Silippo GS, Doneshka P. Mismatch negativity in hyperactive Valeriani M, Galli F, Tarantino S, Graceffa D, Pignata E, Miliucci R, et al. Correlation children: effects of methylphenidate. Psychopharmacol Bull 1993;29:229–33. between normal brain excitability and emotional symptomatology in paediatric Woods DL. Auditory selective attention in middle-aged and elderly subjects: an migraine. Cephalalgia 2008;29:204–13. event-related brain potential study. Electroen Clin Neuro 1992;84:456–68. van der Stelt O, Belger A. Application of electroencephalography to the study of Woods DL, Clayworth CC. Age-related changes in human middle latency auditory cognitive and brain functions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2007;33:955–70. evoked potentials. Electroen Clin Neuro 1986;65:297–303. van der Stelt O, van Boxtel GJM. Auditory P300 and mismatch negativity in Woods DL, Knight RT. Electrophysiologic evidence of increased distractibility after comatose states. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:2172–4. dorsolateral prefrontal lesions. Neurology 1986;36:212–6. van Hooff JC, de Beer NAM, Brunia CHM, Cluitmans PJM, Kortsen HHM. Event- Woods DL, Alho K, Algazi A. Intermodal selective attention I: effects on event- related potential measures of information processing during general anesthesia. related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli. Electroen Clin Electroen Clin Neuro 1997;103:268–81. Neuro 1992;82:341–55. 458 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Wu JC, Maguire G, Riley G, Lee A, Keator D, Tang C, et al. Increased dopamine Yppärilä H, Karhu J, Westerén-Punnonen S, Musialowicz T, Partanen J. Evidence of activity associated with stuttering. NeuroReport 1997;8:767–70. auditory processing during postoperative propofol sedation. Clini Neurophysiol Wynn JK, Sugar C, Horan WP, Kern R, Green MF. Mismatch negativity, social 2002;113:1357–64. cognition, and functioning in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiat Zarza-Lucianez A, Arce-Arce S, Bhathal H, Sanjuan-Martin F. Mismatch negativity 2010;67:940–7. and conscience level in severe traumatic brain injury. Rev Neurologia Yamasue H, Yamada H, Yumoto M, Kamio S, Kudo N, Uetsuki M, et al. Abnormal 2007;44:465–8. association between reduced magnetic mismatch field to speech sounds and Zerouali Y, Jemel B, Godbout R. The effects of early and late night partial sleep smaller left planum temporale volume in schizophrenia. NeuroImage deprivation on automatic and selective attention: an ERP study. Brain Res 2004;22:720–7. 2010;1308:87–99. Yokoyama Y, Nakashima K, Shimoyama R, Urakami K, Takahashi K. Distribution of Zhang XL, Cohen HL, Porjesz B, Begleiter H. Mismatch negativity in subjects at high event-related potentials in patients with dementia. Electroen Clin Neuro risk for alcoholism. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2001;25:330–7. 1995;35:431–7. Zurrón M, Díaz F. Auditory event-related potentials in mentally retarded subjects Youn T, Park H-J, Kim J-J, Kim MS, Kwon JS. Altered hemispheric asymmetry and during active and passive oddball experiments. Biol Psychiat 1997;41:201–8. positive symptoms in schizophrenia: equivalent current dipole of auditory mismatch negativity. Schizophr Res 2003;59:253–60.