University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana

Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School

2000

Reactivity and ecological momentary assessment using a sample of undergraduate problem drinkers

Alan L. Shields The University of Montana

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y

Recommended Citation Shields, Alan L., "Reactivity and ecological momentary assessment using a sample of undergraduate problem drinkers" (2000). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 8737. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/8737

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Maureen and Mike MANSFIELD LIBRARY

Tlie University ofjMONXANA

Permission is granted by the author to reproduce this material in its entirety, provided that this material is used for scholarly purposes and is properly cited in published works and reports.

* * Please check "Yes” or "No" and provide signature

Yes, I grant permission No, I do not grant permission

Author’s Signature

Date cpa

Any copying for commercial purposes or financial gain may be undertaken only with the author's explicit consent.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY AND ECOLOGICAL MOMENTARY ASSESSMENT USING A

SAMPLE OF UNDERGRADUATE PROBLEM DRINKERS

By:

Alan L. Shields

A Thesis Presented at the University of Montana

Department of

In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Master of Arts

Summer 2000

Approved by:

Chairperson

Dean, Grauate School

S ' 4 - 2.000 Date

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. UMI Number: EP39538

All rights reserved

INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

UMT Oiassrtation Pubtohing

UMI EP39538 Published by ProQuest LLC (2013). Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code

ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Shields, Alan L., M.A., Summer 2000

Reactivity and Ecological Momentary Assessment Using a Sample of Undergraduate Problem Drinkers (43 pp.).

Chairperson: Michael R. Hufford, Ph.D.

Stone and Shiftman (1994) define ecological momentary assessment (EMA) as monitoring or sampling strategies that assess phenomena at the moment they occur in natural settings, thus maximizing ecological validity while avoiding retrospective recall. EMA has been used to better understand addictive behaviors among smokers (Shiffman, Hufford, et al., 1997) and problem drinkers (Collins et al., 1998; Litt et al., 1998). Research has only begun to examine the extent to which real-time monitoring impacts the behaviors and cognitions under observation. This study examined behavioral, motivational, self-efficacy, alcohol urge, and self-reported reactivity to EMA among a sample of 33 undergraduate problem drinkers. Participants completed a 2-week real-time monitoring protocol using palmtop computers that assessed mood, activities, urge to drink, and drinking behavior. Findings suggest that overall magnitude of reactivity to EMA is small. An analysis of participant characteristics that may predict reactivity revealed several interesting findings. Suggestions for future research are presented.

11

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA

Reactivity and Ecological Momentary Assessment Using a Sample of

Undergraduate Problem Drinkers

Recently, researchers have been using new methodologies to overcome the

weaknesses inherent in retrospective self-reports. One such method, Ecological

Momentary Assessment (EMA), stresses the continuous investigation of individual

differences in behavior, cognition, and affect in the natural environment (Shiffman &

Stone, 1998). Stone & Shiffman (1994) outline four qualities that define EMA. First,

EMA assesses phenomena at the moment they occur in order to reduce recall .

Second, EMA is dependent upon careful timing of assessments in order to ensure that an

unbiased sample of the target behavior is assessed. Third, EMA methods typically obtain

a significant number of repeated observations in order to produce more reliable samples

and allow researchers to delineate covarying relationships over time. Finally, EMA

measurements are made in the environment that participants typically inhabit. These

characteristics of EMA are essential for researchers interested in understanding dynamic

processes as they evolve over time (e.g., Marlatt, 1985; Prochaska, DiClemente, &

Norcross, 1992; Vallacher & Nowak, 1994). Currently, EMA is being used to understand

smoking (e.g., Shifftnan, Paty, Gnys, Kassel, & Hickcox, 1996; Shiffman, Hufford et al.,

1997), drinking (e.g., Collins et al., 1998; Hufford et al., under review), the relationship

between coping and chronic pain (e.g., Affleck et al., 1998), stress, coping, and mood

(Stone et al., 1998), and stress and cardiovascular reactivity (e.g., Kamarck et al., 1998).

EMA has utilized electronic technology such as beepers (Johnson & Larson,

1982) and programmed wristwatches (Litt, Coony, & Morse, 1998) to prompt

participants to complete self-report questionnaires. Ecological validity has been further

1 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 2

improved through the use of palm-top computers (Shiffman et al., 1994; Shif&nan,

Hufford et al., 1997) which collect data in near real-time. In this way, all responses from

participants are date and time stamped, and participants can be monitored for compliance

thereby increasing the validity of their self-reports (Stone & Shiftman, 1994).

Work with EMA has demonstrated that traditional retrospective self-report

measures are vulnerable to inaccuracies and recall . Shiftman, Hufford et al. (1997)

showed that never-smokers asked about a “typical smokers” relapse produced extremely

similar relapse profiles as compared with the retrospective report of smokers. This

finding suggests that schemas about a target behavior can significantly influence

retrospective recall. Both the never-smokers’ schemas and smokers’ retrospective recall

failed to correlate to real-time reports of actual smoking lapses. This implies that, no

matter how reasonable and believable a behavioral account may sound, it can still be

inaccurate and systematically biased (Shiftman, Hufford et al., 1997). This is a

significant finding given that a large number of addictive behavior researchers rely on

retrospective methods to gather data. Retrospective reports may be able to address the

static variables associated with addiction, but EMA appears to be the superior

methodology for the accurate understanding of the day-to-day and moment-to-moment

phenomena theorized to be crucial in the study of addictive behaviors (Brownell, Marlatt,

Lichtenstein, & Wilson, 1986).

McKay (1999) called EMA the most important contribution to contemporary

addictive behavior research. He concluded his critical review of addictive behavior

research methodologies by encouraging the use of a multitrait-multimethod design for the

understanding of addictive behaviors. In particular, he called for the use of new

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 3

technologies (e.g., palm-top computers) in addictive behavior assessment as methods that

reduce memory inaccuracies and biases. If EMA represents the emerging methodology

in addictive behavior research, then researchers must begin to understand some of its

potential weaknesses and limitations.

EMA: Methodological Limitations and Reactivity

Despite many advantages, EMA may have unique limitations. One potential

source of inaccuracy and bias is reactivity. A reactive effect describes the degree to

which the intensity, frequency, and/or quality of a target variable will change when being

observed, monitored, or assessed (Nelson, 1977). EMA researchers frequently cite

reactivity as a potential confound in their studies (e.g., Shiffman, Hufford et al., 1997;

Collins et al., 1998; Shiffman et al., 1998; Affleck et al., 1998; Kamarck et al., 1998; Litt

et al., 1998); however, few studies (Cruise Broderick, Porter, Kaell, & Stone, 1996) exist

that systematically explore the reactivity phenomenon in EMA specifically.

Ecological momentary assessment may be particularly prone to reactivity as it

often involves a creative combination of two existing research orientations: (1) self­

monitoring (Nelson, 1977) and (2) the experience sampling method (Csikszentmihalyi &

Larson, 1987).

Self-monitoring. Self-monitoring is theorized to involve two-stages (Nelson,

1977). First, the participant must be able to discriminate aspects of his or her behavior

and effectively determine if the target behavior has indeed occurred. Second, the

participant must make a self-recording response. In other words, participants must then

use whatever method is being employed to record the occurrence of the target event.

Recent definitions of SM have been expanded to assess contextual variables associated

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 4

with the target behavior. Thus, observations regarding both one’s internal and external

environment are incorporated into the SM procedure (Korotitsch & Nelson-Gray, 1999).

Researchers have demonstrated that a wide variety of behaviors (e.g., academic

performance, cigarette smoking, eye-blink response, food consumption) can be reactive

to self-monitoring (e.g., Lam, Cole, Shapiro, & Bambara, 1994; Abrams & Wilson, 1979;

Sieck & McFall, 1976; Romanczyk, Tracy, Wilson, & Thorpe; 1973). The literature

regarding self-monitoring of drinking behavior has yielded mixed results. Some studies

have found no evidence of reactivity (e.g., Camey, Tennen, Affleck, Del Boca, &

Kranzler, 1998; Samo, Tucker, & Vuchinich, 1989, Sobell, Bogardis, Schuller, Leo, &

Sobell, 1989), while others suggest the presence of behavioral reactivity as a function of

self-monitoring (Uchalik, 1979).

Experience sampling. In contrast to self-monitoring, the experience sampling

method relies on an electronic instrument to signal a participant to report his or her

immediate activities, thoughts, and moods (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987). It is a

random sampling technique that has been successful in gathering base rate data of

behaviors and their antecedents (e.g. Delespaul & deVries, 1987). This assessment

strategy may produce reactivity in some circumstances (e.g., Larson, 1989; Magneberg,

1998). For example, a study of heavy drinkers found that monitoring activities and mood

resulted in decreased drinking over a three-week monitoring protocol (Magneberg, 1998).

Summary. Understanding the reactive effects of EMA is important for several

reasons. If EMA produces reactivity, then a loss of methodological validity must be

acknowledged. Also, this information can assist both the EMA researcher in minimizing

reactivity and serve as a guide to the applied clinician who may want to maximize

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 5

therapeutic reactivity. Moreover, understanding reactive effects of EMA can lead to

more interpretable research conclusions. For example, if EMA is introduced along with a

therapeutic intervention (e.g., Collins et al., 1998), changes in behavior due to reactivity

may make it hard to evaluate the effects of the intervention. In other words, reactivity

may be a source of unwanted variance in the dependent variables of interest. In addition,

if EMA is used to establish the frequency “baseline” of behaviors, researchers may be

misled as the EMA may alter the frequency of the target behavior and reduce the

representativeness of the target behavior (e.g., Paty et al., 1992). Finally, understanding

reactivity to EMA is crucial because it raises one of the most important and fundamental

questions concerning self-report - How does monitoring behavior, affect, and cognitions

affect these phenomena?

Present Study

Understanding reactivity to EMA is an important methodological issue for the

behavioral sciences (Affleck, Zautra, Tennen, & Armeli, 1999; Korotitsch & Nelson-

Gray, 1999). This study sought to explore reactivity to EMA by engaging hazardous

drinkers in an EMA study of their drinking behavior. Five possible domains of reactivity

were assessed: drinking behavior, levels of motivation to change alcohol use patterns,

self-efficacy reports, drinking urges, and self-reported reactivity.

Behavioral reactivity. This study examined the magnitude of change in self-

reported drinking behavior between pre- to post-EMA monitoring. The historic literature

suggests that monitoring an addictive behavior will decrease the frequency of its

occurrence (e.g., Abrams & Wilson, 1979; Fremouw & Brown, 1980). Collins et al.

(1998) found support for this hypothesis in a study of heavy drinkers recruited from the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 6

community. Study participants tended to report slight decreases in their weekly

consumption of alcohol (Collins et al., 1998). Based on this evidence, it was

hypothesized that EMA participants in the present study would report decreased quantity

and frequency of alcohol consumption during monitoring compared to their pre­

monitoring, baseline levels of consumption.

Motivational reactivitv. This study also examined changes in motivation to

change. Motivation to change has been found to correlate with self-awareness

(Prochaska, Velicer, DiClemente, & Fava, 1988). The Transtheoretical Model of Change

(Prochaska et al., 1992) suggests that increasing information about self and one’s

problem and assessing how one feels and thinks about oneself with respect to a problem

(e.g., self-monitoring) can be an important predictor of change. Based on this premise, it

was hypothesized that participants in an EMA protocol would demonstrate forward

movement on pre- to post-reports of their readiness to change. Specifically, it was

hypothesized that EMA participants would increase their Contemplation scores, and

decrease their Precontemplation scores, as a function of real-time monitoring.

Furthermore, it was predicted that there would be a significant increase in mean

Readiness to Change (RtC) scores, a composite index of motivation for change, from pre-

to post-monitoring.

Self-Efficacy Reactivitv. This study also explored change in alcohol self-efficacy

expectancies; in other words, one’s confidence in one’s ability to resist heavy drinking.

Based on the empirical literature relating self-monitoring to increases in problem

awareness (Prochaska et al., 1992; Litt et al., 1998), it was hypothesized that participants

in an EMA protocol would report decreased self-efficacy expectancies after completing

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 7

the real-time monitoring protocol. That is, once participants monitor their drinking

patterns in near real-time, they will be more likely to report decreased confidence in their

ability to resist heavy drinking.

Urge reactivity. The magnitude and direction of change in alcohol urge as a

function of completing the real-time assessments is also of interest. Current theories of

urges suggest that they are supported by effortful, controlled cognitive processing

(Tiffany, 1990). Researchers have confirmed that urges require attentional resources

(e.g., Sayette & Hufford, 1994; Sayette & Parrott, 1999). Furthermore, Litt et al. (1998)

reported that their alcoholic participants reported decreased urges to drink while self­

monitoring. By completing a real-time interview it was hypothesized that EMA

participants would sustain a reallocation of their attentional resources from the urge itself

to completing the real-time reports. That is, participants were signaled to complete an

urge report at the beginning and end of five daily assessments. It was hypothesized that

urge magnitude would decrease between the 1*‘ and 2"** urge report within each

assessment.

Self-reported reactivitv. This study explored participants’ accounts of the impact

monitoring had on their drinking behavior. Litt et al. (1998) explored self-reported

reactivity to EMA among drinkers. In post-monitoring interviews they found that

alcoholic participants reported that the monitoring protocol made them more aware of

their drinking and consequently made them drink less. However, when compared with a

no-monitoring control group, reported drinking behavior did not significantly change.

Based on their data, it was hypothesized that after completing the EMA protocol

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 8

participants would self-report decreased drinking quantity and frequency relative to their

pre-monitoring baseline alcohol consumption.

Methods

Participants

A total of 389 (39.6% female and 20.7 (SD = 4.4] years of age) Psychology 100

students were screened for this study. Participants scoring 10 or greater (N = 148, 38%)

on the Alcohol Use Disorders Inventory Test were eligible for participation (AUDIT;

Allen, Litten, Fertig, & Babor, 1997). AUDIT scores can range from 0 to 40.

Participants meeting inclusionary criteria were contacted by phone to inquire about their

participation in the real-time monitoring study. During this initial telephone contact

participants were given information about the study, including the use of the electronic

diaries (ED’s) for assessment. Thirty-eight participants were invited to attend the initial

interview and EMA training session. Thirty-three participants completed this study. Five

participants withdrew or were removed from the study as a result of not showing up for

the training session (n = 2), an inability to integrate ED into their lives (n = 2), or losing

the Palm Pilot (n = 1). All participants received full experimental credit for their

Psychology 100 class requirement.

Design and Procedures

Participants at the psychology 100 screening who met inclusionary criteria for the

present study were asked to the Addictive Behaviors Research Laboratory (ABRL) for an

interview and EMA training session. Prior to completing any questionnaires, participants

signed an informed consent form. At that time, they completed a series of questionnaires

(see Table 1) and participated in an interview session with either a graduate student or

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 9

Ph.D. in Clinical Psychology. Two interviews, the Addiction Severity Index (ASI;

McLellan et al., 1992) and the mood disorders and alcohol use disorders modules of the

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders-I (SCID-I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, &

Williams, 1997) were administered. Participants received 1 to 1 16 hours of group EMA

training. The training included instruction on the use of the electronic diary (ED),

including how to record drinks and temptations to drink, how to respond to signal-

contingent assessments, and how to use the sleep/wake, nap, and other functions.

Participants were given the opportunity to practice the electronic interviews and to ask

questions regarding the ED and monitoring protocol.

Once mastery of the ED had been demonstrated by the participant’s successfully

navigating the electronic questionnaires, each participant was given his or her own ED

and instructed to return to the ABRL after 3 days of self-monitoring. This session was

scheduled to assess initial compliance with monitoring and answer questions that arose as

a function of monitoring in the field. Two subsequent meetings were scheduled with

each participant. These were conducted to continue monitoring compliance and upload

data from each ED to a main computer. Sessions were scheduled for one and two weeks

after intake into the study. During the final upload, participants completed post­

monitoring questionnaires (see Table 1). Participants were then debriefed and given

appropriate referrals if they were interested in talking with someone about their drinking.

Finally, participants were given their experimental credits.

Timeline. This study ran participants in four waves of monitoring. Each

participant’s timeline approximated the following (see table 1):

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 10

Table 1. Timeline and Self-Report Measures.

DAY STUDY DESCRIPTION OF EVENT EVENT

Psychology 100 1) Psychology 100 students were group screened Screening with the Alcohol Use Disorders Inventory Test (AUDIT).

Phone 1) Review demographics and data from screening confirmation interview. --- with prospective 2) Describe study including EMA protocol. study 3) Inquire about their continued participation in the participants study. 4) Schedule for intake interview and EMA training.

1 ) Participants arrive at the Addictive Behaviors Research Laboratory (ABRL). 2) Participants sign informed consent form and Intake Interview complete Timeline Follow-Back (TLFB), 1 and Situational Confidence Questionnaire (SCQ), EMA Training Alcohol Expectancy Questionnaire III (AEQ), the University of Rhode Island Change Assessment Scale (URICA), and the Social Desirability Scale. 3) Participants interviewed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (mood and substance use modules; SCID-I) and the Alcohol Severity Index (ASI). 4) Participants receive training and given ED.

1 ) Participants return to ABRL and meet with 4 Shakedown research assistant to monitor compliance with ED. 2) Participants sent back into the field and instructed to continue self-monitoring.

1 ) Participants return to ABRL and meet with 8 Data Upload research assistant to upload data from ED to main computer. 2) Participants return to the field for final week of monitoring.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 11

1 ) Participants return to laboratory to return ED. 15 2"*^ Data Upload 2) Participants complete post-monitoring TLFB, URICA, and AEQ-III.

Measures

The study employed standard measures of substance use, psychiatric symptoms,

and trait characteristics important in the study of self-report. These data allowed us to

examine individual characteristics that may influence reactivity

Alcohol Use Disorders Inventorv Test. The Alcohol Use Disorders Inventory

Test-Core Instrument (AUDIT; Connigrave, Saunders, & Reznik, 1995) is a widely used

self-report measure of alcohol consumption. The measure requires 5 minutes to

administer and was used to screen participants for hazardous alcohol use (Allen et al.,

1997). This study used a cutoff score of 10 (range = 0 to 40), which has been found to be

a valid and reliable predictor of harmful and hazardous alcohol use. Saunders, Ashland,

Babor, de la Puente, & Grant (1993) demonstrated that when a cutoff score of 10 was

used, the overall sensitivity of the AUDIT for hazardous drinking was 80% and the

overall specificity was 98%. Other researchers have replicated these findings (Fleming,

Barry, & MacDonald, 1991; Claussen & Aasland, 1993). The measure contains 10 items

that ask, for example, “How often do you have a drink containing alcohol?” and “Has a

relative or friend or doctor or other health worker been concerned about your drinking or

suggested you cut down?”

Demographic Data Sheet. This form gathered basic demographic data including

age, sex, ethnicity, and year in school.

Addiction Severity Index. The Addiction Severity Index (ASI; McLellan, et al.,

1992) is a widely used measure in addictive behavior research (Ball & Corty, 1988;

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 12

Gawin et al., 1989; Kadden, Cooney, Getter, & Litt, 1990). Average overall reliability of

trained technicians administering the ASI in person is .89 and .83 or higher for each of

the seven scales (McLellan et al., 1985). The ASI uses 57 items to assess an individuals’

recent status (the 30 days just prior to the interview) in 7 categories relating to addiction

severity. These categories include medical (e.g., “How many days have you experienced

medical problems in the past 30 days?’’), emnlovment (e.g., “How many days were you

paid for work in the past 30 days?”), alcohol problems (e.g., “Alcohol-any use in the past

30 days?”), drug use (e.g., “Heroin, Cocaine, etc., any use at all in the past 30 days?”),

legal issues (e.g., “Are you currently awaiting charges; trial or sentence?"), family

problems (e.g., “How many days in the past 30 have you had serious conflicts with your

family?”), and psychiatric status (e.g., “In the past 30 days, have you had a significant

period [that was not a direct result of your drug/alcohol use] in which you have

experienced serious depression?”).

Timeline Follow-Back. The Timeline Follow-Back (TLFB; Sobell & Sobell,

1992) is a sensitive retrospective measure of drinking behavior. It has well established

psychometric qualities as a method for evaluating drinking quantity and frequency. For

example, Sobell, Sobell, Klajner, Pavan, & Basian (1986) demonstrated a test-retest

reliability of .92 to .96 among a college age population. Similar findings have been

demonstrated across a variety of populations including inpatient and outpatient alcoholics

(Maisto, Sobell, Connors, & Sobell, 1979). O’Farrell, Cutter, Bayog, Dentch, &

Fortgang (1984) compared collateral reports of a subjects drinking and found very high

correlations for total drinking days (r = .88). Finally, Sobell et al. (1986) demonstrated

strong concurrent validity of the TLFB by comparing it with established measures of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 13

alcohol related disabilities (Alcohol Dependence Scale, ADS; Short Michigan Alcohol

Screening Test, SMAST). As predicted, higher scores on the ADS and SMAST were

positively and significantly correlated with TLFB alcohol quantity and frequency

variables.

Using the TLFB method, participants are asked to provide estimates of their

drinking quantity and frequency over a designated length of time (the 30 days prior to the

interview in this study). The measure uses a calendar and a standard drink card to allow

the researchers to equate different alcoholic beverages with ethanol content. In this way,

researchers are able to standardize the amount of alcohol consumed across participants.

Situational Confidence Questionnaire. The Situational Confidence Questionnaire

(SCQ; Annis & Davis, 1988) is a 39-item measure of how confident an individual feels

regarding their ability to successfully resist the urge to drink alcohol in response to a

variety of stimuli (e.g., stress at work, fight with spouse, etc.). Annis & Graham (1988)

provide support for the construct validity of the SCQ. These authors correlated SCQ

subscale scores (situation-specific confidence levels; e.g., dealing with unpleasant

emotions/frustrations) with measures of alcohol consumption. They concluded that the

more drinking reported by individuals, the less likely they will be to endorse high

confidence levels in their ability to resist the urge to drink across all categories of

drinking situations. Furthermore, Annis & Graham (1988) provide evidence for the

reliability of the SCQ and conclude that individuals tend to respond consistently to the

items within each subscale. SCQ global scores have been foimd to be strong predictors

of alcohol treatment outcome (Greenfield, Hufford et al., 2000). Examples of SCQ items

include, “I would be able to resist the urge to drink heavily if other people didn’t seem to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 14

like me” and “I would be able to resist the urge to drink heavily if I felt that I had let

myself down.” Participants respond using a 6-point scale that ranges from 0% (not at all

confident) to 100% (very confident).

Alcohol Expectancv Ouestionnaire-III. Alcohol expectancies have been found to

be powerful predictors of alcohol use and abuse (Marlatt & Rohsenow, 1980). The

Alcohol Expectancy Questionnaire (AEQ-III; George et al., 1995) is a 39-item

measurement of alcohol related outcome expectancies. Although the AEQ can be broken

down into 8 expectancy subscales (global positive, social and physical pleasure, social

expressiveness, sexual enhancement, power and aggression, tension reduction and

relaxation, cognitive and physical impairment, and careless unconceml. this study

focused on the two general categories (positive and negative expectancies) as predictor

variables. These two categories provide a more conceptually distinct representation of

beliefs about alcohol and avoid analytic and psychometric difficulties (e.g.,

multicollinearity) that research with the eight subscales generally encounters (George et

al., 1995). This measure has participants respond on a 6-point scale ranging from “Agree

Strongly” to “Disagree Strongly.” For example, participants are asked to respond to

items such as, “Drinking makes the future seem brighter to me” or “Alcohol makes me

careless about my actions.”

University of Rhode Island Change Assessment Scale. The University of Rhode

Island Change Assessment Scale (URICA; McCoimaughy, Prochaska, & Velicer, 1983)

is a 32-item measure of motivation to change a problem behavior. A follow-up study to

this original research (McCoimaughy, DiClemente, Prochaska, & Velicer, 1989)

confirmed that the URICA reliably measures 4 distinct stages. These stages include

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 15

Precontemplation. Contemplation. Action, and Maintenance. Participants are asked

questions like, “As far as Tm concerned, I don’t have any alcohol problems that need

changing” and “It might be worthwhile to work on my alcohol problem.” Participants

respond on a 5-point scale (1 = Strongly Disagree to 5 = Strongly Agree). Factor scores

from the URICA place individuals into one of these stages of change which are thought

to improve prediction of the probability that one is likely to change the identified problem

behavior (e.g., alcohol use; Prochaska et al., 1992). Recently, researchers have been

using a readiness to change (RtC) index score by adding scores from the Contemplation.

Action, and Maintenance subscales of the URICA and subtracting Precontemplation

factor scores. The resulting RtC index provides a single continuous measure of readiness

to change (DiClemente, et al., 1999).

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders-I. The Structured Clinical

Interview for DSM-IV Disorders-I (SCID-I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997) is

a semi-structured clinical interview that allows for assessment of DSM-IV Axis I clinical

disorders. Test-retest reliability studies have indicated that the SCID-I is at least as good

as other diagnostic instruments in obtaining stable results (Williams et al. 1992). Results

on the validity of the SCID-I remain elusive because of the difficulty in the procedures

for comparing diagnoses across measures and standards. However, Kranzler, Kadden,

Babor, Tennen, & Rounsaville (1995) were able to demonstrate excellent validity in a

study of SCID-I diagnosed substance abusers compared with a standard clinical intake

interview. Participants in the present study were administered the alcohol use and mood

disorders modules of the SCID-I.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 16

Social Desirability Scale. The Social Desirability scale (SD; Edwards, 1970) is

39-item measure thought to discriminate individuals who are, more or less, attempting to

represent themselves in socially desirable ways. Items are endorsed as either true or false

and include such statements as, “I find it hard to keep my mind on a task or job” and “I

am happy most of the time.”

Electronic Diarv Debriefing Sheet. The Electronic Diary (ED) debriefing sheet

was utilized post-EMA monitoring in order to obtain information on how participants

perceived the EMA monitoring protocol and its effect on their behavior.

Real-time monitoring

Participants were trained to use the ED, a Palm Pilot Professional Edition (by

3COM) palmtop computer, for data collection. The ED is small (11.8cm X 7.7cm X

1.5cm), lightweight (234 grams including batteries and case), and includes an illuminated

liquid crystal display (6cm X 6cm), speaker, real-time clock, and calendar. It runs on 2

AAA batteries and uploads its data to a notebook computer via a Palm Pilot Cradle

attached to a serial port. All data receives a date and time stamp. In addition, the

software flags non-responses to prompts, allowing for checks regarding participants’

compliance with monitoring. The software has proven itself to be stable in past studies

(Shiffman, Enberg, et al., 1997), and the hardware itself is durable (e.g., one participant

accidentally dropped her Palm Pilot into a cooler of water; after drying out the data was

captured and the unit was sent back out into the field!).

The use of EMA requires careful attention to the type of sampling used (Stone &

Shiffman, 1994). Therefore, participants were instructed on how to respond to signal-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 17

and time-contingent sampling schedules as well as on how to initiate event-contingent

records.

Signal-contingent sampling included computer-generated random prompts that

signal the participant via a “beep” to complete a report an average of 5 times per day.

The signal- contingent interviews included items assessing activities (e.g., work, leisure,

interacting with others), setting (e.g., home, work-place), mood (10 positive and negative

affect mood adjectives drawn from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; Watson,

Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), and alcohol urges. The urge assessment at the beginning of the

signal-contingent assessment asked participants “Urge to drink?” The end assessment of

alcohol urge was drawn from the Alcohol Urge Questionnaire, Bohn, Krahn, & Staehler,

1995, and asked, “All 1 want to do now is have a drink?” This item had the highest factor

loading on the AUQ. Participants could drag a visual marker up and down a 1-11 scale to

record their responses to both urge questions.

Time-contingent sampling included regular morning and evening reports.

Morning reports included items assessing sleep quality and current mood. Evening

reports included items summarizing their mood for the day and the occurrence of any

positive or negative life events. An event-contingent schedule was also implemented to

encourage participants to report any subjective change in their temptation to drink alcohol

or if any alcohol is consumed (including the quantity consumed, triggers for drinking,

and mood pre- and post-drinking). Participants were instructed to complete the

temptation or drink reports after these episodes were over. These near real-time

assessments included activity, setting, mood, and coping questions, as well as questions

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 18

regarding triggers for the temptation or drinking episode. The types of sampling

contingencies and domains assessed are presented in Table 2.

TYPE OF MOOD ACTIVITIES TRIGGERS URGE RtC COPING CONSUME CONTINGENCY 1 Signal XX X Event: Temptation XXXXXX Event: Drinking XXXXX X X Time (morning & XX XX evening report) 1 Readiness to change items

As part of the EMA monitoring, participants were expected to return to the

laboratory on three separate occasions to upload the real-time data onto a main computer

and receive feedback from research assistants regarding their compliance. Following the

completion of the EMA protocol, each participant received a short debriefing

questionnaire focused on obtaining subjective experience data on the real-time

monitoring.

Data Management

Use of EMA methodology creates very large and complex data sets. The 33

participants who completed this 2-week monitoring protocol yielded a data set with 167

drinking episodes, 248 temptations to drink, the completion of 1926 signal-contingent

assessments, and a total of 462 person-days of monitoring. A data interpreter developed

by Saul Shiffman and the Smoking Research Group at the University of Pittsburgh was

used to upload and store the ED data. This interpreter creates text files ready to be

imported into SAS for Windows (v6.12) for analysis.

Data Analysis

This examination of reactivity focused on three types of data. First, within-

participant changes over the course of the two-week EMA monitoring protocol were

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 19

explored in the areas of drinking behavior, motivation for change, and alcohol self-

efficacy expectancies. Evidence for mean reactivity across participants was tested by

using common parametric within-participant analyses. Second, the conceptual

implications of participant characteristics influencing reactivity in the absence of mean

group-level reactive effects are also of interest. This exploratory study sought better

understanding of which participant characteristics predicted reactivity even in the absence

of group level effects (Fremouw & Brown, 1980). The following sets of predictors were

used in stepwise multiple regression equations to examine participant characteristics that

could influence reactivity: addiction severity (ASI alcohol use subscale and number of

DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence criteria met), alcohol outcome expectancies

(global positive and negative alcohol expectancies from AEQ-III at intake), alcohol self-

efficacy expectancies (global scores from SCQ), readiness to change (each factor score-

Precontemplation, Contemplation, Action, and Maintenance-from the URICA at Time 1),

and social desirability. For each regression equation, demographic variables (age and

gender) were forced into the equation prior to the stepwise procedure in order to hold

constant the variability in the criterion variable associated with age and gender.

Finally, reactivity to each signal-contingent assessment of alcohol urge was

addressed. Consistent with contemporary cognitive models of urges suggesting that they

are governed by effortful cognitive processing (Tiffany, 1990; Sayette & Hufford, 1994),

it was hypothesized that the magnitude of urge at Time 1 within each signal-contingent

assessment would predict a greater decrease in urge magnitude. That is, responding to

the prompt would require an expenditure of attentional cognitive resources leading to a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 20

decrease in reported urge. Therefore, the higher the urge measurement in Time 1 the

greater the reduction between Time 1 and Time 2 alcohol urges.

Generalized estimating equations (GEE) are ideally suited for data analysis

involving repeated measures designs with correlated data. A primary convenience of the

GEE method is that it is capable of appropriately acconunodating an unequal number of

repeated observations that are correlated over time (see, e.g., Zeger & Liang, 1986).

Before using GEE, it is important to determine which correlation structure best

approximates the relationship between the within-participant variable of interest. Three

correlation structures have been considered in EMA studies: independence, exchangeable

correlation, and discretization of an exponentially decaying model or autoregressive

correlation structure. An independence correlation structure makes the assumption that

every individual observation is completely uncorrelated with all other observations within

that same individual. That is, all within-individual observations are independent of all

others. Within the current analysis, it is unreasonable to assume that observations within

participants will be independent of each other. An exchangeable correlation structure

assumes that every observation within an individual is equally correlated with every other

observation in that same individual, regardless of the amount of time separating the

observations. Finally, the autoregressive model makes the assumption that any two

observations within an individual will have a stronger correlation the closer they are

taken over time. Furthermore, this model assumes that the strength of the relationship

between within-individual observations will decay over time. Similar to other

researchers, we used an exchangeable correlation model (e.g., Collins et al., 1998;

Shiffinan, Enberg, et al., 1997). Conceptually, an exponentially decaying model appears

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 21

to better approximate within-person variables over time; however, the model assumes

observations are equally spaced in time, which simplifies the model to a first-order

autoregressive (ARl) correlation structure. Our study employed computer generated

random signal-contingent assessments, so we expected to observe a wide spectrum of

time duration between assessments. The ARl model cannot account for these

continuous-time variables. It is important to note that findings from GEE have been

found to be robust even when different types of correlational models are specified (Zegar

& Liang, 1992).

Results

Participants

Participants who completed monitoring (n = 33, 53% female) averaged 19.8 (SD

= 2.5) years of age. Ethnicity was 97% Caucasian and 3% Native American. Participants

reported consuming 48.8 (SD = 38.7) drinks in 9.4 (SD = 5.6) days of drinking in the 30

days prior to the interview. Participants endorsed an average of 2.1 (SD = 1.4) alcohol

abuse symptoms and 1.7 (SD =1.6) alcohol dependence symptoms from the SCID-I

interview. Participants scored .16 (SD = .10) on the alcoholism subscale of the ASI. On

the depression module of the SCID, participants endorsed an average of 2.0 (SD = 2.4)

symptoms. Twenty-four participants met DSM-IV (APA, 1994) criteria for alcohol

abuse, four met DSM-IV criteria for alcohol dependence, and five met DSM-IV criteria

for major depression.

Compliance with Monitoring

Participants were remarkably compliant with the monitoring. For example,

participants responded to 86% of the 2248 random prompts administered over the 2-week

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 22

monitoring protocol. These results are parallel to recent EMA studies by Collins et al.

(1998) and Stone et al. (1997) that report overall participant compliance to random

prompts as 85% and 90%, respectively. In the present study, social desirability scores

did not predict compliance as measured by the total number of missed random prompts (r

= .18,p=.35).

Behavioral Reactivitv

Behavioral reactivity was examined by contrasting participants’ real-time reports

of drinking behavior to their data from the 30-day TLFB administered pre-monitoring.

The direction of change suggested a decrease in both mean number of drinks per week

(M = 1.8 ^ = 8.1) and drinking days (M = . 1 ^ = 1.2); however, a paired comparison

t-test yielded no significant differences for either drinks per week or days drinking per

week. Real-time reports of drinks per week (r = .86, p < .001) and drinking days (r = .84,

P < .001) were significantly correlated with the two-week timeline follow-back data.

We examined whether the participant characteristics outlined above would predict

behavioral reactivity using a stepwise multiple regression analysis. After controlling for

age and gender, participants’ Action scores from the URJCA were inversely related to

behavioral reactivity (partial r^ = .44, t = 3.1, p = .01). Participants who reported they

were actively trying to ameliorate an alcohol problem were less likely to demonstrate

behavioral reactivity by reducing their average drinks per week. Alcohol use subscale

scores from the ASI were marginally related to behavioral reactivity (partial if = .30, t =

2.2, p = .054) such that participants reporting more alcohol problems in the 30 days prior

to the onset of the study were somewhat less likely to reduce their drinking during

monitoring than those participants reporting less of a problem with alcohol.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 23

Motivational Reactivitv

At intake to the study, participants’ scores on the URICA indicated that 46% were

in Precontemplation. 33% were in Contemplation, and 21% were in the Action stages of

change. This distribution remained largely unchanged at the end of the study with 49%,

42%, and 9% of participants falling into the Precontemplation. Contemplation, and

Action stages of change, respectively. No participants were identified as being in the

Maintenance stage of change at either assessment.

Motivational reactivity was examined in two ways. First, each of the four factors

from the URICA were analyzed using a repeated measures MANOVA. This 2 (Time) X

4 (Factor: Precontemplation. Contemplation. Action, and Maintenance) repeated

measures MANOVA demonstrated that the main effect for Time approached significance

(F [1,32] = 3.95, p = .055). The analysis also illustrated a main effect for Factor (F [3,30]

= 39.96, p < .0001). The Time X Factor interaction was not significant (F [3,30] = 2.0, g

= .13).

Next, the URICA data was analyzed using the readiness to change (RtC) index

score. A repeated measures ANOVA with the RtC index as the dependent measure

revealed a main effect for Time (F [1,32] = 5.5, g = .03). Participants RtC index score

decreased between pre- (M = 40.7, ^ - 15.2) and post-monitoring (M = 32.1, SD =

20.3).

In order to examine whether individual differences related to motivational

reactivity, a stepwise multiple regression analysis was conducted using the predictors

outlined above (excluding the readiness to change factors themselves) and change on the

RtC index as the dependent variable. This analysis revealed no significant effects.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 24

Self-EfTicacv Reactivitv

First, SCQ total scores were examined for change between pre- and post­

monitoring. This within-participant ANOVA revealed no effect for Time (F [1,24] = .56,

p = .46). Participants’ global alcohol self-efficacy scores did not differ between Time 1

(M = 65.9, SD = 17.8) and Time 2(M = 64.3, SD = 16.6).

A stepwise multiple regression analysis with the variables outlined above as

predictors and change score on the SCQ as the dependent measure revealed that, after

controlling for gender and age, the Precontemplation factor from the URICA predicted

change in alcohol self-efficacy expectancies (partial r^= .64, t = 6.8, p = .001). The

stronger the participants identified themselves as having no desire to change an alcohol

problem, the more likely they were to increase their self-efficacy expectancies as a

function of the real-time monitoring. Total negative alcohol expectancies approached

significance in predicting change in alcohol self-efficacy expectancies (partial r^ = .33, t =

2.2, p = .052). Participants with strong negative alcohol outcome expectancies were

more likely to demonstrate a decrease in their alcohol self-efficacy expectancies over the

course of the study.

Urge Reactivitv

For each participant, we created a change score computed as the difference

between their self-reported urge at the beginning (T1 ; M = 3.86, ^ = 1.3) and end (T2;

M = 3.57, SD = 1.3) of the signal-contingent assessments. Participants urge scores

significantly decreased during the signal-contingent assessment (M = 29, ^ = .60, t [32]

= 2.8, p = .01). In addition, an Urge Change Ratio (UCR) was created by using the T1

urge in the numerator and the T2 urge in the denominator. For example, if a participant

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 25

endorsed a T1 urge magnitude of 10 and T2 urge magnitude of 5, then the UCR would be

2. Similarly, if a participant endorsed an urge magnitude of 5 at both T1 and T2, the

UCR would then be I, To provide a more stable test of change, a t-test comparing the

mean UCR with a test value = 1 was performed. This also resulted in a significant

change score (M = 1.28, SD = .33, t [32] = 4.9, p < .001).

Moreover, generalized estimating equations (Zeger & Liang, 1986) using an

exchangeable correlational model found that the magnitude of the urge at T1 was

positively related to urge reactivity (0 = .34, Z = 13.7, g < .001). That is, the greater the

initial urge to drink at the outset of the signal-contingent assessment, the greater the urge

reduction at T2. It was also examined whether urge reactivity would change over time in

the study and found this not to be the case = -.003, Z = -.68, g = .50). Participants’

urge reactivity showed no signs of increasing or decreasing over the 2-week real-time

monitoring protocol.

In order to examine whether participant characteristics influenced the mean level

of urge reduction across participants, we ran a stepwise multiple regression analysis with

the mean change urge for each participant across the random prompts as the dependent

measure. This analysis revealed no significant predictors for change in urge. However,

when using the UCR as the criterion variable and controlling for age and gender, total

negative outcome expectancies from the AEQ-111 (partial r^ = .46, t = 3.0, g = .011) and

the Maintenance stage of change (partial r^ = .38, t = -2.58, g = .026) predicted urge

reactivity. Participants who strongly endorsed negative outcome expectancies were more

likely to demonstrate urge reactivity while items on the URICA indicating past successful

changes in alcohol behavior predicted a decrease in urge reactivity.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 26

Self-Reported Reactivitv

At the end of the study, participants completed a measure assessing their

subjective impressions of whether EMA affected their drinking. Twenty-nine percent of

participants (n = 9) believed that the real-time monitoring caused them to decrease their

drinking while 10% (n = 3) reported that monitoring caused them to increase their

drinking. Sixty-one percent (n = 19) of participants reported that the real-time

monitoring did not affect their drinking behavior. For those reporting decreased drinking,

the monitoring had an estimated average impact of 5.7 (SD = 1.8) on an 11-point scale (0

= no impact to 10 = a great deal of impact). The participants who indicated monitoring

caused them to increase their drinking estimated the impact to be a 4.7 (SD = 1.5) on an

11-point scale. Of all the participants who thought that real-time monitoring affected

their drinking, either an increase or decrease (n = 12, 39%), also demonstrated behavioral

reactivity for both drinks per week (r = -.70, g = .01) and days drinking per week (r = -

.69, g = .01). The more the participant acknowledged a reactive decrease in their alcohol

consumption as a result of monitoring, the greater the decrease in self-reported drinking

over the course of the study.

Discussion

This study examined five types of reactivity to EMA using a sample of

undergraduate problem drinkers. It was hypothesized that participants’ real-time reports

would produce significantly less days drinking per week and number of drinks consumed

per week compared to their retrospective reports for the 30-days before the study (Collins

et al., 1998; Litt et al., 1998). Real-time monitoring of drinking behavior for short

periods (i.e., 2-weeks) failed to produce significant overall changes in drinking quantity

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 27

and frequency. Evidence for behavioral reactivity producing decreased drinking during

real-time monitoring has been shown in studies using longer monitoring protocols (e.g.,

Collins et al., 1998). This suggests that the short monitoring protocol used in this study

may have been partially responsible for the lack of behavioral reactivity.

The majority of our participants did not report that the monitoring affected their

drinking behavior. The minority (39%) of participants who believed that monitoring

affected their drinking also showed changes in their EMA-assessed drinking. This is a

marked departure from the findings of Litt et al. (1998), who found that fully 70% of

their alcoholic participants self-reported decrease in drinking due to monitoring but

demonstrated no significant change when their actual drinking data was compared to a

non-monitoring group. This contrast in findings is likely a function of the significant

differences between the two samples (e.g., addiction severity, motivation for change).

However, researchers interested in reactivity should consider these divergent findings

prior to assessing various alcohol using populations. Further research into the agreements

and discrepancies between participant perceptions versus the actual impact EMA has on

drinking behavior as measured in real-time is warranted.

Contrary to the hypothesized relationship between monitoring and readiness to

change, participants were slightly 1 ^ likely to endorse being ready to change their

drinking behavior after completing the real-time monitoring protocol. This was most

evident in the analysis using the RtC index score (DiClemente et al., 1999). The majority

of our undergraduate sample, however, viewed their drinking as non-problematic, despite

scoring as hazardous drinkers on the AUDIT and consuming approximately five drinks

per drinking occasion. It is unknown whether participants seeking help for an alcohol

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 28

problem would show a similar pattern of motivational reactivity to EMA. It could be the

case that participants in the contemplation or action stages of change would manifest

greater therapeutic reactivity to EMA (Abrams & Wilson, 1979).

We found no evidence of self-efficacy reactivity as a function of the real-time

monitoring. Alcohol self-efficacy expectancies have been shown to change over the

course of 4- week treatment (e.g., Brown, Carrello, Vik, & Porter, 1998). Somewhat

surprisingly, this group of undergraduate drinkers scored relatively low on the SCQ,

averaging approximately 65% confidence to resist heavy drinking in a variety of

situations. This is remarkable in that SCQ global scores less than 50% have been found

to predict relapse among inpatient alcoholics (Greenfield, Hufford et al., 2000).

However, less is known about the stability of alcohol self-efficacy expectancies among

participants not involved in alcoholism treatment.

Our findings regarding urge reactivity were consistent with cognitive models of

urges that suggest they require effortful cognitive processing (Tiffany, 1990; Sayette &

Hufford, 1994). The data revealed that the magnitude of alcohol urge assessed at the

beginning of each signal-contingent assessment decreased over the course of the

assessment. Furthermore, this lower level in self-reported alcohol urge was greater in

subjects with greater levels in pre-assessment urge. This finding may have clinical

implications for studies using EMA to produce and/or maintain therapeutic effects. It

may be possible to program palmtop computers to magnify the reductions in urge

magnitude observed in the present study. However, this finding is also consistent with

regression to the mean effects; as alcohol urges at T1 increased, greater opportunity

existed for them to decrease at T2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 29

Like other addiction researchers with an interest in reactivity (Fremouw & Brown,

1980), we examined a variety of individual difference characteristics, including

demographic factors, addiction severity, readiness to change, alcohol outcome and self-

efficacy expectancies, and social desirability as influences on reactivity. The results

demonstrated that even in the absence of mean group-level reactive effects, certain

participant characteristics were related to reactivity.

Factor scores on the URICA (i.e., stages) predicted behavioral, self-efficacy, and

urge reactivity. Participants who reported they were taking some Action to change an

alcohol problem were less likely to decrease their drinking behavior simply as a function

of monitoring. Similarly, participants who resisted acknowledging a problem with

alcohol (Precontemplation) were more likely to show increased confidence in their ability

to resist heavy drinking in high-risk situations after the real-time monitoring. This

suggests that real-time monitoring may serve as an arena for precontemplators to test (in

this case, successfully) their alcohol self-efficacy expectancies. Further, individuals

endorsing URICA items associated with the Maintenance stage of change (indicative of

successful application of past alcohol behavior change techniques) were less likely to

demonstrate urge reactivity. This implies that individuals with a history of alcohol

behavior change attempts are less likely to have their urges affected by real-time

monitoring. Taken together, these findings suggest that the motivation for change is an

important influence on reactivity.

This study has a number of limitations. First, by not having a no-monitoring

control group, these results should be viewed with caution. Typically, control groups

have not been included in real-time studies, but even if they were it is difficult to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 30

understand how their inclusion might yield strong evidence for or against reactive effects.

In the present study, it is possible that coming to a research laboratory and filling out self-

report measures and completing interviews could have been partially responsible for our

findings. Several issues mitigate this concern, including that test-retest studies of

questionnaires rarely find significant effects simply as a matter of administering the

measure. Moreover, the lack of behavioral reactivity indicates that most participants did

not significantly change their behavior as a result of completing the strongest test case:

self-report measures and a real-time monitoring protocol. Lastly, there is the question of

interpretation of data from a control group. Affleck et al. (1999) note that the inclusion

of any single control group to understand reactivity to EMA is unlikely to be helpful. For

example, what is the key variable being controlled: carrying the ED, being prompted by

ED, receiving feedback on ED compliance; recording drinking versus mood states or

other variables on ED?

Many studies using EMA in addictive behaviors research focus on treatment-

seeking populations (e.g., Collins et al., 1998; Litt et al., 1998; Shiffman et al., 1997). It

is unknown whether the general absence of reactive effects observed among these

undergraduate problem drinkers would also be absent in a treatment-seeking sample. The

relatively short monitoring protocol used in this study also differentiates it from studies

using 4- to 8-week monitoring protocols (e.g., Collins et al., 1998; Shiffman et al.,

1997b). It is possible that increased or decreased reactive effects could emerge in studies

using longer monitoring protocols. However, time trends in the data can be explained by

factors other than reactivity and must be examined carefully. For example, participants

in this study were remarkably compliant to a demanding EMA protocol (e.g., participants

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 31

responded to 86% of the random prompts). However, longer monitoring periods could

show decreased compliance over time. Still, the high correlation between the real-time

and retrospective reports of drinking behavior on the TLFB suggest that our participants

were not only compliant but also relatively accurate in terms of their self-report reported

quantity and frequency of alcohol consumption (Carney et al., 1998). This pattern of

compliance to a rigorous monitoring protocol is in agreement with finding from a number

of studies suggesting that participants’ momentary experiences can be reliably and

accurately measured using EMA (Shiffman & Stone, 1998).

This research has important implications, both for EMA researchers and the study

of self-report more generally. Although other EMA and self-monitoring studies have

addressed reactivity (e.g., Collins et al., 1998; Cruise et al., 1996, Sobell et al., 1989), this

is the first study to systematically examine reactivity as a multidimensional construct

involving behavioral, motivational, and cognitive components. Our findings indicate that

reactive effects to EMA are generally small in magnitude. What remains to be seen is

whether reactivity can be enhanced by using EMA for therapeutic gain. Additional

research is needed to more fully understand the reactive effects of EMA on treatment-

seeking populations. More specifically, research explicitly addressing the nature of

individual differences and how participant characteristics, such as motivation to change a

behavior, can influence the reactivity to EMA. Instead of viewing reactivity as a unitary

phenomenon, future research should continue to examine how intensive self-monitoring

can impact behavioral, cognitive and motivational variables important to the study of

addictive behaviors.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 32

References

Abrams, D. B & Wilson, G. T. (1979). Self-monitoring and reactivity in the

modification of cigarette smoking. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 47,

243-251.

Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Urrows, S., Higgens, P., Abeles, M., Hall, C., Karoly, P.,

& Newton, P. (1998). Fibromyalgia and women's pursuit of personal goals; A daily

process analysis. , 17. 40-47.

Affleck, G., Zautra, A., Tennen, H., & Armeli, S. (1999). Multilevel daily

process designs for consulting and clinical psychology: A preface for the perplexed.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 67, 746-754.

Allen, J.P., Litten, R.Z., Fertig, J.B., & Babor, T. (1997). A review of research on

the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT). Alcoholism: Clinical and

Experimental Research. 21. 613-619.

American Psychological Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual

of Mental Disorders (4**' Ed). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, D C.

Annis, H. M. & Davis, C. S. (1988). Assessment of expectancies. In D. M.

Donovan & G. A. Marlatt (Eds ), Assessment of Addictive Behaviors, pp.84-111. New

York: Guilford Press.

Annis, H. M. & Graham, J. M. Situational Confidence Questionnaire (SCQ-39)

User’s Guide, Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation, 1988.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 33

Ball, J.C. & Corty, E. (1988). Basic issues pertaining to the effectiveness of

methadone maintenance treatment. In Carl G. Lukefeld & Frank M. Times (Eds.)

Compulsory Treatment of Drus Abuse: Research and Clinical Practice. Rockville, MD:

National Institute on Drug Abuse.

Bandura, A., & Simon, K.M. (1977). The role of proximal intentions in self­

regulation of refractory behavior. Cognitive Research and Theranv. 1. 177-193.

Bellack, A.S., Rozensky, R. & Schwartz, J. (1974) A comparison of two forms of

self-monitoring in a weight reduction program. Behavioral Therapy. 5, 523-530.

Bohn, M. J ., Krahn, D. D., Staehler, B.A. (1995). Development and initial

validation of a measure of drinking urges in abstinent alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical

and Experimental Research. 19. 600-606.

Broden, M., Hall, R.V., Mitts, B. (1971). The effect of self-recording on the

classroom behavior of two eighth-grade students. Journal of Applied Behavioral

Analysis. 4, 191-199.

Brownell, K.D., Marlatt, G.A., Lichtenstein, E., & Wilson, G.T. (1986).

Understanding and preventing relapse. American Psychologist. 41. 765-782.

Burton, P., Gurrin, L., & Sly, P. (1998). Extending the simple linear regression

model to account for correlated responses: An introduction to generalized estimating

equations and multi-level mixed modeling. Statistics in Medicine. 17, 1261-1291.

Carney, M. A., Tennen, H., Affleck, G., Del Boca, F. K., & Kranzler, H. R.

(1998). Levels and patterns of alcohol consumption using timeline follow-back, daily

diaries and real-time “electronic interviews.” Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 59, 447-454.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 34

Claussen, B. & Aasland, O.G. (1993). The alcohol use disorders identification

test (AUDIT) in a routine health examination of long-term unemployed. Addiction. 88.

363-368.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2"*^

ed.y Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Collins, R.L., Morsheimer, E.T., Shiffman, S., Paty, J.A., Gnys, M., &

Papandonatos, G.D. (1998). Ecological momemtary assessment in behavioral drinking

moderation training program. Experimental and Clinical Psvchopharmacology. 6, 306-

315.

Connigrave, K.M., Saunders, J.B., & Reznik, R.B. (1995). Predictive capacity of

the AUDIT questionnaire for alcohol-related harm. Addiction. 90, 1479-1485.

Cruise, C.E., Broderick, J., Porter, L., Kaell, A., & Stone, A. (1996). Reactive

effects of diary self-assessment in chronic pain patients. International Association for the

Studv of Pain. 96. 253258.

Csikszentmuhalyi, M. & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the

experience-sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175. 526-534.

Delespaul, P. & deVries, M. (1987). The daily life of ambulatory chronic mental

patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 175. 537-534.

DiClemente, C.C., Daniels, J.W., Bellino, L.E., & Neavins, T.M. (1999).

Readiness to change and abstinence self-efficacy in alcoholism treatment. Symposia

presentation at the 33*^^ Annual Convention of the Association for the Advancement of

Behavior Therapy. Toronto, Canada.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 35

Edwards, A. L. (1970). The measurement of personality traits by scales and

inventories. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Fleming, M.F., Barry, K.L., & MacDonald, R. (1991). The alcohol use disorders

identification test (AUDIT) in a college sample. International Journal of Addictions. 26,

1173-1185.

First, M B., Spitzer, R.L., Gibbon, M, & Williams, J.B.W. (1997). User's Guide

for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disordrs-Clinical Version

(SCID-CV). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Fredriksen, L.W., Epstein, L.H., & Rosevsky, B.P. (1975). Reliability and

controlling effects of three procedures for self-monitoring smoking. Psychological

Record. 25, 255-264.

Fremouw, W.J. & Brown, Jr., J.P. (1980). The reactivity of addictive behaviors to

self-monitoring: A functional analysis. Addictive Behaviors. 5, 209-217.

Gawin, F.H., Kleber, H.D., Byck, R., Rounsaville, B.J., Kosten, T.R., Jatlow, P.I.,

& Morgan, C. (1989). Abstience symptomatology and psychiatric diagnosis in cocaine

abusers: Clinical observations. Archives of General Psychiatry. 43. 107-113.

George, W. H., Frone, M. R., Cooper, M.L., Russell, M., Skinner, J. B., &

Windle, M. (1995). A revised alcohol expectancy questionnaire: Factor structure

confirmation and invariance in a general population sample. Journal of Studies on

Alcohol. 56, 177-185.

Green, L. (1978). Temporal and stimulus dimensions of self-monitoring in the

treatment of obesity. Behavior Theranv. 9, 328-341.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 36

Greenfield, S., Hufford, M. R., Vogge, L, & Weiss, R. (2000). The relationship

of self-efficacy expectancies to relapse among alcohol dependent men and women: A

prospective study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 61, 345-351.

Hay, L.R., Hay, W.M., & Angle, H.V. (1977). The reactivity of self-recording: A

case report of a drug abuser. Behavior Therapy, 8, 1004-1007.

Hayes, S.C. & Cavoir, N. (1977). Multiple tracking and the reactivity of self­

monitoring: I. Negative behaviors. Behavior Therapy. 8, 819-831.

Hufford, M R., Kodya, S., Shields, A.L., Noice, A., Balabanis, M., Paty, J., &

Shiffman, S. (poster under review). Proximal determinants of temptations to drink

among alcoholics discharged from inpatient treatment: Application of real-time

monitoring methodology.

Johnson, C.L., & Larson, R. (1982). Bulimia: An analysis of moods and

behavior. Psvchomatic Medicine, 44, 341-351.

Kadden, R.M., Cooney, N.L., Getter, H., & Litt, M.D. (1990). Matching

alcoholics to coping skills or interactional therapies: Post treatment results. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 851-863.

Kamarck, T.W., Shiffman, S., Smithline, L., Goodie, J.L., Paty, J., Gnys, M., &

Yi-Kuan Jong, J. (1998). Effects of task strain, social conflict, and emotional activiation

on ambulatory cardiovascualr activity: Daily life consequences of recurring stress in a

multiethnic adult sample. Health Psychology. 17. 17-29.

Kanfer, F.H. (1975). Self-management methods. In F.H. Kanfer & A.P. Goldstein

(Ed's.), Helping People Change: A Textbook of Methods. New York: Pergamon Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 37

Karoly, P. & Doyle, W.W. (1975). Effects of outcome expectancy and timing of

self-monitoring on cigarette smokeing. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 351-355.

Kazdin, A. (1974a). Self-monitoring and behavior change. In M.J. Mahoney &

C.E. Thoresen (Ed's), Self-Control: Power to the Person. Monterey, California:

Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.

Kazdin, A. (1974b). Reactive self-monitoring: The effects of response

desirability, goal setting, and feedback. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,

42, 704-716.

Korotitsch, W.J. & Nelson-Gray, R.O. (1999). An overview of self-monitoring

research in assessment and treatment. Psychological Assessment, 11. 415-425.

Kozlowski, L.T. & Wilkinson, D A. (1987). Use and misuse of the concept of

craving by alcohol, tobacco, and drug researchers. British Journal of Addictions. 82, 31-

36.

Lam, A. L., Cole, C. L., Shapiro, E. S., & Bambara, L. M. (1994). Relative

effects of self-monitoring on-task behavior, academic accuracy, and disruptive behavior

in students with behavior disorders. Review. 23,44-58.

Larsen, R. J. & Diener, E. (1987). Affect intensity as an individual difference

characteristic: A review. Journal of research in Personality. 21. 1-39.

Larson, R. (1989). Beeping children and adolescents: A method for studying

time use and daily experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 18. 511-530.

Lipinski, D.P., Black, J.L., Nelson, R.O., & Ciminero, A.R. (1975). Influences on

motivational variables on the reactivity and reliablity of self-recording. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 43, 637-646.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 38

Litman, G., Stapleton, J., Oppenheim, A.N., Peleg, M., & Jackson, P. (1983).

Situations related to alcoholism relapse. British Journal of Addiction. 78, 381-389.

Litt, M.D., Cooney, N.L., & Morse, P. (1998). Ecological momentary assessment

(EMA) with treated alcoholics: Methodological problems and potential solutions. Health

Psychology. 17.48-52.

Magneberg, R. (1998). The effect of electronic suryeillance on alcohol misuse

and eyeryday behayior. Addictiye Behayiors. 23. 281-301.

Mahoney, M.J. (1974). Self-reward and selff-monitoring techniques for weight

control. Behayior Therapy. 5, 48-57.

Mahoney, M.J., Moore, B.S., Wade, T.C., & Moura, N.G.M. (1973). Effects of

continuous self-monitoring on academic behayior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology. 41,65-69.

Maisto, S.A., Sobell, M B., Cooper, A.M., & Sobell, L.C. (1979). Test-retest

reliability of retrospectiye self-reports in three populations of alcohol abusers. Journal of

Behayjoral Assessment, i, 315-326.

Marlatt, G.A. (1985). Coping and substance use; Implications for research,

preyention, and treatment. In S.Shiftman & T.A. Wills (Eds), Coping and Substance Use.

San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

Marlatt, G.A. & Gordon, J R. (1980). Determinants of relapse: Implications for

the maintenance of behayior change. In P.O. Dayidson & S.M. Dayidson (Eds.),

Behayioral Medicine: Changing Health Lifestyles. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.

Marlatt, G.A. & Gordon, J R. (1985). Relapse Preyention: Maintenance

Strategies in the treatment of addictiye behayiors. New York: Guilford Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 39

Marlatt, G.A. & Rohsenow, D.J. (1980). Cognitive processes in alcohol use:

Expectancy and the balanced placebo design: In Mello, N.K. (Ed.) Advances in

Substance Abuse: Behavioral and Biological. Vol. 1. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, Inc.

McConnaughy, E. A., DiClemente, C. C., Prochaska, J. O., Velicer, W. F.

(1989). Stages of change in psychotherapy: A follow-up report. Psychotherapy. 26, 494-

503.

McConnaughy, E.A., Prochask, J.O., & Velicer, W.F. (1983). Stages of change in

psychotherapy: Measurement and sample profiles. Psychotherapy: Theory. Research,

and Practice. 20, 368-375.

McFall, R.M. (1970). Effects of self-monitoring on normal smoking behavior.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 35. 135-142.

McFall, R.M. (1977). Parameters of self-monitoring. In R.B. Stuart (Ed ),

Behavioral Self-Management: Strategies. Techniques, and Outcomes. New York:

Brunner/Mazel, Publishers.

McFall, R.M. & Hammen, C.L. (1971). Motivation, structure, and self­

monitoring: Role of nonspecific factors in smoking reduction. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology. 37. 80-86.

McKay, J R. (1999). Studies of factors in relapse to alcohol, drug and nicotine

use: A critical review of methodologies and findings. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 67.

566-576.

McLellan, A., Kushner, T., Metzger, D., Peters, R., Smith, I., Grissom, G.,

Pettinati, H., Argeriou, M. (1992). The fifth edition of the addiction severity index.

Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 9, 199-213.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 40

McLellan, A., Luborsky, L., Cacciola, J., & Griffith, J.E. (1985). New data from

the Addiction Severity Index: Reliability and validity in three centers. Journal of

Nervous and Mental Disorders. 173.412-423.

Nelson, R.O. (1977). Assessment and therapeutic functions of self-monitoring.

In M. Hersen, R.M. Eisler & P. Miller (Eds), Progress in Behavioral Modification. New

York: Academic Press.

Nelson, R.O., Lipinski, D P. & Boykin, R.A. (1978). The effects of self­

recorders' training and the obtrusiveness of the self-recording device on the accuracy and

reactivity of self-monitoring. Behavior Theranv. 9, 200-208.

O’Farrell, T.J., Cutter, H.S.G., Bayog, R.D., Dentch, G., & Fortgang, J. (1984).

Correspondence between one-year retrospective reports of pretreatment drinking by

alcoholics and their wives. Behavioral Assessment. 6, 263-274.

Paty, J., Kassel, J., & Shiffman, S. (1992). The importance of assessing base

rates for clinical studies; An example of stimuluscontrol of smoking. In M. W. deVries

(Ed.) The Experience of Psychopathology: Investigating Mental Disorders in Their

Natural Setting, pp. 347-352. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Prochaska, J.O., DiClemente, C.C., & Norcross, J.C. (1992). In search of how

people change: Applications to addictive behaviors. American Psvchologist. 47, 1102-

1114.

Rehm, L., Komblith, S., O’Hara, M., Lamparski, D., & Romanao, J. (1981). An

evaluation of major components in a self-control therapy for depression. Behavior

Modification. 5, 459-490.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 41

Romanczyk, R. G., Tracy, D. A., Wilson, G. T., & Thorpe, G. L. (1973).

Behavioral techniques in the treatment of obesity: A comparative analysis. Behavior

Research and Therapy. 11. 629-640.

Samo, J. A., Tucker, J. A., & Vuchinich, R. E. (1989). Agreement between self­

monitoring, recall, and collateral observation measures of alcohol consumption in older

adults. Addictive Behaviors. 11, 391-409.

Saunders, J.B., Ashland, O.G., Babor, T.F., de la Fuente, J R., & Grant, M.

(1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO

collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-ll.

Addiction. 88, 791-804.

Sayette, M., & Hufford, M. (1994). The effects of cue exposure and deprivation

on cognitive resources in smokers. Journal of . 103. 812-818.

Shiffman, S., Enberg, J.B., Paty, J.A., Perz, W.G., Gnys, M., Kassel, J.D., &

Hickox (1997). A day at a time: Predicting smoking lapse from daily urge. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology. 106, 104-116.

Shiffman, S., Fisher, L.A., Paty, J.A., Gnys, M., Kassel, J.D., Hickox, M., & Perz,

W. (1994). Drinking and smoking: A field study of their association. Annals of

Behavioral Medicine. 16. 203-209.

Shiffman, S., Hufford, M., Hickox, M., Paty, J.A., Gnys, M., & Kassel, J. (1997).

Remember that? A comparison of real-time versus retrospective recall of smoking

lapses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 292-300.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 42

Shiffman, S., Paty, J.A., Gnys, M., Kassel, J.A., & Hickcox, M. (1996). First

lapses to smoking; within-subjects analysis of real-time reports. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology.64, 366-379.

Shiffman, S., Shumaker, S.A., Abrams, D.B., Cohen, S., Garvey, A., Grunberg,

N.E., & Swan, G.E. (1986). Models of smoking relapse. Health Psychology. 5. 13-27.

Sieck, W.A. & McFall, R.M. (1976). Some determinants of self-monitoring

effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 44. 958-965.

Sobell, M. B., Bogardis, J., Schuller, R., Leo, G. I., & Sobell, L. C. (1989). Is

self-monitoring of alcohol consumption reactive? Behavioral Assessment. 11. 447-258.

Sobell, L.C. & Sobell M.F. (1973). A self-feedback technique to monitor

drinking in alcoholics. Behavior research and therapy. 11, 237-238.

Sobell, L.C. & Sobell, M B. (1992). Timeline follow-back: A technique for

measuring self-reported alcohol consumption. In R. Litten & J. Allen (Eds.) Measuring

Alcohol Consumption. Totowa NJ: Humana Press.

Sobell, M B., Sobell, L.C., Klajner, F., Pavan, D., Basian, E. (1986). The

reliability of a timeline method for assessing normal drinker college students’ recent

drinking history: Utility for alcohol research. Addictive Behaviors. 11, 149-161.

Sobell, L.C., Sobell, M B., Leo, G.I., & Cancilla, A. (1988). Reliability of a

timeline method: Assessing normal drinkers' reports of recent drinking and a comparative

evaluation across several populations. British Journal of Addiction. 83. 393-402.

Stone, A.A. & Shiftman, S. (1994). Ecological Momentary Assessment;

Measuring real world processes in behavioral medicine. Annals of Behavioral Medicine.

16, 199-202.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. REACTIVITY TO EMA 43

Stone, A.A., Schwartz, J.E., Neale, J.M., Shiffman, S., Marco., C.A., Hickox, M.,

Paty, J., Porter, L.S., & Cruise, L.J. (1998). A comparison of coping assessed by

ecological momentary assessment and retrospective recall. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology. 74. 1670-1680.

Tiffany, S. T. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior:

Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review. 147-168.

Uchalik, D. C. (1979). A comparison of questionnaire and self-monitored reports

of alcohol intake in a nonalcoholic population. Addictive Behaviors. 4, 409-413.

Vallacher, R.R. & Nowak, A. (Eds.). (1994). Dynamical systems in social

psychology. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of

brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality

and , 1063-1070.

Zeger, S. L. & Liang, K.-Y. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and

continuous outcomes. Biometrics. 42. 121-130.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.