Specific Language Impairment (SLI) Versus Speech Sound Disorders

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Specific Language Impairment (SLI) Versus Speech Sound Disorders https://edition.pagesuite-professional.co.uk/html5/reader/production/default.aspx?pubname=&edid=558600cb-8de9-4616-9242-f8f3b41da315 This article originally appeared in the May 2018 PROFILE issue of Open Access Government Specific Language Impairment (SLI) versus Speech Sound Disorders (SSD) The important differences between Specific Language Impairment (SLI) in children and Speech Sound Disorders (SSD) in children are placed under the spotlight by Mabel L. Rice, Fred & Virginia Merrill Distinguished Professor of Advanced Studies at the University of Kansas round the world young chil- they are hearing. Their first job is to SSD can be obvious to adult listeners dren are expected to learn refine them to match the ones they of young children. Young children’s the language they overhear in need and to drop the ones that may attempts to talk can be unintelligible, Aconversations around them. This is a not matter and to develop the motor especially when they are very young. robust, spontaneous ability of humans, control needed to do that. If unintelligibility persists as children unlike, for example, reading, which age, it becomes noticeable and a must be explicitly taught. One promi- Humans at birth are equipped with matter of concern because it is nent scholar of children’s language motor movements for breathing, not “typical.” Some mispronunciations acquisition once put it this way: “… sucking and swallowing (basic func- are understandable but regarded as there is virtually no way to prevent it tions for survival), along with a wide immature, such as “wabbit” for “rabbit”, from happening short of raising a child range of sounds. Some vocalisations “thoup” for “soup”, or “bawoon” for in a barrel.”1 serve communicative purposes, and “balloon.” Scholars have tracked the some are biological (such as burps or order in which children learn their Although true of most children, chil- coughs). Between one and two years speech sounds and have developed dren with Specific Language Impair- of age, babies master more refined age norms for evaluating whether a ment (SLI) are the exceptions to this tongue, lips and palatal movements child meets age expectations4. The assumption. These are children who do needed for speaking the words and prevalence of SSD in 4-6-year-old not have overt neurodevelopmental sentences of a language. This children in population-based cohorts disorders, hearing impairments, or requires a fine-tuned synchrony of is approximately 3-6%5 and the condi- other obvious causes of developmental muscles, sound perception and the tion appears to resolve in 75% of disorders and who live in ordinary cognitive centres of the brain. The children by age 66. families. Yet they are later than other output is a sequenced speech children in learning a language and pattern, such as what we hear in a People often assume SSD is the same are at risk for persistent low language phone conversation. as SLI, such that children’s speech abilities into adulthood. The best abilities reflect their underlying estimates for the prevalence of SLI are The second part of the human language abilities or vice versa. This is 7-10% of children at school entry (5-6 language capacity is more covert, a not true. In the most precise study of years).2,3 matter of cognitive processes in the a population-based sample of 5-year- brain that do not require a speech old children, the co-occurrence of SLI is often confused with Speech production system. For example, deaf speech and language impairments, Sound Disorders (SSD) in children. children can acquire a language once adjusting for age expectations, Between one and five years of age system that can be expressed in phys- was estimated at less than 2%5. For children are learning two distinctly ical signs of the hands, face and body the children with SLI, speech impair- different parts of the human language postures. Language emerges in young ment was evident in approximately capacity. One is the production of children first in short utterances 5-8% of the children. The authors speech sounds needed in their native that lengthen with age. In English, lan- concluded that SSD and SLI are inde- language. In the beginning, children guage emerges as one or two words pendent; they are not likely to co-occur. can produce more sounds than they at a time, which relatively quickly Thus, SSD is not a diagnostic marker need for the language or languages expand to phrases or sentences. of SLI and presumably, the two condi- 310 PROFILE tions do not share a common causal says “why ah de wabbits wed?” The References pathway. substitution of w/r in “rabbit” and 1. Pinker S. Language learnability and language development. Cam- “red”, along with the omission of the bridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1984. The non-overlap of SLI and SSD carries final /r/ in the word “are,” are not 2. Tomblin JB, Records NL, Buckwalter P, Zhang X, Smith E, O'Brien M. The prevalence of specific language impairment in kinder- implications for public health services unusual speech errors in the speech garten children. J Speech Hear Res. 1997;40:1245-1260. and for scientific studies of the nature of young boys. 3. Norbury CF, Gooch D, Wray C, et al. The impact of nonverbal ability and origins of SLI and SSD. on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: Such errors are quite noticeable evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2016;57(11):1247-1257. • A big issue for public health services although they often do not interfere 4. Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 is that children with SLI are likely to with adults’ understanding of the (2nd Edition). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; be overlooked as needing language intended meanings. The other boy, 2000. intervention services, perhaps in part dressed in green, asks a question 5. Shriberg LD, Tomblin JB, McSweeny JL. Prevalence of speech because of the fact that SSD may be formulated in the adult grammar as: delay in 6-year-old children and comorbidity with language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42:1461-1481. obvious to adults/caregivers, but SLI “why is/are that rabbit/those rabbits is not7. red?” The boy says “Why that red?”, a 6. Shriberg LD. Five subtypes of developmental phonological disorders. Clinical Communication Disorders 1994;4:38-53. sentence consistent with the gram- 7. Rice ML. Specific Language Impairment in Children. https://ww- • SSD is likely to resolve with age mar rules for children this age with wopenaccessgovernmentorg/specific-language-impairment-in- 7 (children are likely to “outgrow” it) SLI . He demonstrates a deficiency in chi 2017. whereas SLI is likely to persist into sentence structure, with the omission 8. Catts HW, Fey ME, Weismer SE, Bridges MS. The relationship adulthood7. of the obligatory copula form of BE between language and reading abilities. In: Tomblin JB, Nippold MA, eds. Understanding individual differences in language (“is”’ or “are”) and the substitution of a development across the school years New York City, New York: • At school entry, SLI predicts later pronoun (“that”) for the common Psychology Press; 2014:144-165. 8 reading impairments whereas SSD noun “rabbit.” Furthermore, the 9. Sices L, Taylor HG, Freebairn L, Hansen A, Lewis B. Relationship predicts weakly, if at all, once specification of singular versus plural between speech-sound disorders and early literacy skills in preschool-age children: Impact of comorbid langauge impair- adjusted for co-occurring language for the noun phrase is vague because ment. J Dev Behav Pediatr,. 2007;6:438-447. impairments.9,10 the noun information is underspeci- 10. Hayiou-Thomas ME, Carroll JM, Leavett R, Hulme C, Snowling fied. His articulation of speech sounds MJ. When does speech sound disorder matter for literacy? The • Scientific studies of children’s com- is at adult levels and his meaning is role of disordered speech errors, co-occurring language impair- ment and family risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology munication problems in medical con- effectively conveyed. and Psychiatry. 2017;58(2):197-205. ditions should differentiate between 11. Rice ML, Russell JS, Frederick T, et al. Risk for speech and language SLI and SSD. A recent study of children Although the speech sound errors of impairments in pre-school aged HIV-exposed uninfected children exposed to Human Immunodefi- the child with SSD are likely to be with in utero combination antiretroviral exposure. The Pediatric Infectious Disease Journal. Accepted Dec 2017. ciency Virus (HIV) is the first report of noticed and to generate attempts by SSD outcomes compared to primary adults to correct the problem, the language impairments (i.e., without grammar errors of the child with SLI other developmental disorders). The are less likely to be noticed or under- risk for language impairments in the stood as flags for concern. Yet it is the children was higher than population child in green who is at higher risk of norms but the risk for SSD was not adverse developmental outcomes elevated.11 than the child in purple, who is more likely to “outgrow” the SSD and less Examples of how children talk can likely to encounter problems with illustrate the differences between SLI literacy, school achievement, or and SSD. Consider two 5-year-old long-term persistence of subtle but Mabel L. Rice boys. They are talking about a picture very important elements of grammar Fred & Virginia Merrill Distinguished of red rabbits. One, dressed in purple, and vocabulary. Our research and Professor of Advanced Studies has an SSD, apparent in his mispro- our service systems will be improved University of Kansas nunciations of the speech sounds by increased recognition of the Tel: +1 785 864 4570 needed to say “why are the rabbits important differences between SLI [email protected] red.” Within his speech system, he and SSD.
Recommended publications
  • Phonological Processing Deficits As a Universal Model for Dyslexia
    DOI: 10.1590/2317-1782/20142014135 Systematic Review Phonological processing deficits as a universal model Revisão Sistemática for dyslexia: evidence from different orthographies Ana Luiza Gomes Pinto Navas1 Déficit em processamento fonológico como um modelo universal Érica de Cássia Ferraz2 Juliana Postigo Amorina Borges2 para a dislexia: evidência a partir de diferentes ortografias Keywords ABSTRACT Dyslexia Purpose: To verify the universal nature of the phonological processing deficit hypothesis for dyslexia, since Education the most influential studies on the topic were conducted in children or adults speakers of English. Research Language strategy: A systematic review was designed, conducted and analyzed using PubMed, Science Direct, and Reading SciELO databases. Selection criteria: The literature search was conducted using the terms “phonological Review processing” AND “dyslexia” in publications of the last ten years (2004–2014). Data analysis: Following screening of (a) titles and abstracts and (b) full papers, 187 articles were identified as meeting the pre- established inclusion criteria. Results: The phonological processing deficit hypothesis was explored in studies involving several languages. More importantly, we identify studies in all types of writing systems such as ideographic, syllabic and logographic, as well as alphabetic orthography, with different levels of orthography- phonology consistency. Conclusion: The phonological processing hypothesis was considered as a valid explanation to dyslexia, in a wide variety of spoken languages and writing systems. Descritores RESUMO Dislexia Objetivo: Verificar a natureza universal da hipótese do déficit de processamento fonológico para a dislexia, uma Educação vez que os estudos mais influentes sobre o tema foram conduzidos com crianças ou adultos falantes do Inglês. Linguagem Estratégia de pesquisa: Uma revisão sistemática foi planejada, conduzida e analisada utilizando as bases de Leitura dados PubMed, Science Direct e SciELO.
    [Show full text]
  • Recognizing and Referring Children at Risk for Developmental Coordination Disorder: Role of the Speech-Language Pathologist
    Recognizing and Referring Children at Risk for Developmental Coordination Disorder: Role of the Speech-Language Pathologist Reconnaitre et envoyer en consultation des enfants a risque de trouble de r acquisition de la coordination: le role de r orthophoniste Cheryl Missiuna, B. Robin Gaines, Nancy Pollock Abstract Speech-language pathologists are in a unique position to assist families with the process of early identification of motor coordination disorders in children. While families and physicians may be focused primarily on the child's communication delay, a child's fine and gross motor abilities should also be progressing at a rapid rate during the preschool years. Research reviews indicate that a significant number of children with speech-language delays and disorders will demonstrate concomitant motor coordination difficulties which, when left untreated, may impact the child's later social and academic progress. Many ofthese children display the characteristics of Developmental Coordination Disorder. This article describes the clinical observations of motor development specialists and delineates some key child-behaviours and some clinician-helping behaviours to watch for when working with a preschool speech and language delayed child. This information may assist speech-language pathologists in identifying children who are at risk of having developmental coordination disorder and in facilitating referral to occupational therapists or physical therapists for assessment. Abrege Cheryl Missiuna, PhD, OT Les orthophonistes sont eminemment bien places pour aider les familles a effectuer le depistage Reg. (Ont) is an Assistant precoce de troubles de coordination motrice chezles enfants. Tandis que les familles etles medecins Professor and Nancy se preoccupent principalement de retards de l'enfant sur le plan de la communication, la motricite Pollock, MSc, OT Reg fine et glob ale de l'enfant doit elle aussi progresser rapidement pendant les annees prescolaires.
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Language Disorder and Reading Comprehension
    The Reading and Writing Centre your centre for expert advice and professional learning in child and adolescent reading and writing disorders Developmental Language Disorder and reading comprehension Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) is diagnosed when children present with persistent difficulty producing or understanding language for no apparent reason (Bishop, Snowling, Thompson, Greenhalgh, CATALISE consortium 2017). Children with DLD may have difficulty understanding what people say to them and may struggle to express their ideas and feelings. DLD is a spectrum disorder ranging from mild to severe and often leads to significant functional Developmental impacts. On average, two children in every class of 30 will experience DLD severe enough to Language hinder academic progress (Bishop et al 2017). Despite its high prevalence and persistent Disorder functional impacts, DLD is largely undetected and underdiagnosed (Adlof, Scoggins, Brazendale, Babb and Petscher 2017). Research conducted by the University of Sydney identified 16% of Year 8 students as presenting with language disorder (Speech Pathology Australia Submission to Senate Enquiry 2015). Speech language pathologists have primary responsibility for the diagnosis and treatment of DLD. In an education setting this encompasses functional impact on literacy and educational Diagnosis outcomes. Speech Pathology Australia (Clinical guidelines 2016) endorses the critical role of speech language pathologists in prevention, identification, and management of literacy difficulties from infancy to adolescence. Learning to read is a complex and dynamic system with multiple points of vulnerability for children with DLD (Catts, Nielsen, Bridges and Liu 2016; Murphy, Justice, O’Connell, Pentimonti and Kaderavek 2016). Children with persistent language difficulties are highly likely to experience reading comprehension difficulties in components of word reading, Reading listening comprehension or both.
    [Show full text]
  • Characteristics of Children with Learning Disabilities
    National Association of Special Education Teachers NASET LD Report #3 Characteristics of Children with Learning Disabilities Children with learning disabilities are a heterogeneous group. These children are a diverse group of individuals, exhibiting potential difficulties in many different areas. For example, one child with a learning disability may experience significant reading problems, while another may experience no reading problems whatsoever, but has significant difficulties with written expression. Learning disabilities may also be mild, moderate, or severe. Students differ too, in their coping skills. According to Bowe (2005), “some learn to adjust to LD so well that they ‘pass’ as not having a disability, while others struggle throughout their lives to even do ‘simple’ things. Despite these differences, LD always begins in childhood and always is a life-long condition” (p. 71). Over the years, parents, educators, and other professionals have identified a wide variety of characteristics associated with learning disabilities (Gargiulo, 2004). One of the earliest profiles, developed by Clements (1966), includes the following ten frequently cited attributes: • Hyperactivity • Impulsivity • Perceptual-motor impairments • Disorders of memory and thinking • Emotional labiality • Academic difficulties • Coordination problems • Language deficits • Disorders of attention • Equivocal neurological signs Almost 35 years later, Lerner (2000) identified nine learning and behavioral characteristics of individuals with learning disabilities:
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Language Disorder (DLD)
    Information for patients Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) Name: ________________________________ Who to contact and how: ________________________________ Notes: ________________________________ ________________________________ ________________________________ ________________________________ ________________________________ Diana, Princess of Wales Scunthorpe General Hospital Goole & District Hospital Scartho Road Cliff Gardens Woodland Avenue Grimsby Scunthorpe Goole DN33 2BA DN15 7BH DN14 6RX 03033 306999 03033 306999 03033 306999 www.nlg.nhs.uk www.nlg.nhs.uk www.nlg.nhs.uk For more information about our Trust and the services we provide please visit our website: www.nlg.nhs.uk Information for patients Introduction DLD stands for Developmental Language Disorder. Having DLD means your child may have difficulties with understanding and/or using all known languages. DLD can be identified in children from the age of 5 who are likely to have difficulties which may affect their academic progress and persist into adulthood. DLD is believed to affect around 2 children in every classroom. DLD is more common in boys than girls. DLD was previously known as Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Causes There is no known cause of DLD, which can make it hard to explain. DLD is not caused by emotional difficulties or reduced exposure to language. However, a child or young person with DLD may or may not have difficulties in other areas. A child may or may not have medical conditions co-existing with DLD, but these do not cause DLD. Signs that a child or young person may have DLD? • Speech is difficult to understand • Difficulty saying words or sentences • Lower than average literacy skills e.g. reading, writing and spelling • Difficulty understanding how and when to use language appropriately in social situations • Difficulty understanding words or instructions that they hear from others • Difficulty understanding or remembering what has been said to them Remember: DLD looks different in each individual child.
    [Show full text]
  • Speech/Language Impairment Sheet
    West Virginia State Department of Education Office of Special Education * 1-800-558-2696 * http://wvde.state.wv.us/osp/ Fact Speech/Language Impairment Sheet DEFINITION Voice: Speech impairment where the child’s voice has an IDEA defines a speech or language impairment as a abnormal quality of pitch, resonance, or loudness. communication disorder, such as stuttering, impaired articulation, a language impairment, or a voice Persistent voice issues can indicate problems such as impairment that adversely affects a child’s educational vocal cord nodules, which should be addressed. Some performance. students who have structural abnormalities, such as cleft palate or palatal insufficiency, may exhibit a nasal SPEECH DISORDERS sound to their speech. An otolaryngologist must verify Speech Sound Disorders: Speech impairment where the the absence or existence of structural or functional child has difficulty producing sounds. Most children pathology before voice therapy can be provided. make some mistakes as they learn to say new words. A speech sound disorder occurs when mistakes continue Dysphagia: Some children may have difficulty eating past a certain age. Every sound has a different range of (chewing and swallowing food) and drinking without ages when the child should make the sound correctly. aspiration. The speech-language pathologist may be involved in developing a plan, along with other team Speech sound disorders include problems with members, to ensure that the child is provided with safe articulation (making sounds) and phonological nourishment and hydration during school hours. processes (patterns of sounds). COMMUNICATION DISORDERS Sounds can be substituted (saying “tat” for “cat”), left Augmentative and Alternative Communication: the use off (saying “ca” instead of “cat”), added (saying “kwat” of an alternative to speaking as a substitute for speech instead of “cat”) or changed (saying the sound or to supplement speech.
    [Show full text]
  • Preventing Speech and Language Disorders ASHA
    ASHA / Preventing Speech and Language Disorders ASHA / Keep your communication in top form • Tips for preventing – speech sound disorders – stuttering – voice disorders – language disorders ASHA / Speech sound disorders • Signs include – substituting one sound for another (wabbit for rabbit) – leaving sounds out of words (winnow for window) – changing how sounds are made, called distortions ASHA / Speech sound disorder prevention tips • Talk, read, and play with your child every day. – Children learn sounds and words by hearing and seeing them. • Take care of your child’s teeth and mouth. • Have your child’s hearing checked. • Have your child’s speech screened at a local clinic or school. ASHA / Stuttering • Many children may stutter sometimes. This is normal and should go away. • Signs of stuttering include – repeating sounds at the beginning of words (“b-b-bball”) – pausing while talking – stretching sounds out (“sssssssnake” for “snake”) – saying “um” or “uh” a lot while talking ASHA / Stuttering prevention tips • Give your child time to talk. • Try not to interrupt your child while he or she is speaking. • Have your child tested by a speech-language pathologist (SLP) if you are worried. ASHA / Voice disorders Signs include • hoarse, breathy, or nasal-sounding voice • speaking with a pitch that is too high or too low • talking too loudly or too softly • easily losing your voice ASHA / Voice disorders prevention tips • Try not to shout or scream, or to talk in noisy places. • Drink plenty of water. – Water keeps the mouth and throat moist. • Avoid alcohol, caffeine, chemical fumes (such as from cleaning products), and smoking. • See a doctor if you have allergies or sinus or respiratory infections.
    [Show full text]
  • Dyscalculia: an Essential Guide for Parents
    Dyscalculia: An Essential Guide for Parents Stephanie Glen, MAT 1 Copyright 2014 Stephanie Glen MAT All rights reserved. ISBN: 978-1499672893 2 Contents Introduction ................................................................................. 5 Signs of Dyscalculia .................................................................... 13 What Causes Dyscalculia? .......................................................... 21 Diagnosis .................................................................................... 29 Related Disorders ....................................................................... 51 References ................................................................................. 87 3 4 Introduction Mathematics Learning Disorder (commonly called “dyscalculia”) is a brain-based learning disorder that affects arithmetic skills. About 5% to 7% of students have dyscalculia, which is just as common in girls as it is in boys. It is not an intellectual disability. In fact, the definition of dyscalculia according to the American Psychiatric Association is a specific learning disability that affects the normal acquisition of arithmetic skills in spite of normal intelligence. This particular learning disability is also not affected by opportunities to learn, emotional stability, or motivation. In other words, the quality of education your child receives and their 5 desire to learn math has nothing to do with dyscalculia because it is caused by physical issues in the brain. Research suggests that, like other learning disabilities,
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Verbal Dyspraxia
    RCSLT POLICY STATEMENT DEVELOPMENTAL VERBAL DYSPRAXIA Produced by The Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists © 2011 The Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists 2 White Hart Yard London SE1 1NX 020 7378 1200 www.rcslt.org DEVELOPMENTAL VERBAL DYSPRAXIA RCSLT Policy statement Contents EXECUTIVE SUMMARY ............................................................................................... 3 Introduction ............................................................................................................. 4 Process for consensus .............................................................................................5 Characteristics of Developmental Verbal Dyspraxia .....................................................5 Table 1: Characteristic Features of DVD ....................................................................7 Change over time ...................................................................................................8 Terminology issues ................................................................................................. 8 Table 2: Differences in preferred terminology ........................................................... 10 Aetiology ............................................................................................................. 10 Incidence and prevalence of DVD ........................................................................... 11 Co-morbidity .......................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Pragmatic Language
    Pragmatic Language What is pragmatic language? Pragmatic language is the use of appropriate communication in social situations (knowing what to say, how to say it, and when to say it). Pragmatic language involves three major skills: Using language for different purposes such as: • Greeting (Hello. Goodbye. How are you?) • Informing (I am leaving.) • Demanding (Say “Good-bye.” Pick up the toy.) • Stating (I am going to the playground.) • Requesting (Do you want to go along?) Changing language according to the listener or the situation, such as: • Talking to a teacher versus talking to a baby • Speaking in a classroom versus talking in the cafeteria • Talking about family to another family member versus a stranger Following rules for conversation, such as: • Taking turns while talking • Introducing new topics • Staying on topic • Continuing the same topic as the other speaker • Re-wording when misunderstood • Using and understanding nonverbal signals (facial expression, eye contact, etc.) • Respecting personal space What causes a pragmatic language disorder? Although a specific cause of a pragmatic language disorder is not known, the problem is related to dysfunction of the language centers of the brain. Difficulty with pragmatic language can exist on its own, in combination with other language problems, or as part of another diagnosis such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD). What are the characteristics of a pragmatic language disorder? Children with a pragmatic disorder may demonstrate a general language delay. They may have trouble understanding the meaning of what others are saying. They may also have difficulty using language appropriately to get their needs met and to interact with others.
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) Information for Teachers New Terminology for DLD
    Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) Information for teachers New terminology for DLD • DLD stands for Developmental Language Disorder. Having DLD means that a child or young person has severe, persistent difficulties understanding or using spoken language. DLD was previously known as Specific Language Impairment (SLI). • DLD is diagnosed by a Speech and Language Therapist (SLT) only and is used for children over the age of 5 years. DLD is only identified when a child continues to have severe Language and Communication Needs (LCN) following targeted intervention. • There is no known cause of DLD which can make it hard to explain. DLD is not caused by other biomedical conditions (such as ASD, hearing loss), emotional difficulties or limited exposure to language. • LCN is the term used for all children with language difficulties under the age of 5 years and is also used for a school aged child who does not have DLD or a Language Disorder associated with a biomedical condition, but presents with language difficulties. • These terms replace language delay in all age groups. • DLD can co-occur with other difficulties such as Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), dyslexia and speech sound difficulties. • Language Disorder associated with a Biomedical Condition is the term given by an SLT when a CYP has severe persisting language difficulties over and above a known biomedical condition such as hearing loss, physical impairment, ASD, severe learning difficulties or brain injuries. For example Language Disorder associated with ASD. What signs may a CYP with DLD show? • A child may talk less than their peers and find it difficult to express themselves verbally.
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Language Disorder (Dld) Fact Sheet
    DEVELOPMENTAL LANGUAGE DISORDER (DLD) FACT SHEET There are three things you need to know about DLD 1. Developmental Language Disorder is when a child or adult has difficulties talking and/or understanding language. 2. DLD is a hidden disability that affects approximately two children in every classroom, affecting literacy, learning, friendships and emotional well-being. 3. Support from professionals, including speech and language therapists and teachers, can make a real difference. DLD: Diagnostic terminology, frequency, causes • Consensus on terminology: The recommendation for the use of the diagnostic term Developmental Language Disorder has been published (Bishop et al., 2016; 2017), with an account of how consensus was reached. • Frequency: DLD affects approximately two children in every classroom. A recent epidemiological study in the UK, the SCALES study (Norbury et al. 2016), found that 7.5% of children had DLD with no associated biomedical condition. • Causes: DLD tends to run in families. Twin studies indicate strong genetic influence on DLD, but this seems to reflect the combined impact of many genes, rather than a specific mutation (Bishop, 2006). The popular view that DLD is caused by parents who don’t talk to their children has no empirical support. • Neurobiology: There is no evidence of any brain damage in vast majority of cases; there may be subtle differences in size of different brain regions and proportions of grey matter, but this is inconsistent from child to child. As yet we have no ‘biomarker’ for DLD (Leonard et al, 2006) DLD: Associated difficulties • Relationship to other conditions: DLD commonly occurs with ADHD and dyslexia.
    [Show full text]