Chapter 1. the Lawyer and Policymaker As Problem Solver And
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Part One
CHAPTER 1. THE LAWYER AND POLICYMAKER AS PROBLEM SOLVER AND DECISIONMAKER: THE ROLES OF DELIBERATION, INTUITION, AND EXPERTISE
Getting it right and wrong This course is a mixture of prescriptive materials on how to make good decisions and materials describing systematic ways that people make poor decisions. The former are drawn from common sense, statistics and decision science. The latter come from social psychology, the psychology of judgment and decision making (JDM), and the emerging field of behavioral economics. We hope that students’ understanding of decision making errors will result in better decisions for themselves, their clients, constituents, and other stakeholders. In any event, the insights from these disciplines are a useful aspect of professional students’ repertoire of knowledge.
Slide. Layout of the book.
Slide. Terminology. As we mention in the preface, the terms problem-solving, decision making, and judgment have different, though often overlapping, meanings. You might think of them as three overlapping circles in a Venn diagram. We often use “decision making” as a shorthand for all three.
Problem solving: We describe three sorts of problems. Two are deviations from the way things should be: something has already gone wrong or might go wrong in the future if we don’t take preventative measures. Here, problem-solving requires understanding what caused or will cause the undesirable situation and determining alternative solutions in terms of the interests and objectives at stake. A third sort of problem solving involves moving the world in the direction you desire even if nothing has gone or will go wrong.
2 Slides. Problem Space, Navigating the Problem Space
Decision making. If the core of problem solving is sizing up the situation in terms of the interests and objectives at stake and mapping out possible solutions, then decision making involves identifying the tradeoffs among solutions and choosing the best alternative. Most important decisions implicate several values, interests, or attributes (e.g., cost, quality, and safety for many consumer decisions). Sometimes one alternative will dominate others in all respects, but usually the decisionmaker must make tradeoffs. (More of this in Chapter 4 and 12.)
In its JDM sense, judgment involves empirical perception and analysis; it is “sizing up” the external physical or social world—as distinguished from the internal interests or psychology of the decision maker. In its broader sense, it is wisdom or discernment involving internal as well as external matters.
PS TM Part One Page 1 5/7/10 Chapter 1 introduces two different methods of problem solving/decision making: (1) deliberative and (2) intuitive or schematic. The book approaches these differently.
Deliberative decision making is an analytic discipline, the foundations of which can be learned through repetitive exercises. We accompany Part I of the book with a number of exercises of this sort.
By contrast, our approach to intuitive or schematic decision making is descriptive—descriptive of the underlying psychological processes and of how one develops expertise. For the most part, intuitive decision making must be learned on the job, or perhaps through simulated clinical exercises—in either case, through reflective practice. Perhaps becoming better at deliberative decision making can improve intuitive decision making as well. In Chapter 20, we quote Constantin Stanislavski's description of an actor's preparation:
One cannot always create subconsciously and with inspiration. No such genius exists in the world. Therefore our art teaches us first of all to create consciously and rightly, because that will best prepare the way for the blossoming of the subconscious, which is inspiration. The more you have of conscious creative moments in your role, the more chance you will have of a flow of inspiration.
Slide. Deliberative Decision Making. Our particular version of the multiple steps of deliberative decision making is pretty generic. Students are strongly tempted (as are we all) to skip steps and head toward what they see as an obvious solution. The Terra Nueva, Newport Records, Trampoline, Shark Attack, and other problems1 provide opportunities to walk through most of the steps and show the potential dangers of skipping some.
2 Slides. Evers v. Newport Records. In Newport Records, Anna defines the problem as how to get summary judgment. Although it leads to some repetition of material in the book, we ask students in class to describe Evers’ plausible interests:
Case resolved; closure Favorable judgment Money now (for new venture) avoid litigation costs
We ask students to generate a range of plausible solutions or alternative courses of action:
do nothing (get the money for his new venture elsewhere) seek summary judgment Go to trial make a settlement offer
1 Color-coded problems are included in the Problems section of the web site. Those that are not color-coded appear in the text itself.
PS TM Part One Page 2 5/7/10 And we ask them to predict the consequences of the courses of action and assess their impact on the relevant interests or objectives. (Chapter 15 uses Newport Records’ litigate/settle decision to introduce decision trees.)
Although Newport Records doesn’t call for diagnosing causes and dealing with major uncertainties, Christine Lamm’s analysis of Terra Nueva in Chapter 2 does that explicitly.
Trampoline problem The trampoline problem is a good way to get students to go through the steps of the deliberative process. It creates expectations that this is a “legal” problem, but most of the analysis and plausible solutions don’t involve the law at all. Here and for many of the other problems in this book you can, for example:
Ask students to send responses by email Ask students to work on the problem in class, individually or in groups In either case, then discuss the problem in the full class
Here, from a recent class, is a student-generated list of the client’s possible interests.
Reduce risk of liability Keep the baby sitter Ensure all children’s safety Help her children make friends Help compensate for her children’s shyness Maintain a good reputation with other parents Maintain children’s custody Understand good practices for using trampoline
Here is a student-generated list of uncertainties to be resolved:
What are the risks? Was the baby sitter following the instructions/good practices? What were the conditions when the neighboring child was injured? Who is liable for the injury, and under what circumstances? Why can’t the kids make friends? What are the benefits of the trampoline in terms the child’s interests, as compared to other activities? How can safety be improved? What are other peoples’ views (besides Ms. Trisolini’s) of the situation?
Only after students have gone through these two steps, ask them to suggest possible alternative solutions. (If you make this part of the initial exercise, quite a few students may define the problem and interests in terms of the solution.)
PS TM Part One Page 3 5/7/10 As a segue into intuitive decisionmaking, ask: Why can’t all DM be deliberative? All problem solving begins with series of hunches or hypotheses (intuitive). If we had limitless cognitive power and time, we could deal with all problems deliberatively. But bounded rationality—in effect cognitive transaction costs—require satisficing (mostly intuitive).
Intuitive decision making What are schemas? How do they work?
Imagine perceiving the world without any knowledge structures or schemas. I would look around the room and not be able to identify discrete objects—people, chairs, tables. Just a lot of incomprehensible data.
In fact, we place every perception into a pre-existing knowledge structure or schema in our memory. We’re right 99% of the time. Occasionally we’re surprised or disappointed: Walking into the store, we thought we saw a person but it was a manikin. Sometimes we retain a belief in the schematic perception even in the face of evidence to the contrary. Cf. optical illusions.
Schemas are also important for understanding complex social/organizational phenomena.
Developing competence/expertise in a field requires learning sound schemas and pattern recognition.
Examples of using schemas in everyday life. Solving crossword and other puzzles Listening to a piece of music and identifying the composer or singer Reading a crumpled or torn piece of paper where not all the words are legible Skimming a book or article
Examples from book. Christine Lamm’s dead computer. Why does she assume it’s the plug? How did she learn this? Anna (new lawyer): hearsay, summary judgment schemas Klein: firefighters Neonatal nurses Soldier suspecting IED
Q: examples from your experience?
PS TM Part One Page 4 5/7/10 Scripts. How to behave in a courtroom, in a classroom, subway, restaurant. Navigating a crowd in a busy airport.
The mix of intuitive and deliberative
Slide. Convergent/divergent. The basic point is that problem solving always starts with intuitions about the nature of the problem and solutions. The question is how and when one tests these intuitions deliberatively
Divergent: framing, identifying objectives Both divergent and convergent: identifying causes Convergent: evaluating and selecting alternatives
Some of your students may have read Malcolm Gladwell’s Blink, and drawn the conclusion that intuitive decision making usually trumps deliberation. But there are many examples in Blink where this is not the case. And when it is, it’s usually decision making by experts, who have acquired expertise through lots of deliberation as well as feedback from practice. (More about this later.)
Affect often plays a role in schematic processing: when you see a lion or snake you run, and in many social situations you have a gut feeling, long before there is any conscious cognition. Negative affect narrows range of deliberative DM decision making by taking some choices off the table.
This is a good place to discuss expertise. Can you recall developing competence/expertise in something, and what it felt like to be competent/expert? What does it mean to be an expert? To have know-how in a field? How does an expert approach differently from someone without know-how?
Knows more about the substantive (procedural) subject Knows geography and extent of domain In some cases, physical skills (sports, music, surgery) Organization of knowledge in terms of schemas o Recognition of patterns (Darlene the nurse) “flow”
Slide. What’s unusual about this hand? The downsides of schematic processing. After introducing schematic processing (as foundational to intuitive decision making), we show students the Bruner and Postman hand of cards. Many students don’t see what’s wrong because of their expectations.
Slide. Two systems. This is where we introduce the two-systems view of decision making that pervades the book and which is one of the cornerstones of JDM research. Here, or later, one can say a few words about the origins and intellectual history of JDM research. While they didn’t start on a blank slate, Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman are the founders. Tversky
PS TM Part One Page 5 5/7/10 died in 1996. Kahneman received the Nobel Prize in Economics , in 2002, essentially for both of their work. The new field of behavioral economics grows out of the JDM research. (Chapters 14-18 deal with aspects of behavioral economics.) Although the two-systems approach precedes fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging), the imaging work indicates that affective responses (System 1) and cognitive work (System 2) may take place in different parts of the brain.
You can also introduce the idea of heuristics, though the book doesn’t focus on this until Part Two.
Slide. Bat and ball. Slide. Heuristics.
We end by tying the discussion of expertise with the students’ own aspirations. Where /how did Anna, Luis, and Christina learn what they know? What jobs do you imagine having, and what skills and knowledge will you need? What do you hope to get from professional school and from this course?
PS TM Part One Page 6 5/7/10 CHAPTER 2. FRAMING PROBLEMS AND IDENTIFYING OBJECTIVES AND IDENTIFYING PROBLEM CAUSES
The main point about the decision context is that it limits the solutions that a particular decisionmaker can pursue.
Some students find the trampoline problem troubling because they believe that the lawyer-client decision context does not encompass the personal or psychological counseling that may be the best approach to Ms. Trisolini’s problem. Like Keeney’s public utility charged with reducing radiation danger, Christine Lamm has limited authority to deal with the Terra Nueva problem: she can engage in rule- making, but she does not have the power to require tenants to evacuate the premises.
Problem definitions and frames. The trampoline case comes packaged as a legal problem, but (notwithstanding the comments about decision contexts) a good lawyer would not be limited by legal frameworks. A savvy problem solver challenges each proposed objective with a “why” until she runs out of “becauses.” But you can’t do this relentlessly for all problems, because probing further into the nature of the problem in order to arrive at the right solution can be time consuming and costly. There is a tension between going deeper and deeper about objectives and establishing a workable scope for the problem.
Symptoms vs. problems. It is important to ask: what triggered this problem? Why am I even considering it? But the trigger may be a symptom rather than the core of the problem itself.
Frames are inevitable. A frame is an instantiation of a particular schema. It comes with a host of expectations and constraints for interpreting and analyzing the world.
An initiative by the Hewlett Foundation to “reduce the need for abortion” provides an interesting example of framing designed (with reasonable success) to appeal to people on both sides of the abortion debate. The idea is to help young adults reduce unintended pregnancies by using contraceptives. Most social conservatives, including evangelicals, do not object to contraception, and many regard providing contraception for people engaged in sex outside of marriage as a lesser evil than abortion. No one on the pro-choice side regards abortion as a desirable method of birth control. Note that the characterization of a position as “pro-choice” or “pro-life” itself has a parallel framing effect. In designing this initiative, the Foundation learned that evangelicals and other social conservatives had very different reactions to “unwanted,” “unplanned,” and “unintended” pregnancies.
An important aspect of persuading others is to get them to accept your frames. In any event, be wary when someone else frames a problem—especially as a limited choice.
Part of expertise is having a perspective that does not inevitably accept a client’s or stakeholder’s framing of a problem. But experts have their own (narrow) frames, which may prevent them from registering and incorporating aspects of the story that are salient from another's perspective.
PS TM Part One Page 7 5/7/10 Values, interests, and objectives get at essentially the same thing, but in descending levels of generality.
Slide. Stakeholder analysis consists of looking at problem from different people’s points of view. This isn’t necessarily altruistic, but rather a way of understanding all of one’s own or client’s or constituents’ interests.
The book walks through a stakeholder analysis from Serrano’s point of view. You might ask students in class to do the same from Lamm’s perspective.
Causal analysis. This is a completely different subject from objectives. In principle, it might have a chapter of its own, but we put it here to avoid overly short chapters.
Cause is about counterfactuals: the putative cause made something “go wrong” that wouldn’t have gone wrong absent the occurrence of the cause. The Kepner-Tregoe method (which, like our list of the steps of deliberative decision making, is just one of a number of systematizations of causal analysis) focuses on distinctions between what happened and did not happen. We teach causal analysis through Slide. Christine Lamm’s discussion of the causes of the illnesses at Terra Nueva, and Slide. Perrin Stryker’s classic, Can You Analyze this Problem?, which can be ordered and downloaded online from the Harvard Business School. We don’t give the students the accompanying How to Analyze this Problem, which you can treat as a teacher’s guide. The case study misleads most students to focus only on the personnel issue, but it turns out that the problem can be analyzed and “solved” in technical terms. In outline:2
1. Defining the problem: Immediate: deviation from desired norm reject-rate of burred panels on lines 1, 2 & 4 Longer term: personnel-labor management problem with Farrell and Valenti and upcoming contract negotiations
2. Specifying the problem’s “what,” “where,” “when,” and “extent” Relevance of different extents? Could be random variation. If any correlation with problem, though, we would expect Farrell’s line #2 to have higher > lower rejects because that’s Farrell’s line
3. Spotting distinctions and identifying changes responsible for the distinction Lines 1,2,4 but not 3 Line 3: one stack, Cheetah Stacks of old blanks used up before new Zenith blank o Explains why line 4 took longest
2 The following makes sense only if you have read the problem.
PS TM Part One Page 8 5/7/10 o Line 3 shortest, but Cheetah
The main hint that the students are given is buried in this colloquy:
PETERS: It’s hard to say what might be causing it. We’ve been checking the sheets from Zenith Metals we started using this morning, and they looked perfect going through the blanker. Besides, it’s only on lines #1 and #2 that we’re getting burrs, so maybe we’ve got trouble with those presses. …
COGGIN: What about that man who got hurt last night on overtime while unloading those sheets? PATELLA: He’s been on the job for a couple of months, but he tells me he wasn’t familiar with the method of blocking that Zenith Metals uses. He’s not hurt bad, but he’ll get workmen’s compensation OK.
Of course, there may remain underlying personnel problems:in any event, students who were originally invested in this analysis tend to believe so. We ask students to submit a brief analysis by email, then discuss the problem in small groups in class, giving them a blank form to fill out, and then have a class-wide discussion.
Strategic planning Terra Nueva and the Stryker case study involve gone-wrong problems. Strategic planning, whether done by a state or local government, or a nonprofit organization, is paradigmatic of forward-looking problem solving. The chapter ends with an example of strategic planning for the Hewlett Foundation’s Environment program’s work in the American West.
This may be challenging to teach for someone not familiar with philanthropy. You can get a better sense of the underlying approach from Doing good today and better tomorrow: A roadmap to high impact philanthropy through outcome-focused grantmaking, http://www.hewlett.org/news/doing-good-today-and-better-tomorrow.
As background, these are the basic tenets of the sort of “strategic,” or “outcome-focused,” philanthropy practiced by the Hewlett Foundation. They are different from, but essentially congruent with, the deliberative decision making steps.
1. Clear goals 2. Targets for those goals 3. A logical strategy for how one's efforts can help achieve those goals, based on … 4. A sound analysis of the problem and an evidence-based theory of change 5. Good information about individuals and entities that can address the problem (i.e. thorough due diligence); 6. Sufficient resources, whether individual or aggregated with others’, applied to achieving the goal, over a sufficient period of time
PS TM Part One Page 9 5/7/10 7. Measuring progress and results and making changes accordingly 8. An “expected-return attitude” that takes appropriate account of benefits and risks.
Although we include the expected return analysis here to make the example complete, it anticipates the formal discussion of expected utility theory and cost benefit analysis in Chapter 12.
PS TM Part One Page 10 5/7/10 CHAPTER 3. GENERATING ALTERNATIVES: CREATIVITY IN LEGAL AND POLICY PROBLEM SOLVING
As our discussion of expertise in Chapters 1 and 2 describes, many of the benefits of expertise (e.g., expedited problem recognition and framing, enhanced ability to distinguish diagnostic or predictive information from non-diagnostic or non-predictive information, and rapid identification of promising alternative solution strategies) can also blind experts to new ways of approaching problems. Expertise can, for this and other reasons, inhibit innovation. This Chapter is designed to illustrate the important role that “out-of-the-box” thinking can play in problem solving and decision making, and to help loosen students up a bit, so that they can freely experiment with and experience the benefits of divergent thinking and the obstacles that constrain it.
In exploring creativity, Chapter 3 focuses on topics and perspectives grounded in theoretical and empirical research. The Chapter is therefore short on some of the most “brain- catching” creativity-enhancement exercises and tools one finds in the popular and on websites and blogs. However, we recognize that teaching the material on creativity will work better if students can be engaged, challenged, and generally loosened up by vivid illustrations of some of the Chapter’s core ideas. So, in this section, we provide some of these exercises, tools, and illustrations, and we point you to places where you can find more.
A. On “Pickers,” “Choosers” and the Paradoxes of Schematized Thinking
Here are some possible illustrations of the benefits and shortcomings of schematized thinking, discussed at pp. 61- 63 and alluded to throughout the chapter.
Benefits of Expertise: Finding Patterns in Perceptual Chaos
In Conceptual Blockbusting (pp. 21-22), Stanford engineering professor, James L. Adams uses the following exercise to illustrate the role that experience plays in our ability to find patterns in the midst of perceptual chaos.
Have students look at this picture, and ask them what they see (warn those who have done this before not to reveal what they know).
PS TM Part One Page 11 5/7/10 Now show students this second image, from which they will readily see the cow they could not see in the first:
After seeing the whole pattern, students will find it virtually impossible not to see the cow head in the first image.
This exercise illustrates an important point: the kind of expertise we gain through exposure and experience enables us to recognize patterns in the midst of perceptual chaos. Lawyers, for example, can hear a complex, sometimes emotionally overwhelming story about harmful events and from that story “hear” one or more legally cognizable claims. In the public policy realm, using perceptual tools unavailable to untrained laypersons, epidemiologists, demographers, and civil engineers can “see” patterns that provide important information to public policy makers about the existence, extent, nature, causes or effects of important phenomena.
Drawbacks of Expertise: Path Dependence and Norm Following
The flipside of these advantages, though, are embodied in what some sociologists call path dependence: the habit of seeing one problem or solution so automatically that you can no longer see others. These habits can assume normative dimensions. Becoming part of an expert community entails learning and following the explicit and implicit norms that structure the field. Path dependence and norm following, along with other barriers to innovation, are covered in more depth at the end of Chapter 3, but it is useful at the outset to confront students with their own vulnerability to them in the generation of alternative problem solutions. The Nine Dot Problem (p. 67) provides an effective starting point:
Here is the “classic” solution to the Nine Dot Problem. Start drawing the line at 0, return to the origin at 3, and end at 4.
PS TM Part One Page 12 5/7/10 Of course, even an “out-of-the-box” solution like this can become habituated and normative, so you might ask students to generate others, lest they bask overlong in the self- satisfied glow of Gnostic, Nine-Dot Initiation. Here are some the more wicked solutions to the Nine Dot Problem found in Jim Adams’ collection (Conceptual Blockbusting, pp. 30, 31):
PS TM Part One Page 13 5/7/10 PS TM Part One Page 14 5/7/10 Different Kinds of Options: Some Illustrative Exercises
At pages 61-66, Chapter 3 describes the potential benefits of information-buying and time-buying alternatives. It would be easy for students to fly by this material and miss its significance, so it may be beneficial to pause and emphazise it to increase its salience and memorability.
In Lateral Thinking: Creativity Step by Step, Edward deBono explains how the generation of alternative solutions is affected by the sequence in which information arrives during a multi-stage problem solving process. He illustrates this phenomenon with an exercise in which participants are presented with pieces of thin plastic or cardboard, and are directed to arrange them, as they are presented, into a shape that would be easy to describe. In the first five steps in the sequence, the shapes are easily arranged into a series of expanding rectangles. However, the last piece presented, a parallelogram, can not be integrated into the shape. The following graphic, found in Lateral Thinking at p. 32, illustrates the problem:
PS TM Part One Page 15 5/7/10 If, however, problem solvers know about the parellelogram (piece #6) at the beginning of the problem solving sequence, the problem becomes easier to solve, as the following graphic (Lateral Thinking, p. 34) illustrates:
PS TM Part One Page 16 5/7/10 This exercise may be useful to illustrate the point that commiting to a course of action before having as much information as one can gather may prove disadvantageous over time.
PS TM Part One Page 17 5/7/10 Legal Professional Ambivalence Toward Creativity
At pages 67-68, and elsewere in Chapter 3, we highlight the ambivalence toward creativity in the legal profession. Here are some additional sources teachers might draw on to illustrate or amplify upon this ambivalence:
. Tresa Baldas, Considering the Alternatives: With Prison Costs Skyrocketing, Creative Sentencing Takes Off, NATIONAL LAW JOURNAL, News, p. 18, Column 1 (November 5, 2004)(describing innovative ideas in sentencing and the many rhetorical and constitutional challenges to them); . ADA Solutions Require ‘Creativity:’ Inconvenience Held Not Enough to Reject Proposal by Disabled, NATIONAL LAW JOURNAL, Business, p. A18, Column 1 (July 23, 2001)(notice the scare quotes in the title); . Rees W. Morrison, Law Departments and Firms Should Nurture Creativity, THE TEXAS LAWYER, Section: In-House Texas (February 5, 2007)(The article’s opening sentence is, “What’s so scary about creativity?”) . Rees W. Morrison, The Rewards of Creativity: Useful Creativity Increases Productivity and Effectiveness, NEW JERSEY LAW JOURNAL, Law Office and Trust Account Management (March 5, 2007)(stating in its first paragraph, “Neither law departments nor the law firms they retain appear to be comfortable with creativity. They talk about the value of doing things differently -- and better -- but the truth is that creativity is unusual and mistrusted. Both sides are ambivalent: law departments favor step-by-step, well- understood services, even as their lawyers claim that they welcome breakthrough thinking; law firms boast of their cutting-edge abilities but find little actual reward in innovation.”)
To highlight the tensions presented by innovation’s polarized upside and downside risk profiles, public policy professors might want to consider using as supplemental reading Maximillian Martin, Balancing Creativity and Control, an online pamplet available at http://ssrn.com/abstract=1322402.
Case Studies on Creativity and Legal Innovation -- Potential Resources
At pages 71-72, and again at pp. 78-79, Chapter 3 describes various innovations in legal theory, doctrine, and policy that could reasonably be characterized as creative. Additional examples can be found in law review article and legal periodicals. For instructors who want to steer students in this direction, here are some additional sources to explore:
. Robert F. Blomquist, Thinking About Law and Creativity: On the 100 Most Creative Moments in American Law (Draft available at http://ssrn.com/abstract=1133631). . Frank Newton, Creativity Leads to a New Way to Fund Pro Bono, TEXAS LAWYER, April 29, 2002. . Maria Vogel-Short, N.Y. Chief Judge: Creativity Key to Luring More to Pro Bono Work,
PS TM Part One Page 18 5/7/10 New Jersey Lawyer: The Weekly Newspaper, (November 11, 2002).
Constraints on Creativity, And How to Overcome Them
Much of Chapter 3 reviews the literature on constraints on creativity. On of the best ways to help students understand how cognitive, social, practical, and personal factors can exercise these constraining effects is to give them a chance to experience the constraints themselves in the classroom context. There are numerous popular books and websites, many of the latter free, that provide creativity exercises instructors might find valuable for these purposes. Although we have way to know, empirically, whether exercises like these actually improve divergent thinking in law or public policy domains, they are useful for many purposes, particularly for:
. Showing students how much more comfortable they feel critiquing ideas than generating, articulating, or building on them; . How powerfully the professional norms they have already assimilated during school constrain the types of alternatives they can imagine, let alone articulate; . What their own strengths and weaknesses are in relation to different types of frame- breaking cognitive activities (visual, analogical, etc…); and . How information overload and the high velocity pace at which we live and work powerfully constrains how much time we are able or willing to spend idle, a factor that has been empirically demonstrated to constrain innovation.
There are numerous popular books and websites that contain ideas for what we might call “creativity warm-ups.” Here are some books that contain useful exercises:
. James Adams, CONCEPTUAL BLOCKBUSTING (4th Ed. 2001) . James Adams, THE CARE AND FEEDING OF IDEAS: A GUIDE TO ENCOURAGING CREATIVITY (1986); . Robert Bookstrom, DEVELOPING CREATIVE AND CRITICAL THINKING: AN INTEGRATED APPROACH (1992) . Edward deBono, CREATIVITY STEP BY STEP (1973); . Paul Sloane, THE LEADER’S GUIDE TO LATERAL THINKING SKILLS (2nd Ed. 2006); and . Anthony Weston, CREATIVITY FOR CRITICAL THINKERS (2007).
Scores of websites and blogs contain creativity exercises of various types, along with information about the uses of and procedures for brainstorming sessions. Many of these are thinly veiled advertising tools or are inappropriate for professional students. However, we have found the following two particularly useful as a source of ideas and in-class exercises, including brainstorming sessions:
. http://blog.creativethink.com (This is a website run by Roger Van Oech, author of such popular creativity books as A WHACK ON THE SIDE OF THE HEAD. It features ideas and instructions for bisociation exercises, exercises using humor to generate creative alternatives, and brainstorming sessions.)
PS TM Part One Page 19 5/7/10 . http://www.glencoe.com/sec/busadmin/entre/teacher/creative/stimulate/index.htm (This website, run by McGraw Hill, is specifically designed for educational professionals teaching in entrepreneurship-related programs)
Scenario planning: Creativity in long-term
With its eye on long-term planning, the material on scenario planning parallels the section on strategic planning in Chapter 2. Like other creativity-enhancing techniques, scenario planning is intended to liberate decisionmakers from well-worn cognitive ruts down which much strategic planning proceeds. Scenario planning has been used by multinational corporations and by governments in thinking about national security. The book gives examples of scenario planning by nonprofit organizations.
PS TM Part One Page 20 5/7/10 CHAPTER 4 CHOOSING AMONG ALTERNATIVES
Slide. This materials in this chapter fit logically with much of Chapter 12 (Choices, Consequences, and Tradeoffs), but it rounds out Part One’s once-over-lightly introduction to decision making.
It is useful to remind students about deliberative and (slide) non deliberative approaches to decision making, and (slide) compensatory and noncompensatory processes. You might ask them to provide examples of the “spectrum of decision making processes,” discussing when these approaches are useful, and their benefits and downsides.
Slide. The subjective linear model is a very simple value-based (as distinguished from reason- based) multi-attribute decision model, useful for decisions that have many alternatives, many attributes (i.e., criteria or goals), and that present somewhat difficult tradeoffs.
In teaching the wastewater treatment plant siting problem, we walk students through the charts, and also ask with respect to the attributes involved in the decision:
Whose values does Christine take into account? Slide. How did the staff develop their own objectives? Slide. Where do the community objectives come from, and what should Christine do with heterogeneous interests? What if Christine disagrees with their values? What if she thinks a community is wrong about an objective fact (e.g., whether the plant will smell)? Are you satisfied that she has identified the right attributes for the decision? How could she have done better?
Slide. Map of Edenville Slide. Christine’s Criteria Slide. Map of Edenville Slide. Site Analysis Slide. Qualitative Assessments Slide. Quantitative Assessments Slide. Site Comparisons Slide. Possible Weightings Slide. Values X Weightings Slide. Weighted Site Comparisons
PS TM Part One Page 21 5/7/10 Problem: Applying the subjective linear model to your own problem. We accompany the reading by asking students to bring do the following exercise to class and discuss it in class in small groups.
Chapter 4 describes an approach to making a decision with tradeoffs—a compensatory model—using the example of siting a solid waste disposal plant. Please apply this approach to an example from personal experience (e.g., choosing what class to take, what summer job to take, what apartment to rent, where to take a vacation). It would be ideal, but not necessary, if you used a decision you are actually facing – but if an actual decision does not come to mind, or if you don’t feel comfortable describing an actual problem, feel free to use a hypothetical case. The decision ought to involve three or more alternatives and three or more attributes. Show how you would analyze the problem, step by step, to arrive at a decision Please actually work through the decision process rather than just describe how you might do it.
Where have students actually used (an approximation of) this method? When might it be useful? When might it be useful for policy decisions?
PS TM Part One Page 22 5/7/10