Reinterpreting Visual Patterns in Mental Imagery

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Reinterpreting Visual Patterns in Mental Imagery COGNITIVE SCIENCE 13, 51-78 (1989) ReinterpretingVisual Patterns in Mental Imagery RONALDA.FINKE Texas A&M University STEVENPINKER Massachusetts Institute of Technology MARTHAJ.FARAH Carnegie-Mellon University In a recent paper, Chambers and Reisberg (1985) showed that people cannot reverse classical ambiguous figures in imagery (such OS the Necker cube, duck/ rabbit, or Schroeder staircase). In three experiments, we refute one kind of ex- planation for this difficulty: that visual images da not contain information about the geometry of a shape necessary for reinterpreting it or that people connot apply shape classification procedures to the information in imagery. We show, that given suitable conditions, people con assign novel interpretations to ambigu- ous images which have been constructed out of parts or mentally transformed. For example, when asked to imagine the letter “D” on its side, affixed to the top of the letter “J”, subjects spontaneously report “seeing” an umbrella. We also show that these reinterpretations are not the result of guessing strategies, and that they speak directly to the issue of whether or not mental images of ambigu- ous figures can be recanstrued. Finally, we show that arguments from the phiios- aphy literature on the relation between images and descriptions are not relevant to the issue of whether imoges can be reinterpreted, and we suggest possible ex- planations for why classical ambiguous figures do not spontaneously reverse in imagery. At least since Berkeley’s time, the question of whether mental images can be ambiguous has held a central place in the debate over the nature of imagery. It is easy to see why the two issues are so closely related. The process of per- ception begins with the geometry of the retinal images, and ends with a description of objects in the world. The controversy over imagery has largely concerned whether images are like early perceptual representations contain- ing information about the geometric properties of visual inputs, or like later This research was supported by NIMH Grant lROlMH3980901 to Ronald A. Finke, by NSF Grant 85-18774 to Steven Pinker, and by ONR Grant NOOO14-86-K-0094 and NIH Pro- gram Project Grant NS-06209-21, and NIH Grant R23-NS-23458-01 to Martha J. Farah. We thank Ned Block, James Greeno, Stephen Kosslyn, Howard Kurtzman, Steven Palmer, ROSS Thompson, and Barbara Tversky for helpful comments and suggestions. Correspondence and requests for reprints should be sent to Steven Pinker, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT, Cambridge MA 02139. 51 52 FINKE, PINKER, AND FARAH cognitiverepresentations containing information about the conceptualcate- goriesof interpretedobjects (Kosslyn & Pomerantz,1977; Pylyshyn, 1973). If memory imagespreserve some of the geometricinformation in perceptual representations,it should be possiblefor the imager to recognizethe pres- enceof an object categoryin an imagethat wasnot originally assignedwhen the object wasfirst seen.In the most dramaticcase, an imagershould be able to observean ambiguousfigure, suchas a Neckercube or a duck/rabbit, see it as one object (e.g., a duck), form a visual imageof it when it is no longer present,and then be able to seeit as the other object (e.g., a rabbit). On the other hand, if memory imagesare records of the conceptualcategory or in- terpretationassigned to the stimulus when it was viewed, and information about its geometricproperties is lost or not readily accessibleto interpreta- tive processes,then a reassignmentof the categoryof an object should be impossible;the imager should be stuck with whateverinterpretation he or sheassigned to the stimulus when it was visible. Severalexperimental investigations have cast doubt on people’sability to recategorizeimages of ambiguousfigures. An experimentreported by Reed (1974)explored whether subjects could detect“hidden” figuresin imagesof patternsthat were composedof combinationsof geometricforms. For ex- ample, oneof the patternswas formed by superimposingtwo equilateraltri- angles,one pointing up, and the other pointing down, positionedsuch that the vertexof one was centeredon the baseof the other. After a brief reten- tion interval, the subjectswere shown a secondpattern, and their task was to say whether or not that pattern was a part of the first pattern. Reed found that subjectscould easilydetect only thoseparts that would enterinto a structural descriptionof the pattern, such as one of the equilateraltri- angles,but not a part that cuts acrossthe elementsof such a description, suchas a parallelogram.Reed and Johnson(1975) later found that the parts not fitting into the original composition of a complex pattern could be detectedmuch more easilywhen subjectscould inspectthe original patterns at the time of testing, than when they had to rely on a memory image. Be- causesubjects in theseexperiments could rarely detectthe hidden parts in their images,these results suggestedthat images,unlike visually perceived forms, cannot be reinterpretedor reorganized.Rather, what is detectedin an imagemay dependentirely on how the imaginedpattern was initially con- ceived(see also the relevant work of Hinton, 1979,and Stevens8r Coupe, 1978). Thesefindings conflict with the observationsof other imagery theorists who haveclaimed that the ability to “see” new patternsin an image is one of the prime functions of imagery, for example,in scientific and artistic creativity (Shepard,1978). More importantly, thereare demonstrations that peoplecan detect new patternsin transformed images.Pinker and Finke (1980)reported a seriesof experimentsin which subjectswere able to “see” shapesthat emergedin the projection of a three-dimensionalconfiguration REINTERPRETING IMAGES 53 of objects after it was mentally rotated. Shepardand Feng (reportedin Shepard& Cooper, 1982)demonstrated that subjectscould quickly name the letter resulting from a transformation (rotation, reflection, or some combination of the two) of a starting letter. For example,when given the transformation “rotate 90 degrees”and the starting letter “N,” subjects could reconstruethe resultingimage as a “Z.” In an experimentsimilar to thoseof Reed, Slee(1980, Experiment 3) found that subjectswere able to judge, with successrates greaterthan chance,whether various geometric forms were presentas embeddedfigures in patternsthey had imagined. In anotherexperiment, Slee demonstrated that subjectscould constructa men- tal image from separatelyviewed piecesand then detect emergentforms resulting from a reorganizationof the imagined piecesaccording to the Gestaltlaws of proximity and common fate. Hollins (1985)had a group of subjectsimagine a grid and mentally fill in certain squaresspecified in terms of their Cartesiancoordinates. On different trials, the experimenterdictated patternsof filled-in squaresresembling a dog, a pitcher, a wall plug, a car, and a telephone.Subjects were able to saywhat the resultingimage depicted on about half of the trials. Relatedindirect evidencecomes from experimentson visual synthesisof parts. Palmer (1977)had subjectsmentally synthesizepatterns by mentally superimposingtwo visually presentedparts consistingof connectedline seg- ments.They then had to match the synthesizedpattern against visual probes. The task was easiestwhen the subpatternscorresponded to perceptually “good” geometricfigures such as triangles and boxes,as opposedto open or disconnectedcollections of line segments.However, subjectsreported that evenwhen the original subpatternswere not “good,” they “looked” for emergent“good” figuresin the synthesizedwhole, with greateror lesser successon different trials. Apparently, at least some subjectswere quite successfulwith this strategy:Their matching times were uniformly fast for shapessynthesized out of good, moderatelygood, and bad parts.Thompson and Klatzky (1978)obtained this effect more uniformly by having subjects mentallysuperimpose sets of visually presentedangles and linesthat together definedunified geometricshapes such as a parallelogram.They found that subjectsreally did treat the result as an emergentsingle form: When match- ing thesepatterns against probe stimuli, they wereno slowerwhen they had synthesizedthe pattern by superimposingtwo or threeparts than whenthey had actually seenthe pattern in its entirety. However, a paper has appearedrecently whose authors try to make a strongcase that the reconstrualof mental imagesis impossible.Chambers and Reisberg(1985) conducted a set of four experimentsaimed at assessing whetherpeople can reinterpretan ambiguousfigure stored in a mentalimage. In their experiments,subjects inspectedambiguous forms, such as the “duck/rabbit” figure commonly used to demonstratemultistability in visual perception(e.g., Attneave, 1971),and werethen instructedto form 54 FINKE, PINKER, AND FARAH mental images of the forms and to try to see the reversals in their images. Al- though the subjects were previously trained in detecting such reversals using other types of reversible figures, they never once reported the correct rever- sal in their imagery. This negative finding persisted even when the subjects were screened for high imagery vividness. In addition, the subjects were able to reverse the figures when they later drew the figures from memory and inspected their drawings. Chambers and Reisberg concluded that men- tal images are therefore not subject to reconstrual, in contrast to visually perceived forms, because images do not contain uninterpreted information; the implication is that images are nothing but
Recommended publications
  • VITA ROBERT A. JACOBS Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences Meliora Hall, River Campus University of Rochester Rochester
    VITA ROBERT A. JACOBS Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences February 2021 Meliora Hall, River Campus 585-275-0753 University of Rochester 585-442-9216 (fax) Rochester, NY 14627-0268 [email protected] EDUCATION University of Massachusetts (1984-1990) Computer and Information Science Ph.D. (September 1990) M.S. (June 1987) University of Pennsylvania (1978-1982) B.A. Psychology (June 1982) RESEARCH INTERESTS Cognitive and perceptual learning, memory, and decision making Computational models of cognition and perception Visual and multisensory perception Perception and action Cognitive neuroscience of learning, memory, and decision making WORK EXPERIENCE Professor Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester (July 2003-present); Center for Visual Science (July 2003-present); Department of Computer Science (July 2003-present) Associate Editor Topics in Cognitive Science, journal of The Cognitive Science Society (January 2009- December 2012) Treasurer Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS) Foundation (December 2003-December 2007); this foundation organizes the annual NIPS conference and workshops 1 Senior Editor / Associate Editor Senior Editor (January 1998-December 2000), Associate Editor (January 2001-December 2003) of Cognitive Science, journal of The Cognitive Science Society Associate Professor Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester (July 1997-June 2003); Center for Visual Science (July 1997-June 2003); Department of Computer Sci- ence (September 1998-June 2003) Program Director Cognitive Science Program, University of Rochester (July 1996-June 1998) Assistant Professor Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester (July 1995-June 1997); Center for Visual Science (February 1997-June 1997); Department of Psychology (September 1992-June 1995) Postdoctoral Fellow Laboratory of Dr. Stephen Kosslyn, Department of Psychology, Harvard University (July 1991-August 1992) Postdoctoral Fellow Laboratory of Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • 12 Kosslyn Stephen
    Dies academicus 2012 Die Philosophisch-humanwissenschaftliche Fakultät verleiht die Würde eines Doctor philosophiae honoris causa Herrn Prof. Stephen M. Kosslyn, PhD Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, USA Laudatio Stephen Kosslyn hat mit bahnbrechenden Arbeiten die Kognitionspsychologie nachhaltig geprägt und ihre Beziehung zu den Neurowissenschaften zukunftsweisend vorangetrieben. Getragen von einem beeindruckenden wissenschaftlichen Engagement hat er grosse Verdienste im Brückenschlag zwischen Verhaltens-, Natur- und Ingenieurwissenschaften bei der Erkundung neuer Anwendungsfelder erworben. Biografie: Stephen Kosslyn kam 1948 in Santa Monica, Kalifornien, zur Welt. Er studierte an der University of California in Los Angeles (UCLA) Psychologie (1966-1970) und war danach als Doktorand von Gordon Bower an der Stanford University tätig, wo er 1974 ebenfalls im Fach Psychologie promovierte. Nachfolgend lehrte und forschte er an der Johns Hopkins University (1974-1977), an der Harvard University (1977-1981) und an der Brandeis University (1981-1982). Im Jahre 1983 wurde Stephen Kosslyn an die Harvard University berufen und 2001 wurde ihm der John Lindsley Lehrstuhl für Psychologie (in memoriam William James) eingerichtet. An der Harvard University nahm er verschiedene leitende Funktionen wahr, u.a. die Institutsleitung (Department of Psychology, 2005-2008), Dekan (Social Science, 2008-2011) und sowie die Führung mehrerer interfakultärer Kommissionen im Rahmen des Mind-Brain- Behavior Verbundes der Harvard University. Nach seiner 18-jährigen Tätigkeit in Harvard übernahm Stephen Kosslyn im Jahre 2011 die Leitung des Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences (CASBS) an der Stanford University, seinem aktuellen Arbeitsort. Das CASBS besteht seit 1954 und dient dazu, verhaltens-wissenschaftliche Erkenntnisse (z.B. aus der Soziologie, Psychologie oder den Wirtschafts-wissenschaften) für die Lösung gesellschaftlich relevanter Probleme nutzbar zu machen.
    [Show full text]
  • ED 266 190 TM 860 136 Kosslyn, Stephen M. TITLE Visual Hemispheric Specialization: a Computational Theory. Technical Report
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 266 190 TM 860 136 AUTilOR Kosslyn, Stephen M. TITLE Visual Hemispheric Specialization: A Computational Theory. Technical Report #7. INSTITUTION Harvard Univ., Cambridge, Mass. SPONS AGENCY National Inst. of Mental Health (DHHS), Rockville, MD.; Office of Naval Research, Arlington, Va. Personnel and Training Research Programs Office. PUB DATE 31 Oct 85 GRANT 141139478-01; N00014-83-K-0095; N00014-85-K-0291 NOTE 84p. PUB TYPE Reports - Research/Technical (143) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC04 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Beuavior Patterns; *Cerebral Dominance; Conceptual Tempo; Elementary Secondary Education; Encoding (Psychology); *Lateral Dominance; Memory; Models; Pattern Recognition; *Perceptual Development; Reaction Time; Spatial Ability; Visualization; *Visual Perception IDENTIFIERS Brain Hemispheres; *Imaging; *Neuropsychology ABSTRACT Visual recognition, navigation, tracking, and imagery are posited to involve some of the same types of representations and processes. The first part of this paper develops a theory of some of the shared types of representations and processing modules. The theory is developed in light of neurophysiological and neuroanatomical data from non-human primates, computational constraints, and behavioral data from human subjects. In developing theories of some of the high-level computations that are performed by the visual system, three problems which must be solved by any visual system are considered: (1) position variability; (2) figure/ground segregation; and (3) non-rigid transformations. The second part of the paper develops a melhanism for the development of lateralization of visual function in the brain. The theory rests on a set of relatively simple, uncontroversial properties of the brain, including: (1) processing components; (2) ezarcise; (3) selectivity; (4) "central" bilateral control; and (5) reciprocal innervation.
    [Show full text]
  • The Cognitive Neuroscience of Mind: a Tribute to Michael S. Gazzaniga
    The Cognitive Neuroscience of Mind A Tribute to Michael S. Gazzaniga edited by Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz, Kathleen Baynes, George R. Mangun, and Elizabeth A. Phelps The Cognitive Neuroscience of Mind The Cognitive Neuroscience of Mind A Tribute to Michael S. Gazzaniga edited by Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz, Kathleen Baynes, George R. Mangun, and Elizabeth A. Phelps A Bradford Book The MIT Press Cambridge, Massachusetts London, England © 2010 Massachusetts Institute of Technology All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form by any electronic or mechanical means (including photocopying, recording, or informa- tion storage and retrieval) without permission in writing from the publisher. For information about special quantity discounts, please email special_sales@ mitpress.mit.edu This book was set in Sabon by Toppan Best-set Premedia Limited. Printed and bound in the United States of America. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data The cognitive neuroscience of mind : a tribute to Michael S. Gazzaniga / edited by Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz ... [et al.]. p. cm. “ A Bradford book. ” Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-0-262-01401-4 (hardcover : alk. paper) 1. Cognitive neuroscience— Congresses. 2. Gazzaniga, Michael S. — Congresses. I. Gazzaniga, Michael S. II. Reuter-Lorenz, Patricia Ann, 1958 – [DNLM: 1. Gazzaniga, Michael S. 2. Cognition — Festschrift. 3. Neurosciences — Festschrift. BF 311 C676346 2010] QP360.5.C3694 2010 612.8a 233 — dc22 2009034514 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Contents Preface vii I The Bisected Brain 1 The Bisected Brain (Poem) 1 Marta Kutas 1 Corpus Callosum: Mike Gazzaniga, the Cal Tech Lab, and Subsequent Research on the Corpus Callosum 3 Mitchell Glickstein and Giovanni Berlucchi 2 Interhemispheric Cooperation Following Brain Bisection 25 Steven A.
    [Show full text]
  • Sequential Processes in Image Generation: an Objective Measure. Technical Report #6
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 266 188 TM 860 133 AUTHOR Kosslyn, Stephen M.; And Others TITLE Sequential Processes in Image Generation: An Objective Measure. Technical Report #6. INSTITUTION Harvard Univ., Cambridge, Mass. SPONS AGENCY Office of Naval Research, Arlington, Va. Personnel and Training Research Programs Office. PUB DATE 31 Oct 85 CONTRACT N00014-85-K-0251 NOTE 68p. PUB TYPE Reports - Research/Technical (143) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC03 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Analysis of Variance; College S.udents; Computer Orion Jd Programs; Conceptual Tempo; Control Groups; *Cues; Higher Education; *Language Processing; *Long Term Memory; *Measurement Techniques; Models; Pattern Recognition; Reaction Time; Recall (Psychology); Recognition (Psychology); Regression (Statistics); Research Methodology; *Short Term Memory; Visualization; *Visual Perception IDENTIFIERS *Imaging ABSTRACT This paper investigates the processes by which visual mental images--the precept-like short-term memory representations - -are created from information stored in long-term memory. It also presents a new method for studying image generation. Three experiments were conducted using college students as subjects. In the first experiment, a Podgorny and Shepherd paradigm was adapted to study image generation. In the second experiment, regression analyses were performed on response times in a scanning instruction and no-scanning instruction condition. Experiment three was designed to investigate whether scanning plays a functional role in image generation. Results indicated that leases of upper case lettersare formed segment by segment, in roughly the order in which most people draw the letters. Results were shown not to be an artifact of how people scan images once they are formed, and could not have been due to experimenter-expectancy effects.
    [Show full text]
  • Imagery Without Arrays
    THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1979)2,535-581 Printed in the United States of America Stephen M. KossSyn Department of Psychology and Social Relations, Harvard University, Cambridge, Mass. 02138 Stewen Pinker Department of Psychology and Social Relations, Harvard University, Cambridge, Mass. 02138 George E_ Smith Department of Philosophy, Tufts University, Medford, Mass. 02155 Stewen P- Shwartz Department of Psychology, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Md. 21218 Abstract; What might a theory of mental imagery look like, and how might one begin formulating such a theory? These are the central questions addressed in the present paper. The first section outlines the general research direction taken here and provides an overview of the empirical foundations of our theory of image representation and processing. Four issues are considered in succession, and the relevant results of experiments are presented and discussed. The second section begins with a discussion of the proper form for a cognitive theory, and the distinction between a theory and a model is developed. Following this, the present theory and computer simulation model are introduced. This theory specifies the nature of the internal representations (data structures) and the processes that operate on them when one generates, inspects, or transforms mental images. In the third, concluding, section we consider three very different kinds of objections to the present research program, one hinging on the possibility of experimental artifacts in the data, and the others turning on metatheoretical commitments about the form of a cognitive theory. Finally, we discuss how one ought best to evaluate theories and models of the sort developed here.
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae JACOB FELDMAN
    Feldman, updated 11/2016 Curriculum Vitae JACOB FELDMAN Department of Psychology [email protected] Center for Cognitive Science (848) 445-1621 (office) Rutgers University - New Brunswick (732) 445-6715 (fax) 154 Frelinghuysen Rd. Piscataway, NJ 08854 http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/˜jacob Current Position Professor, Dept. of Psychology, Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, 2007–. (also Member, Graduate Faculty in Computer Science, Rutgers University) Previous Positions Associate Professor, Dept. of Psychology, Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, 2001– 2007. Assistant Professor, Dept. of Psychology, Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, 1995– 2001. Research Assistant Professor, Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University (1992–1995) Education Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Ph.D. in Cognitive Science, September 1992 Thesis: Perceptual Categories and World Regularities Advisor: Prof. Whitman Richards Massachusetts Institute of Technology, M.S. in Brain & Cognitive Sciences, September 1990 Thesis: Perceptual Decomposition as Inference Advisor: Prof. Steven Pinker Harvard College, A.B Magna cum Laude in Psychology & Social Relations, June 1986 Thesis: The Representation of Motor Programs Advisor: Prof. Stephen Kosslyn Research Interests Visual perception and concept learning, focusing on the development of mathematical and compu- tational models of human perceptual and cognitive functions. Principal areas of interest include perceptual organization and grouping; representation and categorization
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Cognition : an Introduction * STEVEN PINKER
    1 Visual cognition : An introduction * STEVENPINKER Massachusetts Institute of Technology Abstract This article is a tutorial overview of a sample of central issues in visual cognition , focusing on the recognition of shapes and the representation of objects and spatial relations in perception and imagery . Brief reviews of the state of the art are presented , followed by more extensive presentations of contemporary theories , findings , and open issues . I discuss various theories of shape recognition , such as template , feature , Fourier , structural description , Marr - Nishi - hara , and massively parallel models , and issues such as the reference frames , primitives , top -down processing , and computational architectures used in spatial cognition . This is followed by a discussion of mental imagery , including conceptual issues in imagery research , theories of imagery , imagery and perception , image transformations , computational complexities of image processing , neuropsychological issues , and possible functions of imagery . Connections between theories of recognition and of imagery , and the relevance of the papers contained in this issue to the topics discussed , are emphasized throughout . Recognizing and reasoning about the visual environment is something that people do extraordinarily well ; it is often said that in these abilities an average three -year old makes the most sophisticated computer vision system look embarrassingly inept . Our hominid ancestors fabricated and used tools for millions of years before our species emerged , and the selection pressures brought about by tool use may have resulted in the development of sophisticated faculties allowing us to recognize objects and their physical properties , to bring complex knowledge to bear on familiar objects and scenes , to *Preparation of this paper was supported by NSF grants BNS 82-16546 and 82-09540.
    [Show full text]
  • 30 Most Influential Psychologists Working Today
    30 Most Influential Psychologists Working Today The quest to understand the workings of human thought and behavior can be traced back to ancient peoples. Before it was established as a scientific discipline, the study of psychology was of interest to many great thinkers and philosophers. These days, it has become a popular field, even outside the realm of science. After all, psychology influences so many different spheres of life, from marketing, education and entertainment to economics and even law enforcement. Here is a list of 30 of the most influential psychologists who continue to do groundbreaking and far-reaching work today. 30. Martin Seligman Martin E. P. Seligman is perhaps best known for his theory of “learned helplessness.” He is currently the Zellerbach Family Professor of Psychology at the University of Pennsylvania and the director of the university’s Positive Psychology Center. Apart from being a psychologist and educator, Seligman is also the author of several bestselling books. Seligman is listed as the 13th most quoted 20th-century psychologist in preparatory psychology textbooks, based on a study by Haggbloom et al. In the same study, Seligman also appeared as the 31st most eminent psychologist overall. Several institutions have backed his research, including The National Institute of Mental Health and the National Science Foundation. Seligman has also been granted honors such as two Distinguished Scientific Contribution awards, presented by the American Psychological Association. 29. Howard Gardner Howard Gardner is currently the John H. and Elisabeth A. Hobbs Professor of Cognition and Education at the Harvard Graduate School of Education and Senior Director of Harvard Project Zero.
    [Show full text]