Decline & Fall of the Freudian Empire

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Decline & Fall of the Freudian Empire Decline & Fall Freudianof the Empire Decline & Fall Freudianof the Empire WithHans a new J. prefaceEysenck by Sybil B. G. Eysenck Originally published in 1985 by Viking Published 2004 by Transaction Publishers Published 2017 by Routledge 2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN 711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017, USA Routledge is an imprint o f the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business New material this edition copyright © 2004 by Taylor & Francis. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers. Notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe. Library of Congress Catalog Number: 2004041192 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Eysenck, H. J. (Hans Jurgen), 1916- Decline and fall of the Freudian empire / Hans J. Eysenck ; with a new preface by Sybil B. G. Eysenck. p. cm. Originally published: Harmondsworth, Middlesex, England ; New York : Viking, 1985. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-7658-0945-1 (pbk.: alk. paper) 1. Psychoanalysis. 2. Psychotherapy. 3. Freud, Sigmund, 1856-1939. I. Title. BF173.E89 2004 150.19’52—dc22 2004041192 ISBN 13: 978-0-7658-0945-2 (pbk) Contents PREFACE TO THE TRANSACTION EDITION vii INTRODUCTION 7 O N E : F R E U D THE M AN 19 TWO: PSYCHOANALYSIS AS A METHOD OF TREATMENT 42 THREE: PSYCHOANAL YTIC TREATMENT AND ITS ALTERNATIVES 6 6 FOUR: FREUD AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE CHILD 92 FIVE: THE INTERPRETATION Of DREAMS AND THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGY OF EVERYDAY LIFE 115 SIX: THE EXPERIMENTAL STUDY OF FREUDIAN CONCEPTS 149 SEVEN: PSYCHO-BABBLE AND PSEUDO- HISTORY 171 EIGHT: REST IN PEACE: AN EVALUATION 193 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 209 BIBLIOGRAPHY 211 INDEX OF PROPER NAMES 219 INDEX OF SUBJECTS 222 P reface to the Transaction Edition Many years ago, my late husband, the author of this book, and I visited a distinguished American psychologist working at a New York psychiatric hospital. He opined that he would surely lose his job if he were to confess to misgivings about the efficacy of psycho- analytic therapy. Privately, however, he had his concerns and he welcomed Hans’s 1952 article casting doubt not only on psycho- analysis but also on psychotherapy as a whole. (“The Effects of Psychotherapy: An Evaluation,” Journal of Consulting Psychology, 16, 319-324) At that time, Freud was considered a messiah and psychoanaly- sis the “savior of souls.” So, the 1952 article brought an avalanche of criticism from practicing analysts who were appalled at the audac- ity of the challenge. Years went by and Hans became more and more convinced that the blind adulation of Freud and his followers was unjustified. In- deed, many patients were being fooled into expensive and time- consuming sessions, yet cures were not by any means guaranteed. After much reading of Freud’s work, Hans’s considered view was that Freud was indeed a literary genius who expressed his theories with great eloquence. However, there was absolutely no attempt at any scientific verification of his psychoanalytic technique. You took Freud and his theory on trust or you were accused of some kind of subconscious resistance. So basically, the main objection to all psychotherapies, and psy- choanalysis in particular, was their lack of scientific theory or vali- dation. Therapists relied on case histories and did not even pretend at scientific proof. There were many claims of successful treatments but also reports of disastrous failures and patients becoming worse after therapy. Perhaps this is why Hans decided, roughly thirty years after his 1952 article, that a more detailed exposé was required and embarked in 1985 on the present book, Decline and Fall of the Freudian Empire. It is all too easy to criticize, but the real strength of Hans’s posi- tion was that he not only cast doubt on traditional psychotherapy and psychoanalysis, but that he suggested a viable alternative: be- havior therapy. This was based on scientific learning theory and was subsequently shown to be infinitely more effective therapeuti- cally to other forms of therapy. It took considerable courage to write this book in the face of the “zeitgeist” of the times. Nowadays, few eyebrows would be raised as more and more psychologists and therapists have cast doubt on the value of counseling, especially as this has now become a growth industry and nobody has bothered to evaluate its efficacy Of all the seventy-eight books Hans wrote this one is my favorite and may well be one of his best. Sybil B. G. Eysenck Introduction This is a book about Sigmund Freud and psychoanalysis. There are many such books, and the reader might rightly demand to know why he or she should be asked to pay good money to buy a new one, and spend precious time reading it. The answer is very simple. Most books on this topic are written by psychoan- alysts, or at least by camp-followers of the Freudian movement; they are therefore uncritical, unaware of alternative theories, and written more as weapons in a war of propaganda than objective assessments of the present status of psychoanalysis. There are, of course, exceptions to this rule, and some of the most important of these are mentioned in the bibliography at the end of this book Important new books, such as those by Sulloway. Ellenberger, Thornton, Rillaer, Roazen, Fromkin, Timpanaro, Gruenbaum, Kline, and others are long and highly technical; they are invaluable to the professional student, but cannot be recommended to non-professional readers trying to find out what modern scholarship has discovered about the truth or falsity of Freudian doctrines. But for the sake of readers who wish to check for themselves, I have referred in the text to the major historical writers who have carefully looked at the evidence and given detailed accounts of what actually happened, with special reference to factual events, publications and other evidence available to all. This book, then, is inevitably based on the scholarship of the people mentioned above, and many others whose works have been consulted. It is, nevertheless, special in bringing together material covering a wide range of topics within the general field of psychoanalysis – the interpretation of dreams, the psycho- pathology of everyday life, the effects of psychoanalytic psy- chotherapy, Freudian psycho-history and anthropology, the experimental study of Freudian concepts, and many more. I have tried to do this in a non-technical manner, to make the book accessible to readers who have only a passing knowledge of Freudian psychoanalysis, and no professional background in psychology or anthropology. It would have been easier to write a book five times as long and full of technical jargon, but I have found it a salutary experience to try to reduce this wealth of material to the confines of a short, non-technical book. The effort of doing so cleared my mind of many preconceptions, and I am grateful to the many experts whose works I consulted, for clarifying puzzles and paradoxes that created numerous diffi- culties before. I have given many lectures on the various topics discussed in this book, and these were inevitably introduced as being ‘controversial’. Similarly, I have no doubt that reviewers will call this book ‘controversial’, but with such an assessment I cannot agree. I have tried to deal with ascertained facts, and to add as little comment and interpretation as possible. The conclusions may be ‘controversial’ because they disagree with previous assessments arrived at without the benefit of recent scholarship, but that does not make them contentious. It simply means that our knowledge has progressed, that our understand- ing has advanced, and that facts have been discovered recently which throw a new light on Freud and psychoanalysis. Much of this new evidence is highly critical of claims made by Freud and his followers, and, as the title of this book suggests, the inevitable result has been a decline in the influence of Freudian theory, and in the esteem in which psychoanalysis is held. That there has been such a decline can hardly be doubted by anyone acquainted with the present climate of opinion among psychiatrists (qualified doctors with specialist training in the medical study of mental disorder) and psychologists (graduates in the scientific study of human behaviour), as well as philosophers, anthropologists and historians, in the United States and the United Kingdom. This disillusionment has not yet advanced as far in South America, France, and a few other countries which still cling stubbornly to outmoded concepts and theories. However, even there doubts are beginning to appear, and gradually they will follow the U.S. and the U.K. In dealing with Freud’s work, I have looked at it exclusively from the scientific point of view. To many people this may appear too strict. They may maintain that Freud’s contribution has been to hermeneutics – the interpretation and meaning of mental events – rather than to the scientific study of human behaviour. Others may insist on the social and literary import- ance of the Freudian opus, or look upon him as a prophet and innovator, a man who changed our sexual and social mores and, like Moses, led us into a new world. Freud may be said to fit into all these different roles, but I am not qualified to deal with them. To judge the importance of prophets, innovators, or literary figures requires a profound knowledge of history, sociology, or literature and literary criti- cism.
Recommended publications
  • Hans Eysenck's Interface Between the Brain and Personality: Modern Evidence on the Cognitive Neuroscience of Personality
    King’s Research Portal DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.009 Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to publication record in King's Research Portal Citation for published version (APA): Mitchell, R. L. C., & Kumari, V. (2016). Hans Eysenck’s interface between the brain and personality: Modern evidence on the cognitive neuroscience of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 74-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.009 Citing this paper Please note that where the full-text provided on King's Research Portal is the Author Accepted Manuscript or Post-Print version this may differ from the final Published version. If citing, it is advised that you check and use the publisher's definitive version for pagination, volume/issue, and date of publication details. And where the final published version is provided on the Research Portal, if citing you are again advised to check the publisher's website for any subsequent corrections. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the Research Portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognize and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. •Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the Research Portal for the purpose of private study or research. •You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain •You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the Research Portal Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.
    [Show full text]
  • Hans J. Eysenck: Introduction to Centennial Special Issue
    Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 1–7 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Hans J. Eysenck: Introduction to centennial special issue Philip J. Corr City University London, Department of Psychology, London EC1V 0HB, United Kingdom first to criticise his views on the efficacy of psychotherapy (Eysenck, article info 1952, 1965) — perhaps there is something to Freudian projection after all! Lastly, it would be shamefully remiss not to acknowledge the pro- Article history: Received 13 July 2016 duction staff at Elsevier for their support and constant hard work, and Accepted 16 July 2016 Tony Vernon, Editor-in-Chief of PAID, for guiding the special issue to fruition. Such vital behind-the-scenes work is rarely seen and all too Keywords: often under-appreciated. Hans Eysenck There are 34 papers in the special issue, and although they span a History Academic and professional psychology wide range of topics and opinions, they fall into thematic groups that allow the convenient structuring of this Introduction. 1. Papers in special issue The year 2016 marks the centenary of the birth of Hans Juergen Eysenck (4 March 1916–4 September 1997). This special issue devoted Sybil Eysenck opens the special issue with reflections on her to his work and life is very timely and its publication in Personality and husband as a scientist, psychologist and family man. She shows the Individual Differences (PAID) most appropriate as this was the influential breadth of Hans's interests and the ways in which he was often journal he founded in 1983 — the year of his de jure, but certainly not de misunderstood.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Personality and Cognitions Underlying Entrepreneurial Intentions Benjamin R. Walker a Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfilment O
    1 Personality and Cognitions underlying Entrepreneurial Intentions Benjamin R. Walker A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy School of Management UNSW Business School March 30, 2015 2 Table of Contents Acknowledgements .................................................................................................................... 6 Originality statement .................................................................................................................. 7 Publications and conference presentations arising from this thesis ........................................... 8 List of abbreviations .................................................................................................................. 9 Thesis Abstract......................................................................................................................... 10 Chapter 1: Introduction ............................................................................................................ 11 Chapter 2: Assessing the impact of revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory ...................... 20 Table 1: Articles with original Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (o-RST) and revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (r-RST) measures .......................................................... 26 Table 2: Categorization of original Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (o-RST) and revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (r-RST) studies in the five years from 2010-2014 ........ 29 Chapter 3: How
    [Show full text]
  • Don't Make a Freudian Slip
    PSYCHOLOGY DON’T MAKE A FREUDIAN SLIP Resources for Courses Activity Overview Outlining Freud’s Theory of Gender Development is a difficult task. Firstly, there are a lot of specialist terms that students often fail to include (e.g. unconscious processes, identification and internalisation) and secondly, it is very difficult to write a concise summary of Freud’s Theory because there is so much to include. The aim of this task is to consolidate student’s knowledge of the key Freudian terms and to practice writing a concise summary of Freud’s Theory of Gender Development. Resources Required . Don’t Make A Freudian Slip Handout Teacher Instructions Teaching & Learning Strategy A Provide the students with a copy of the Don’t Make A Freudian Slip Handout. Ask the students to read each paragraph in turn and add in the missing specialist terminology. After that, once you have gone through the answers, ask your students to highlight the specialist terminology related to Freud’s Theory of Gender Development. Once your students have completed the first task, ask them to write a concise summary of Freud’s Theory in approximately 125-175 words, while trying to incorporate all of the key features of Freud’s Theory. Tell the students to image that they are answering the following exam question: Describe and evaluate Freud’s psychoanalytic explanation of gender development. (16 marks) If you want to be cruel, only give your students six-eight minutes to complete their summary, as this is roughly the length of time they would have to write an outline in their exam.
    [Show full text]
  • Article in Press + Model
    ARTICLE IN PRESS + MODEL International Journal of Psychophysiology xx (2006) xxx–xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho The role of theory in the psychophysiology of personality: From Ivan Pavlov to Jeffrey Gray ⁎ Philip J. Corr , Adam M. Perkins Department of Psychology, University of Wales Swansea, Singleton Park, Swansea SA2 8PP, UK Received 16 June 2005; received in revised form 24 October 2005; accepted 5 January 2006 Abstract Psychophysiological approaches to personality have made significant progress in recent years, partly as a spin-off of technological innovation (e.g., functional neuroimaging) and partly as a result of an emerging theoretical consensus regarding the structure and biology of basic processes. In this field, Jeffrey Gray's influential psychophysiological theory of personality – now widely known as Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) – owes much to Pavlov, who devoted a large proportion of his later life to personality differences and their implications for psychiatry. In this article, we trace the influence of Pavlov on Hans Eysenck's and Jeffrey Gray's work, and then provide a brief description of RST in order to highlight some of the central problems – as well as some tentative solutions – in the psychophysiology of personality. Specifically, the importance of theory in personality research is stressed by the contrast of Gray's theoretically driven model with less fertile atheoretical (i.e., exploratory–inductive) approaches. The fecundity of RST, which has been in continual development over a period of thirty years, is discussed in the light of Karl Popper's views on the nature of science, especially the formulation of the ‘problem situation’, which sets up the theoretical and operational conditions under which hypotheses may be challenged and tested to destruction.
    [Show full text]
  • SLIPS of the TONGUE: the FACTS and a STRATIFICATIONAL MODEL by Gary S. Dell and Peter A. Reich
    SLIPS OF THE TONGUE: THE FACTS AND A STRATIFICATIONAL MODEL by Gary S. Dell and Peter A. Reich One of the best ways of finding out how a system is constructed is to observe what happens when that system breaks down, when it fails to operate perfectly. Suppose you regularly receive a check from a small company, The check may be generated by a human being, or it may be produced by a computer. So long as everything operates perfectly, it would be impossible to determine which. But you may get clues if a mistake occurs, because computers and human beings make different types of errors. If you get a check for $0.00, it is likely to be a computer error. If you get a check for $517.00 when you are owed $571.00, it is likely to be a human error, because human beings often transpose digits. Similarly, if your goal is to find out what the system is like that underlies language, it can be very useful to look at what happens when things go wrong. This is the issue to which this paper is addressed. We shall review one class of language errors-slips of the tongue-to explore the implications this phenomenon has toward understanding in general how language processing works in human beings. We shall argue that stratificational grammar may be on the right track as a model of language behavior, because a modified version of it can be designed that makes very humanlike errors. THE FACTS Slips of the tongue as a Iinguistic phenomenon have been known about and studied since before the time of Freud.
    [Show full text]
  • Ffamous Psychologists
    4/13/2012 Famous Psychologists G. Stanley Hall • G. Stanley Hall's primary interests were in evolutionary psychology and child development • Perhaps his greatest contribution was to the development and growth of early psychology. • By the year 1898, Hall had supervised 30 out of the 54 Ph.D. degrees that had been awarded in the United States • First APA President. • First Lab in USA 1 4/13/2012 Edward Titchner • Leader of Structuralism • Used Introspection • Studied under Wundt • With the goal to describe the structure of the mind in terms of the most primitive elements of mental experience. This theory focused on three things: the individual elements of consciousness, how they organized into more complex experiences, and how these mental phenomena correlated with physical events. The mental elements structure themselves in such a way to allow conscious experience. William James • Functionalism James opposed the structuralism focus on introspection and breaking down mental events to the smallest elements. Instead, James focused on the wholeness of an event, taking into the impact of the environment on behavior. • Stream of conscio u sness • Wrote Principles of Psychology. 2 4/13/2012 Edward Thordike-Behaviorism The law of effect basically states that “responses that produce a satisfying effect in a particular situation become more likely to occur again in that situation, and responses that produce a discomforting effect become less likely to occur again in that situation.” Original puzzle boxes with animals. Ivan Pavlov- Classical Conditioning Phobias, involuntary learning, CNS 3 4/13/2012 John Watson-Behaviorism • According to John Watson, psychology should be the science of observable behavior • Psychology as the behaviorist views it is a purely objective experimental branch of natural science.
    [Show full text]
  • Behavior Therapy Is Behavioristic
    BEHAVIORTHERAPy ( 1972 ) 3, 609-613 I~ESPONSES AND I~EJOINDER Behavior Therapy Is Behavioristic HANS J. EYSENCK1 University of London This article tries to answer an objection sometimes raised by critics of behavior therapy, namely, flaat it is not "behavioristic." The objection is shown to be ungrounded and dependent on an outmoded use of the term "behaviorism." "Is "behavior therapy" behavioristic?" asked Locke (1971), and con- cluded that the answer is in the negative. One feels inclined to quote in reply Sir Francis Bacon (Dicks, 1955, p. 182), who wrote that it is "The first distemper of learning, when men study words and not matter." Locke brought into juxtaposition two terms, "behavior therapy" and "behavior- ism"; in order to form a judgment of their compatibility, one must first arrive at a definition of both which would not be arbitrary, but widely agreed. By using his own arbitrary definitions Locke was able to make a case; I shall try to show just how arbitrary the definitions are. According to Locke, the term "behavior therapy" was coined by Skinner and Lindsley (1954), and "has since been widely promulgated by Eysenck (1960, 1964)." This is incorrect; the term was "coined" inde- pendently by at least three people or groups, namely, the two mentioned above, and by Lazarus (1958). Its use by Skinner and Lindsley was con- fined to a Status Report to the Office of Naval Research, and never pub- lished; it was not familiar to either of the other two authors. Furthermore, it referred entirely to operant methods of treatment, a restriction which makes it clear that its use was quite different from that intended by Eysenck (1959), who explicitly established its present usage as referring to all methods of therapy which are based on modern learning theory-- thus including both the Skinnerian operant methods and Wolpe's desen- sitization method, as well as many others (Eysenck, 1960).
    [Show full text]
  • Personality Associations with Mood, Hoarding, Health and Well-Being Janet Katherine Spittlehouse
    Personality Associations with Mood, Hoarding, Health and Well-being Janet Katherine Spittlehouse A thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Psychological Medicine University of Otago, Christchurch New Zealand 31 August 2016 Abstract Background: Personality has been of interest since ancient times. Hippocrates, also known as ‘The father of Western Medicine’ was possibly the first to document the association between personality and mental and physical health by describing the ancient medical theory of Humourism. Over the last 100 years the study of personality has been evolving and there are many different perspectives. Trait perspectives have become popular but they lack any underlying theory about how personality develops. Psychobiological models offer descriptions of personality and provide testable theories on how biology influences their development. A robust psychobiological model is Cloninger’s psychobiological theory and it provided the basis for this project. Objectives: This project explored the associations of personality in different mental health settings using the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI; Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, & Wetzel, 1994), the personality inventory developed by Cloninger and colleagues, that is suitable for measuring both normal and abnormal personality. The TCI was used to examine the impact of depression on personality measurement and personality associations to self-reported physical and mental health, mood disorders, hoarding behaviours and well-being. Methods: Participants for this project were from three studies. Two randomised clinical trials designed to examine predictors of treatment response for depressed outpatients using either antidepressant medication (N=195) or psychotherapy (N=177) were used to examine the impact of depression on measures of personality.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction to Political Psychology
    INTRODUCTION TO POLITICAL PSYCHOLOGY This comprehensive, user-friendly textbook on political psychology explores the psychological origins of political behavior. The authors introduce read- ers to a broad range of theories, concepts, and case studies of political activ- ity. The book also examines patterns of political behavior in such areas as leadership, group behavior, voting, race, nationalism, terrorism, and war. It explores some of the most horrific things people do to each other, as well as how to prevent and resolve conflict—and how to recover from it. This volume contains numerous features to enhance understanding, includ- ing text boxes highlighting current and historical events to help students make connections between the world around them and the concepts they are learning. Different research methodologies used in the discipline are employed, such as experimentation and content analysis. This third edition of the book has two new chapters on media and social movements. This accessible and engaging textbook is suitable as a primary text for upper- level courses in political psychology, political behavior, and related fields, including policymaking. Martha L. Cottam (Ph.D., UCLA) is a Professor of Political Science at Washington State University. She specializes in political psychology, inter- national politics, and intercommunal conflict. She has published books and articles on US foreign policy, decision making, nationalism, and Latin American politics. Elena Mastors (Ph.D., Washington State University) is Vice President and Dean of Applied Research at the American Public University System. Prior to that, she was an Associate Professor at the Naval War College and held senior intelligence and policy positions in the Department of Defense.
    [Show full text]
  • Sigmund Freud
    Term Test 4 • mean = 73.6 • SD = 12.6 • range = 39-100 • one Q discarded • optional exam viewing to follow once Final exam everyone has written • Thurs April 15, 2 pm test • Winter semester only • 30% of course grade, mult choice, up to 100 Qs • 60% last third, 20% middle third, 20% first third • optional review session, last day of classes Three Minute Review SOCIAL INFLUENCE: OBEDIENCE • Why do people obey to an extreme degree? – Cult followers • Jonestown, Waco – bureaucrats in genocides • Nazi Holocaust, Cambodia, Rwanda, etc. • Milgram’s Obedience Experiments – The majority of people will follow orders to an extreme degree – Results surprised many people, esp. psychologists – Affected by proximity to victim, proximity to authority, and reactions of others in same situation – Not affected by personality traits • Stanford Prison Experiment – Ordinary people get caught up in roles • Banality of evil (Hannah Arendt) – Perhaps Adolf Eichmann was no different than the rest of us • Psychology of genocide 1. difficult living conditions, fierce competition for resources 2. strong in- vs. out-groups 3. violence, blaming the victim 4. violence justifies itself • can’t stop because of cognitive dissonance Take a Personality Test • Take the test • Put your ID but NOT your name • Check the web site for results • Read instructions on the web carefully It’s a Small World After All • Stanley Milgram also did other cool, more optimistic experiments • Milgram (1967) -- If you pick any two people at random, how many intermediate acquaintances does it take to establish a link between them? Joe Smith Timothy Kuhn Omaha, Neb. Boston, Mass.
    [Show full text]
  • The Neural Basis of the Dynamic Unconscious
    Neuropsychoanalysis, 2011, 13 (1) 5 The Neural Basis of the Dynamic Unconscious Heather A. Berlin (New York) A great deal of complex cognitive processing occurs at the unconscious level and affects how humans behave, think, and feel. Sci- entists are only now beginning to understand how this occurs on the neural level. Understanding the neural basis of consciousness requires an account of the neural mechanisms that underlie both conscious and unconscious thought, and their dynamic interac- tion. For example, how do conscious impulses, thoughts, or desires become unconscious (e.g., repression) or, conversely, how do unconscious impulses, desires, or motives become conscious (e.g., Freudian slips)? Research taking advantage of advances in technologies, like functional magnetic resonance imaging, has led to a revival and re-conceptualization of some of the key concepts of psychoanalytic theory, but steps toward understanding their neural basis have only just commenced. According to psychoanalytic theory, unconscious dynamic processes defensively remove anxiety-provoking thoughts and impulses from consciousness in re- sponse to one’s conflicting attitudes. The processes that keep unwanted thoughts from entering consciousness include repression, suppression, and dissociation. In this literature review, studies from psychology and cognitive neuroscience in both healthy and patient populations that are beginning to elucidate the neural basis of these phenomena are discussed and organized within a con- ceptual framework. Further studies in this emerging field at the intersection of psychoanalytic theory and neuroscience are needed. Keywords: unconscious; psychodynamic; repression; suppression; dissociation; neural “Nothing is so difficult as not deceiving oneself.” 1998a). Early psychodynamic theorists attempted to Ludwig Wittgenstein [1889–1951] explain phenomena observed in the clinic, but lat- er cognitive scientists used computational models of the mind to explain empirical data.
    [Show full text]