Gaze Following and Attention to Objects in Infants at Familial Risk for ASD

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Gaze Following and Attention to Objects in Infants at Familial Risk for ASD King’s Research Portal DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01799 Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to publication record in King's Research Portal Citation for published version (APA): Parsons, J. P., Bedford, R., Jones, E. J. H., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., & Gliga, T. (2019). Gaze following and attention to objects in infants at familial risk for ASD. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(JULY), [1799]. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01799 Citing this paper Please note that where the full-text provided on King's Research Portal is the Author Accepted Manuscript or Post-Print version this may differ from the final Published version. If citing, it is advised that you check and use the publisher's definitive version for pagination, volume/issue, and date of publication details. And where the final published version is provided on the Research Portal, if citing you are again advised to check the publisher's website for any subsequent corrections. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the Research Portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognize and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. •Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the Research Portal for the purpose of private study or research. •You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain •You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the Research Portal Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 02. Oct. 2021 fpsyg-10-01799 August 17, 2019 Time: 15:44 # 1 ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 20 August 2019 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01799 Gaze Following and Attention to Objects in Infants at Familial Risk for ASD Janet P. Parsons1, Rachael Bedford2, Emily J. H. Jones1*, Tony Charman3 Mark H. Johnson1,4 and Teodora Gliga1,5* on behalf of the BASIS Team 1 Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom, 2 Biostatistics Department, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom, 3 Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom, 4 Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom, 5 Department of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom Reduced gaze following has been associated previously with lower language scores in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Here, we use eye-tracking in a controlled experimental setting to investigate whether gaze following and attention distribution Edited by: during a word learning task associate with later developmental and clinical outcomes in Jo Van Herwegen, a population of infants at familial risk for ASD. Fifteen-month-old infants (n = 124; n = 101 Kingston University, United Kingdom with familial risk) watched an actress repeatedly gaze toward and label one of two Reviewed by: Carmel Houston-Price, objects present in front of her. We show that infants who later developed ASD followed University of Reading, gaze as frequently as typically developing peers but spent less time engaged with United Kingdom Rechele Brooks, either object. Moreover, more time spent on faces and less on objects was associated University of Washington, with lower concurrent or later verbal abilities, but not with later symptom severity. United States No outcome group showed evidence for word learning. Thus, atypical distribution of *Correspondence: attention rather than poor gaze following is a limiting factor for language development in Emily J. H. Jones [email protected] infants at familial risk for ASD. Teodora Gliga [email protected]; Keywords: gaze following, infants, familial risk, ASD, eye-tracking [email protected] Specialty section: INTRODUCTION This article was submitted to Developmental Psychology, Typically developing infants are sensitive to others’ gaze from birth (Batki et al., 2000; Farroni a section of the journal et al., 2002). Over the first year they follow gaze first reflexively (Hood et al., 1998; Farroni et al., Frontiers in Psychology 2000, 2004) and then learn its referential function (Woodward, 2003; Csibra and Volein, 2008; Received: 12 February 2019 Senju et al., 2008). Being able to follow someone’s gaze, and jointly attend to objects, is thought Accepted: 19 July 2019 to provide a key mechanism by which infants acquire a vocabulary (Baldwin, 1991, 1993; Schafer Published: 20 August 2019 and Plunkett, 1998; Houston-Price et al., 2006) and many studies have associated joint attention Citation: ability with later vocabulary growth (Carpenter et al., 1998; Morales et al., 1998; Charman, 2003; Parsons JP, Bedford R, Brooks and Meltzoff, 2008). Jones EJH, Charman T, Johnson MH Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often have poor joint attention, evidenced by and Gliga T (2019) Gaze Following and Attention to Objects in Infants reduced gaze following in naturalistic situations (e.g., Dawson et al., 2004), and this has been at Familial Risk for ASD. highlighted as one of the most reliable and consistent indicators of ASD during childhood (e.g., Front. Psychol. 10:1799. Baron-Cohen et al., 1996; Charman, 2003). Given that the rate of learning difficulty is often doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01799 high in children with ASD (∼55%; Charman et al., 2011) and there is frequent language delay Frontiers in Psychology| www.frontiersin.org 1 August 2019| Volume 10| Article 1799 fpsyg-10-01799 August 17, 2019 Time: 15:44 # 2 Parsons et al. Gaze Following in Infants At-Risk for ASD (e.g., Charman et al., 2003), studies have suggested that poor in ASD particularly when the face addresses the child (Shic et al., language in ASD may be explained, in part, by difficulties 2014) or when it establishes mutual gaze (Nyström et al., 2017). with engaging in joint attention (e.g., Mundy et al., 1994; One study has directly addressed the question of whether directed Pickard and Ingersoll, 2015). For example, in their study of communication is particularly problematic (Vernetti et al., 2018). children between 22 to 93 months of age, Pickard and Ingersoll In this study, toddlers could choose between animating (by used the Early Social Communication Scales, a play-based looking at them) either a video of a person that established eye structured assessment that captures both the child’s initiating contact and directly addressed them, or a video of a spinning and responding to joint attention, to show that failing to follow mechanical toy. There was no difference between those with someone’s gaze or pointing to an object were best predictors of a later diagnosis of ASD and those without, with all groups concurrent language. In addition, an intervention targeting joint choosing to animate and engage longer with the face rather than attention in children with ASD yielded better expressive language the toy. Those studies which have analyzed dwell time to the face outcomes when compared to an intervention increasing symbolic during gaze following have also failed to find group differences play (Gulsrud et al., 2014). (Chawarska et al., 2012; Billeci et al., 2016; Vivanti et al., 2017), There are several reasons why children with ASD may struggle suggesting that poor gaze following in ASD may not be due to to use joint attention for learning language. Firstly, they may not insufficient engagement with faces. correctly or consistently follow someone’s gaze to the object they During infancy and early toddlerhood, eye-tracking studies are labeling. This could be because they do not spend enough time are consistent in suggesting that the ability to shift one’s gaze looking at faces to notice or process the gaze shifts. Alternatively, to follow someone else’s gaze direction to an object (henceforth despite looking at faces and eyes, they still may not shift their referent) rather than an equally salient distractor, is intact in gaze in the same direction as the person communicating with toddlers with ASD or infants with later ASD, with differences them. It could also be that, despite correctly following gaze, appearing to emerge later in development (see Table 1.2). There they do not spend enough time on the gazed at object to learn is, however, a more mixed picture when studies analyzed the about it. Looking less toward the gazed at object may also dwell time on objects, with most studies finding decreased reflect poor understanding of the referential nature of gaze. That looking toward the gazed at objects, but a few finding no is, word learning could fail not because there was insufficient differences (see Table 1.3). Some of the inconsistency in findings time dedicated to encoding object properties, but because unlike may reflect differences in the way engagement with objects was typically-developing children (Gliga and Csibra, 2009), children measured. Researchers either directly compared time spent on with ASD may have a reduced appreciation of the referential link referent versus distractor or contrasted time spent on the referent between the uttered word and the gazed at object. to time spent on all areas of interest (AOI), including the face Recently, eye-tracking studies have allowed a detailed or the background. While the former measure directly assesses quantification of attention distribution during joint attention an understanding of which object is the referent of the gaze, the episodes, thus making it possible to reveal the different sources of latter measure also captures infants’ engagement with irrelevant atypicality mentioned above. Eye-tracking studies investigating aspects of the scene or differences in looking toward the face.
Recommended publications
  • Eye Gaze and Joint Attention Fundamental Skills for Successful Interactions in Home and School Environments
    VISUAL LANGUAGE & VISUAL LEARNING NSF supported Science of Learning Center on Visual Language and Visual RESEARCH BRIEF: Learning, SBE-1041725. EYE GAZE AND JOINT ATTENTION FUNDAMENTAL SKILLS FOR SUCCESSFUL INTERACTIONS IN HOME AND SCHOOL ENVIRONMENTS JUNE 2012 Key Findings on Eye Gaze and Joint Attention: LEARNING FROM • Deaf children perceive both language and non-linguistic information visually. RESEARCH • Eye gaze and joint attention between child and caregiver function together as a foundation for the development of communicative competence. # 5 • Deaf children with exposure to sign language from birth develop the ability to shift their eye gaze between objects and people in a frequent and meaningful way from an early age. • Language input that occurs during episodes of joint attention is linked to enhanced vocabulary, language, and literacy development in both deaf and hearing children. Written by: • Deaf children who have developed the pragmatic ability to manage, monitor, Amy M. Lieberman and self-regulate their own visual attention are more prepared for the complex Ph.D. visual exchanges that take place in the classroom. 1 of 6 NSF SCIENCE OF LEARNING CENTER ON VISUAL LANGUAGE AND VISUAL LEARNING RESEARCH BRIEF NO. 5: EYE GAZE AND JOINT ATTENTION When babies are exposed to language from birth, including bonding and attachment, as well as early they learn language spontaneously and naturally understanding of turn-taking and meaningful through their interactions with parents, family communication.1 Beginning around six months, as members, peers, and many others. But in addition infants become more mobile and start to explore to learning the language itself, children must also the world around them, the focus of their attention learn how to use the language in meaningful ways shifts to the objects in their environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Perspective-Taking and Its Foundation in Joint Attention
    Perspective-Taking and its Foundation in Joint Attention University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship Online Perception, Causation, and Objectivity Johannes Roessler, Hemdat Lerman, and Naomi Eilan Print publication date: 2011 Print ISBN-13: 9780199692040 Published to Oxford Scholarship Online: September 2011 DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199692040.001.0001 Perspective-Taking and its Foundation in Joint Attention Henrike Moll Andy Meltzoff DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199692040.003.0016 Abstract and Keywords We propose a new developmental model that unites two phenomena that have so far been studied in isolation: joint attention in infancy and perspective-taking in young childhood. In this model, infants' abilities to jointly attend to objects and events with others, even though it does not require any understanding of perspectives, provides the necessary foundation and sets the stage for the later emerging ability to understand perspectives. Any perspectival difference presupposes a shared object onto which the perspectives converge—joint attention constitutes this shared object of perception. The understanding of perspectives then develops in two distinct steps. First, infants and young children learn to take perspectives, which allows them to understand others' speech acts and actions involving (perceptual, epistemic, or conceptual) perspectives that differ from their own. Second, Page 1 of 30 PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a monograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: MPI fuer Evolutionare Anthropologie; date: 03 March 2016 Perspective-Taking and its Foundation in Joint Attention children between 4 and 5 years of age come to confront perspectives: they can now explicitly acknowledge that the same object may be viewed or construed in alternative ways.
    [Show full text]
  • King's Research Portal
    King’s Research Portal DOI: 10.1007/s10803-020-04364-z Document Version Peer reviewed version Link to publication record in King's Research Portal Citation for published version (APA): Taylor, L. J., Charman, T., Howlin, P. A., Slonims, V., Green, J., The PACT-G Consortium, & Emsley, R. A. (2020). Brief Report: Associations between preverbal social communication skills, language and symptom severity in children with autism: An investigation using the Early Sociocognitive Battery. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(4), 1434-1442. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04364-z Citing this paper Please note that where the full-text provided on King's Research Portal is the Author Accepted Manuscript or Post-Print version this may differ from the final Published version. If citing, it is advised that you check and use the publisher's definitive version for pagination, volume/issue, and date of publication details. And where the final published version is provided on the Research Portal, if citing you are again advised to check the publisher's website for any subsequent corrections. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the Research Portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognize and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. •Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the Research Portal for the purpose of private study or research. •You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain •You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the Research Portal Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.
    [Show full text]
  • Joint Attention Revisited: Finding Strengths Among Children with Autism
    HHS Public Access Author manuscript Author ManuscriptAuthor Manuscript Author Autism. Manuscript Author Author manuscript; Manuscript Author available in PMC 2017 July 01. Published in final edited form as: Autism. 2016 July ; 20(5): 538–550. doi:10.1177/1362361315593536. Joint attention revisited: Finding strengths among children with autism Sarah Hurwitz1 and Linda R. Watson2 1Indiana University Bloomington, USA 2The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, USA Abstract Differences in joint attention (JA) are prominent for some children with autism and are often used as an indicator of the disorder. This study examined the JA competencies of young children with autism who demonstrated JA ability and compared them to children with developmental delays. Method: Forty children with autism and developmental delays were matched pairwise based on mental and chronological age. Videos of children engaging in play were coded for the frequency and forms (eye contact, gestures, affect, etc.) of JA. Additionally, concurrent language was compared among children with autism (N=32) by their JA ability. Results: Children with ASD entered into JA significantly less often than children with DD but once engaged, used the forms of JA similarly. For the matched pairs there were no differences in language but the children with autism who used JA had significantly better language than children with autism who did not (even after controlling for mental age). Conclusions: There is a group of young children with autism who can use JA but do so at lower frequencies than children with DD. Possible reasons include difficulty disengaging attention and limited intrinsic social motivation to share.
    [Show full text]
  • Shared Attention and Interindividual Neural Synchronization in the Human Right Inferior Frontal Cortex
    Chapter 11 Shared Attention and Interindividual Neural Synchronization in the Human Right Inferior Frontal Cortex Norihiro Sadato Abstract During a dyadic social interaction, two individuals can share visual attention through gaze, directed to each other (eye contact) or to a third person or an object (joint attention). Eye contact and joint attention are tightly coupled to generate the state of shared attention across individuals. Hyperscanning fMRI conducted with pairs of adults during joint attention tasks showed interindividual neural synchronization in the right inferior frontal gyrus. To explore how joint attention generates the state of shared attention, and whether its memory trace persists during a subsequent eye-contact condition, two-day hyperscanning fMRI study was conducted, in which pairs of participants performed a real-time mutual gaze task followed by a joint attention task on the first day and mutual gaze tasks several days later. The joint attention task enhanced eye-blink synchronization, which is a behavioral index of shared attention. When the same participant pairs underwent mutual gaze without joint attention on the second day, enhanced eye-blink synchronization persisted, which was positively correlated with interindividual neural synchronization within the right inferior frontal gyrus. Neural synchronization was also positively correlated with enhanced eye-blink synchroni- zation during the previous joint attention task session. These results indicate that shared attention is represented and retained by pair-specific neural synchronization during mutual gaze that cannot be reduced to the individual level. This interbrain effect highlights the role of the right inferior frontal gyrus in the execution and learning of attentional coordination and sharing attention between self and others.
    [Show full text]
  • Jigsaw Method Joint Attention in Humans and Animals
    Joint Attention in Humans and Animals J 1663 attention is not always used in the literature, but as Jigsaw Method we will see below, it is important to keep in mind when A cooperative learning technique developed by Aronson comparing the joint attention skills of humans and (1978) in which students work in small groups, where animals. each group member is assigned to become an “expert” on some aspect of a unit of study. After reading about Theoretical Background their area of expertise, the experts from different In humans, joint attention plays an extremely impor- groups meet to discuss their topic, and then return to tant role in many social activities, including communi- their groups and take turns teaching their topics to cation, cooperation, and education. For example, there their group mates. is much research showing that infants and toddlers learn novel words more easily when the words are References presented to them inside, as opposed to outside of, a joint attentional focus. Joint attention supports learn- Aronson, E. (1978). The jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills: Sage. ing (and other cooperative activities) by providing a “meeting of minds” or shared base of understanding on which to build. The clearest signs of joint attention behaviors emerge developmentally in human infants Joint Attention in Humans and around 9–12 months of age. J Animals Nonhuman animals are often credited with the ability to engage in joint attention as well. For example, MALINDA CARPENTER a wide variety of animals (e.g., monkeys, apes, dogs, Department of Developmental and Comparative dolphins, birds, and goats) can follow others’ gaze Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary direction to end up looking at the same thing, and Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany thus many researchers have claimed that they engage in joint attention.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Joint Attention in Pragmatic Language Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Ellen F
    Seattle aP cific nivU ersity Digital Commons @ SPU Clinical Psychology Dissertations Psychology, Family, and Community, School of Spring May 10th, 2018 The Role of Joint Attention in Pragmatic Language Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Ellen F. Geib Seattle Pacific nU iversity Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.spu.edu/cpy_etd Part of the Child Psychology Commons, Clinical Psychology Commons, and the Developmental Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Geib, Ellen F., "The Role of Joint Attention in Pragmatic Language Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders" (2018). Clinical Psychology Dissertations. 32. https://digitalcommons.spu.edu/cpy_etd/32 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychology, Family, and Community, School of at Digital Commons @ SPU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Clinical Psychology Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ SPU. The Role of Joint Attention in Pragmatic Language Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Ellen F. Geib A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy In Clinical Psychology Seattle Pacific University May 10, 2018 Approved by: Reviewed by: Beverly J. Wilson, Ph.D. Amy Mezulis, Ph.D. Professor of Clinical Psychology Chair, Clinical Psychology Seattle Pacific University Associate Professor of Clinical Psychology Dissertation Chair Seattle Pacific University Lynette Bikos, Ph.D. Katy Tangenberg, Ph.D. Professor of Clinical Psychology Dean, School of Psychology, Family & Seattle Pacific University Community Committee Member Seattle Pacific University Beau Reilly, Ph.D., ABPP, ABPdN Adjunct Professor of Clinical Psychology Seattle Pacific University International Center for Autism and Neurodevelopment Committee Member ATTENTION, PRAGMATIC LANGUAGE, ASD i Table of Contents List of Figures ..................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Attention During Social Interaction in Children with Autism
    Attention during social interaction in children with autism: Comparison to specific language impairment, typical development, and links to cognition Hanley, M., Riby, D. M., McCormack, T., Carty, C., Coyle, L., Crozier, N., Robinson, J., & McPhillips, M. (2014). Attention during social interaction in children with autism: Comparison to specific language impairment, typical development, and links to cognition. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(7), 908-924. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.03.020 Published in: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders Document Version: Peer reviewed version Queen's University Belfast - Research Portal: Link to publication record in Queen's University Belfast Research Portal Publisher rights © 2014 Elsevier Ltd. This manuscript version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by- nc-nd/4.0/ which permits distribution and reproduction for non-commercial purposes, provided the author and source are cited. General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Queen's University Belfast Research Portal is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The Research Portal is Queen's institutional repository that provides access to Queen's research output. Every effort has been made to ensure that content in the Research Portal does not infringe any person's rights, or applicable UK laws. If you discover content in the Research Portal that you believe breaches copyright or violates any law, please contact [email protected].
    [Show full text]
  • Joint Attention and the Social Phenotype of Autism Spectrum Disorder: a Perspective from Developmental Psychopathology
    Trim Size: 8.5in x 11in Cicchetti c04.tex V2 - Volume III - 08/26/2015 9:18am Page 116 CHAPTER 4 Joint Attention and the Social Phenotype of Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Perspective From Developmental Psychopathology AMY VAUGHAN VAN HECKE, TASHA OSWALD, and PETER MUNDY OVERVIEW 116 JOINT ATTENTION AND DEFINING THE SOCIAL A HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE ON AUTISM SPECTRUM DEFICITS OF ASD 133 DISORDER 117 The Social-Motivation Model and Joint Attention in ASD 134 DIAGNOSTIC DESCRIPTION OF ASD 118 Joint Attention and the Social-Cognitive Model of ASD 134 JOINT ATTENTION IN TYPICAL DEVELOPMENT 119 The Disassociation of IJA and RJA in ASD 134 Measurement of Subtypes of Joint Attention 120 Specific Effects on Initiating Joint Attention inASD 136 Learning and the Importance of Joint Attention 121 APPLYING THE JOINT ATTENTION Joint Attention and the Social-Cognitive Hypothesis 122 PDPM TO ASD 137 THE NEURAL SYSTEMS OF JOINT ATTENTION 123 Neural Connectivity and Activity-Dependent Genes in ASD 137 Social Cognition and the PDPM of Joint Attention 128 Visual Attention Control and Joint Attention in ASD 138 Inside-Out Processing and the Joint Attention PDPM 130 Joint Attention, Learning, and Interventions for ASD 138 Active Vision and the Joint Attention PDPM 130 SUMMARY 143 DYNAMIC SYSTEMS AND THE JOINT ATTENTION FUTURE DIRECTIONS 144 PDPM 131 REFERENCES 144 OVERVIEW social-cognition. In particular, human social-cognition may be viewed as the outgrowth of a special form of This chapter describes one way translational research human information processing that we call joint attention has changed how we think about, diagnose, and treat the (Mundy & Newell, 2007).
    [Show full text]
  • Children's Theory of Mind, Joint Attention, and Video
    CHILDREN’S THEORY OF MIND, JOINT ATTENTION, AND VIDEO CHAT by RYAN CURRY Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Department of Cognitive Science CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY May, 2021 CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES We hereby approve the thesis of Ryan Curry Candidate for the degree of Master of Arts* Committee Chair Fey Parrill Committee Member Mark Turner Committee Member Elizabeth Short Date of Defense March 9th, 2021 *We also certify that written approval has been obtained for any proprietary material contained therein. Table of Contents List of Tables ....................................................................................................................... ii List of Figures .................................................................................................................... iii Abstract ............................................................................................................................. iv Introduction ........................................................................................................................ 1 Method .............................................................................................................................. 22 Results .............................................................................................................................. 37 Discussion ........................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Joint Attention in the Verbal Behavior Development Trajectory
    The Role of Joint Attention in the Verbal Behavior Development Trajectory Ginger T. Harms Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy under the Executive Committee of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY 2020 © 2020 Ginger T. Harms All Rights Reserved 2 ABSTRACT The Role of Joint Attention in the Verbal Behavior Development Trajectory Ginger T. Harms Three experiments are presented in which I sought to determine the relation between joint attention and the related verbal behavior developmental cusps. Participants in Experiment I were 37 preschool students with and without disabilities with verbal behavior levels ranging from prefoundational to bidirectional cusps. Participants were selected from a convenience sample within a preschool for children with and without disabilities. Measures included the participants’ responses to opportunities for mand joint attention (MJA), responding to joint attention (RJA), initiation of joint attention (IJA), conditioned reinforcement for adult faces (CRF), conditioned reinforcement for adult voices (CRV), generalized matching, generalized imitation (GI), listener literacy (LL), mands, tacts, praise and a conditioned reinforcer, say-do correspondence, self-talk during fantasy play, conversational units (CUs) unidirectional Naming (UniN), bidirectional Naming (BiN), and observational learning (OL) as a listener and speaker. Pearson’s correlations and independent samples t-tests were conducted to determine which cusps were related to joint attention. Furthermore, stepwise multiple regression analyses were utilized to determine the largest significant predictors of joint attention. The results showed strong associations between many of the verbal behavior cusps and joint attention, specifically with conditioned reinforcement for adult faces explaining 27% of the variance in participants’ RJA, and conditioned reinforcement for adult faces combined with a tact repertoire explaining 64% of the variance in participants’ IJA.
    [Show full text]
  • Joint Attention Without Recursive Mindreading: on the Role of Second-Person Engagement
    Joint Attention Without Recursive Mindreading: On the Role of Second-Person Engagement León, Felipe Published in: Philosophical Psychology DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2021.1917533 Publication date: 2021 Document version Peer reviewed version Citation for published version (APA): León, F. (2021). Joint Attention Without Recursive Mindreading: On the Role of Second-Person Engagement. Philosophical Psychology, 34(4), 550-580. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2021.1917533 Download date: 23. sep.. 2021 Joint Attention Without Recursive Mindreading: On the Role of Second-Person Engagement Felipe León1 1) Center for Subjectivity Research, Department of Communication, University of Copenhagen, Karen Blixens Plads 8, DK-2300 Copenhagen, Denmark. E-mail: [email protected] Abstract: On a widely held characterization, triadic joint attention is the capacity to perceptually attend to an object or event together with another subject. In the last four decades, research in developmental psychology has provided increasing evidence of the crucial role that this capacity plays in socio-cognitive development, early language acquisition, and the development of perspective-taking. Yet, there is a striking discrepancy between the general agreement that joint attention is critical in various domains, and the lack of theoretical consensus on how to account for it. This paper pursues three interrelated aims: (i) it examines the contrast between reductive and non-reductive views of (triadic) joint attention, by bringing into focus the notion of recursive mindreading; (ii) it assembles, advances, and discusses a number of arguments against reductive views; (iii) finally, in dialogue with some prominent non-reductive views, it concludes by outlining the case for a non-reductive view that gives pride of place to the idea that co-attenders relate to one another as a ‘you’.
    [Show full text]