Investigating Criteria for Different Identity Judgments

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Investigating Criteria for Different Identity Judgments University of Wollongong Research Online Faculty of Health and Behavioural Sciences - Papers (Archive) Faculty of Science, Medicine and Health 1-1-2010 How different is different? Investigating criteria for different identity judgments Harold C. Hill University of Wollongong, [email protected] Michelle Corcoran [email protected] Peter Claes John Clement The University Of Melbourne Follow this and additional works at: https://ro.uow.edu.au/hbspapers Part of the Arts and Humanities Commons, Life Sciences Commons, Medicine and Health Sciences Commons, and the Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Hill, Harold C.; Corcoran, Michelle; Claes, Peter; and Clement, John: How different is different? Investigating criteria for different identity judgments 2010, 156-156. https://ro.uow.edu.au/hbspapers/987 Research Online is the open access institutional repository for the University of Wollongong. For further information contact the UOW Library: [email protected] How different is different? Investigating criteria for different identity judgments Abstract Any face seen for the first time will have a closet neighbour in memory. In order to avoid false alarms, we must be able to distinguish similar from identical faces. Work is reported investigating same/different judgments as a function difference in three dimensional shape defined in terms of standard deviation in a principal component based face space. The aim is to determine the criterion difference below which observers respond “same”. A threshold corresponding to a dprime of 1 was also calculated. Both were first measured under three conditions – same view images, different view images and animated images of the faces rotating. Criterion did not differ significantly between presentation conditions and the mean criterion corresponded to 0.45 SD when the average face was used as the reference. Threshold did differ significantly between conditions, and was significantly higher for different view faces (.65SD) than for same view (.39SD) or animated faces (.44SD). The results are interpreted as indicating that the criterion for same/different judgements is relatively stable across presentation conditions, but that sensitivity is lower when making the judgement across a change in view. Further work will investigate whether criterion varies as a function of distinctiveness. Keywords different, judgments, investigating, criteria, identity Disciplines Arts and Humanities | Life Sciences | Medicine and Health Sciences | Social and Behavioral Sciences Publication Details Hill, HC, Corcoran, M, Claes, P & Clement, J, How different is different? Investigating criteria for different identity judgments, In ECVP'10: 33rd European Conference on Visual Perception, 22 August, Lausanne, Switzerland. Perception, 39(Abstract Supplement), 2010, p 156. This conference paper is available at Research Online: https://ro.uow.edu.au/hbspapers/987 ____ ___ _ ____ ____ __ ______ __________ ______ __ ______ _________ ______________________ Perception, 2010, volume 39, supplement, page 1 – 203 Thirty-third European Conference on Visual Perception Lausanne, Switzerland 22 – 26 August 2010 Abstracts Sunday Symposium in honour of the late Richard Gregory 1 Perception Lecture (C Koch) 3 Monday Talks: Attention 4 Talks: Adaptation, Brightness and Contrast 6 Symposium: Face perception in man and monkey 9 Talks: Objects 10 Symposium: Colour appearance mechanisms 12 Talks: Neural mechanisms 14 Posters: Applied vision 16 Posters: Biological motion 18 Posters: Clinical vision and ageing 20 Posters: Contours and crowding 25 Posters: Emotions and cognition 30 Posters: Eye movements I 33 Posters: Learning and memory 38 Posters: Models and theory 44 Posters: Non-human vision 49 Posters: Perception and action I 50 Tuesday Talks: Eye movements 55 Talks: Clinical vision 57 Talks: Spatial vision 60 Talks: Learning and memory 62 Symposium: Visual social perception: Brain imaging and sex differences 64 Talks: Multisensory processing and haptics 66 Rank Lecture 68 Posters: 3D vision, depth I 68 Posters: Adaptation, aftereffects 74 Posters: Attention I 78 Posters: Colour 84 Posters: Face perception I 89 Posters: Motion I 94 Posters: Natural images 99 Posters: Objects and scenes 101 Wednesday Talks: 3D Vision, depth 107 Symposium: The complexity of visual stability 110 Symposium: Psychophysics: yesterday, today, and tomorrow 112 Posters: Art and vision 113 Posters: Attention II 117 Posters: Consciousness and rivalry 123 Posters: Face perception II 125 Posters: Haptics 130 Posters: Imaging 133 Posters: Motion II 137 Posters: Multisensory processing 142 Posters: Reading 147 Thursday Talks: Motion 150 Talks: Multistability, rivalry 152 Talks: Face perception 155 Talks: Colour 157 Posters: 3D Vision, depth II 160 Posters: Brightness, lightness 165 Posters: Contrast 168 Posters: Eye movements II 170 Posters: Illusions 174 Posters: Perception and action II 180 Posters: Perceptual organization 184 Posters: Temporal processing 187 Posters: Visual search 191 Publisher’s note. In the interests of efficiency, these abstracts have been reproduced as supplied by the Conference with little or no copy editing by Pion. Thus, the English and style may not match those of regular Perception articles. Organiser Michael Herzog Local Committee Gijs Plomp Laure Dayer Silvia Marchesotti Val ´ erie Jacot-Descombes Marc Repnow Scientific Committee Talis Bachmann Anton Beer Eli Brenner Emanuela Bricolo Anna Brooks Isabelle B¨ ulthoff Claus-Christian Carbon Patrick Cavanagh Heiner Deubel Casper Erkelens Manfred Fahle Jozsef Fiser Sergei Gepshtein Tandra Ghose Alan Gilchrist Mark W. Greenlee Elisabeth Hein Sven Heinrich Frouke Hermens Robert Hess Todd Horowitz Anya Hurlbert Alumit Ishai Astrid Kappers Dirk Kerzel Akiyoshi Kitaoka Joseph Krummenacher G¨unter Meinhardt Marino Menozzi Michael Morgan Thomas Papathomas Galina Paramei Baingio Pinna Gregor Rainer Brian Rogers Bettina Rolke Takao Sato Thomas Schmidt Robert O’Shea Claudio de’Sperati Hans Strasburger Ian Thornton Simon Thorpe Frans Verstraten Thomas Wachtler Johan Wagemans Andrew Watson Sophie Wuerger Qasim Zaidi Johannes Zanker Li Zhaoping Supporting organisations Canton de Vaud (www.vd.ch) Cooperation@epfl (cooperation.epfl.ch) ECVP 2008 (www.fss.uu.nl/psn/web/people/projects/ECVP2008/) ECVP 2009 (www.ecvp2009.org) F. Hoffmann-La Roche AG (www.roche.ch) Merck Serono SA (www.merckserono.net) Novartis Pharma AG (www.novartis.ch) Optical Society of America (www.osa.org) Pion Ltd (www.pion.co.uk) Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF) (www.snf.ch) The City of Lausanne (www.lausanne.ch) The Rank Prize Funds (www.rankprize.org) The European Conference on Visual Perception is an annual event. Previous conferences took place in: 1978 Marburg (D) 1979 Noordwijkerhout (NL) 1980 Brighton (GB) 1981 Gouvieux (F) 1982 Leuven (B) 1983 Lucca (I) 1984 Cambridge (GB) 1985 Pe˜ niscola (E) 1986 Bad Nauheim (D) 1987 Varna (BG) 1988 Bristol (GB) 1989 Zichron Yaakov (IL) 1990 Paris (F) 1994 Eindhoven (NL) 1995 T¨ubingen (D) 1996 Strasbourg (F) 1997 Helsinki (FI) 1998 Oxford (GB) 1999 Trieste (I) 2000 Groningen (NL) 2001 Kus¸adasi (TR) 2002 Glasgow (GB) 2003 Paris (F) 2004 Budapest (H) 2005 A Coru˜na (E) 2006 St Petersburg (RU) 2007 Arezzo (I) 2008 Utrecht (NL) 2009 Regensburg (D) © Copyright 2010 a Pion publication printed in Great Britain Symposium in honour of the late Richard Gregory Sunday 1 ECVP 2010 Abstracts Sunday SYMPOSIUM IN HONOUR OF THE LATE RICHARD GREGORY m Richard and randomness drove my research S Anstis (University of California, San Diego (UCSD), USA; e-mail: [email protected]) In the mid-20th century, Richard was working on motion perception, so I, his first graduate student, did likewise. First, using a rotating kymograph drum, a xerox of Bela Julesz’ first random-dot patterns, and a stroboscope, I failed to discover Dmax . (Ol Braddick succeeded in discovering that later.) Then, using some fluorescent tubes driven by a home-made power pack, I failed to make a power stroboscope, but I stumbled across a ramp aftere_ect, which I published in Science. Encouraged, but only faintly, by Richard, I discovered reversed phi by putting the wrong slides into a projector. Inspired by Richard’s declaring that my PhD. thesis was ‘surprisingly good’, I went on to stumble across the footsteps illusion, the chopsticks illusion, four-stroke cycle motion, crossover motion, zigzag motion; and I have been stumbling on ever since. Richard has helped me to demonstrate that fortune favours the prepared mind (Pasteur) and also the unprepared mind (Anstis). Thank you, Richard. m All jazzy. Gregorian explorations of pure colour P Cavanagh (Université Paris Descartes, France; e-mail: [email protected]) Like many of us, I was captured, enthralled, and enslaved by Richard Gregory the first time I heard him speak. He showed us how faces melt into indecipherable islands of colour when presented at ‘isoluminance’. Inspired by Richard’s flapping eyebrows, his big smile and a laugh that boasted of the fun of vision research, I joined legions of others abandoning their plodding first careers and became a vision scientist. Richard had shown us how to use filters in front of a slide projector to adjust the red to green luminance and find the treasured ‘Critical Zone’ of equal luminance, where all turned to jelly, uncovering after 60 years the pioneering Gestalt discoveries of perceptual losses and the slowing of motion. For Richard’s Gregory Fest in Bristol, we built Big Chroma, the world’s largest equiluminant
Recommended publications
  • CHAPTER Personality TEN Psychology
    10/14/08 CHAPTER Personality TEN Psychology Psychology 370 Sheila K. Grant, Ph.D. SKINNER AND STAATS: Professor California State University, Northridge The Challenge of Behaviorism Chapter Overview Chapter Overview RADICAL BEHAVIORISM: SKINNER Part IV: The Learning Perspective PSYCHOLOGICAL BEHAVIORISM: STAATS Illustrative Biography: Tiger Woods Reinforcement Behavior as the Data for Scientific Study The Evolutionary Context of Operant Behavior Basic Behavioral Repertoires The Rate of Responding The Emotional-Motivational Repertoire Learning Principles The Language-Cognitive Repertoire Reinforcement: Increasing the Rate of Responding The Sensory-Motor Repertoire Punishment and Extinction: Decreasing the Rate of Situations Responding Additional Behavioral Techniques Psychological Adjustment Schedules of Reinforcement The Nature-Nurture Question from the Applications of Behavioral Techniques Perspective of Psychological Behaviorism Therapy Education Radical Behaviorism and Personality Theory: Some Concerns Chapter Overview Part IV: The Learning Perspective Personality Assessment from a Ivan Pavlov: Behavioral Perspective Heuristic Accendental Discovery The Act-Frequency Approach to Personality Classical Conditioning Measurement John B. Watson: Contributions of Behaviorism to Personality Theory and Measurement Early Behaviorist B. F. Skinner: Radical Behaviorism Arthur Staats: Psychological Behaviorism 1 10/14/08 Conditioning—the process of learning associations . Ivan Pavlov Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning . 1849-1936
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Wrong with the No-Report Paradigm and How to Fix It Ned Block New York University Correspondence: [email protected]
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by PhilPapers Forthcoming in Trends in Cognitive Sciences What is wrong with the no-report paradigm and how to fix it Ned Block New York University Correspondence: [email protected] Key words: consciousness, perception, rivalry, frontal, global workspace, higher order Abstract Is consciousness based in prefrontal circuits involved in cognitive processes like thought, reasoning, and memory or, alternatively, is it based in sensory areas in the back of the neocortex? The no-report paradigm has been crucial to this debate because it aims to separate the neural basis of the cognitive processes underlying post-perceptual decision and report from the neural basis of conscious perception itself. However, the no-report paradigm is problematic because, even in the absence of report, subjects might engage in post-perceptual cognitive processing. Therefore, to isolate the neural basis of consciousness, a no-cognition paradigm is needed. Here, I describe a no-cognition approach to binocular rivalry and outline how this approach can help resolve debates about the neural basis of consciousness. Acknowledgement: Thanks to Jan Brascamp, Susan Carey, Thomas Carlson, David Carmel, David Chalmers, Christof Koch, Hakwan Lau, Matthias Michel, Michael Pitts, Dawid Potgieter and Giulio Tononi for comments on an earlier version. What is the Neural Basis of Consciousness? In recent years the scientific study of consciousness (see Glossary) has focused on finding the neural basis of consciousness in the brain. There are many theories of the neural basis of consciousness, but in broad strokes theories tend to divide on whether consciousness is rooted in the ‘front’ or the ‘back’ of the brain.
    [Show full text]
  • Visible Binocular Beats from Invisible Monocular Stimuli During Binocular Rivalry Thomas A
    Brief Communication 1055 Visible binocular beats from invisible monocular stimuli during binocular rivalry Thomas A. Carlson and Sheng He When two qualitatively different stimuli are presented in rivalry [14–16]. Random dot stereograms can be seen at the same time, one to each eye, the stimuli can either superimposed on, but not interacting with two orthogonal integrate or compete with each other. When they gratings engaged in rivalry, a phenomenon that Wolfe compete, one of the two stimuli is alternately termed ‘trinocular vision’ [5]. Hastorf and Myro also suppressed, a phenomenon called binocular rivalry reported that form and color rivalry could be de-coupled [1,2]. When they integrate, observers see some form of from one another ([17]; and see [5,18] for a general review). the combined stimuli. Many different properties (for Nevertheless, there are also challenges to the coexistence example, shape or color) of the two stimuli can induce of stereopsis and rivalry, particularly for the claim that they binocular rivalry. Not all differences result in rivalry, can be perceived at the same spatial location [6,7]. however. Visual ‘beats’, for example, are the result of integration of high-frequency flicker between the two During the experiment, an observer’s left eye was pre- eyes [3,4], and are thus a binocular fusion phenomenon. sented with a red triangle facing left and the right eye a It remains in dispute whether binocular fusion and green triangle facing right (each side of the triangle rivalry can co-exist with one another [5–7]. Here, we extended 2.1° visual angle), and each was illuminated, report that rivalry and beats, two apparently opposing respectively, with red or green light-emitting diodes (LEDs) phenomena, can be perceived at the same time within from behind a plastic diffuser.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning Theory of Conditioning
    JOURNAL OF CRITICAL REVIEWS ISSN- 2394-5125 VOL 7, ISSUE 08, 2020 Learning Theory of Conditioning Hasan Basri1, Shofia Amin2, Mirsa Umiyati3, Hamid Mukhlis4, Rita Irviani5 1Universitas Islam 45, Bekasi, Indonesia. 2Universitas Jambi, Jambi, Indonesia. 3Universitas Warmadewa, Bali, Indonesia. 4Universitas Aisyah Pringsewu, Lampung, Indonesia 5STMIK Pringsewu, Lampung, Indonesia. E-mail: [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Received: 11.03.2020 Revised: 12.04.2020 Accepted: 28.05.2020 ABSTRACT: This paper presents learning theory of conditioning. According to conditioning theory, learning is a process of change that occurs because of the conditions which then cause a reaction. To make that person study, we must give certain conditions. The most important thing in learning according to conditioning theory is continuous practice. Priority in this theory is learning that occurs automatically. This theory says that all human behavior is also the result of conditioning, that is the result of training or habit of reacting to certain conditions or stimuli experienced in life. The weakness of this theory is that learning only happens automatically and activeness and personal determination in certain learning such as learning about certain skills and habituation in young children. KEYWORDS: learning theory, conditioning, children, student, learning process I. INTRODUCTION In the learning process the priority is how the individual can adapt to environmental stimuli and then this individual can react. The reaction is an attempt to create activities as well as finish them, and finally get result that make changes to the individual as a new thing and increase knowledge. The behavioral change must be in the form of repetitive stimuli that are beneficial to individual and have a positive value in learning new thing.
    [Show full text]
  • Tutorial Stimulus Control: Part II James A
    The Behavior Analyst 1995, 18, 253-269 No. 2 (Fall) Tutorial Stimulus Control: Part II James A. Dinsmoor Indiana University The second part of my tutorial stresses the systematic importance of two parameters of discrimi- nation training: (a) the magnitude of the physical difference between the positive and the negative stimulus (disparity) and (b) the magnitude of the difference between the positive stimulus, in par- ticular, and the background stimulation (salience). It then examines the role these variables play in such complex phenomena as blocking and overshadowing, progressive discrimination training, and the transfer of control by fading. It concludes by considering concept formation and imitation, which are important forms of application, and recent work on equivalence relations. Key words: stimulus control, disparity, salience, blocking, overshadowing, transfer, fading, con- cept formation, imitation, equivalence relations The first part of this tutorial dealt a positive correlation between the S + with the basic principles that account and the primary reinforcer that selects for the acquisition of stimulus control the one relevant stimulus out of the under what is conventionally known as many that impinge upon the organism discrimination training. Among the sa- and transforms it into a conditioned re- lient points that were raised were sug- inforcer of observing behavior. Note gestions that (a) control by antecedent also that if the subject is exposed ex- stimuli is just as important in operant clusively to S + training or to a single as it is in respondent behavior; (b) Pav- period of S + training followed by a lov's conditional stimulus is a discrim- single period of S - training rather than inative stimulus; (c) stimulus general- to a continued alternation, control is ization is not a behavioral process in- less adequate (Honig, Thomas, & Gutt- dependent of and antagonistic to dis- man, 1959; Yarczower & Switalski, crimination but is simply another way 1969).
    [Show full text]
  • How Does Binocular Rivalry Emerge from Cortical
    HOW DOES BINOCULAR RIVALRY EMERGE FROM CORTICAL MECHANISMS OF 3-D VISION? Stephen Grossberg, Arash Yazdanbakhsh, Yongqiang Cao, and Guru Swaminathan1 Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems and Center for Adaptive Systems Boston University 677 Beacon Street, Boston, MA 02215 Phone: 617-353-7858 Fax: 617-353-7755 Submitted: July, 2007 Technical Report CAS/CNS-2007-010 All correspondence should be addressed to Professor Stephen Grossberg Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems Boston University 677 Beacon Street Boston, MA 02215 Phone: 617-353-7858/7857 Fax: 617-353-7755 Email:[email protected] 1 SG was supported in part by the National Science Foundation (NSF SBE-0354378) and the office of Naval Research (ONR N00014-01-1-0624). AY was supported in part by the Air Force Office of Scientific Research (AFOSR F49620-01-1-0397) and the Office of Naval Research (ONR N00014-01-1-0624). YC was supported by the National Science Foundation (NSF SBE-0354378). GS was supported in part by the Air Force Office of Scientific Research (AFOSR F49620-98-1-0108 and F49620-01-1-0397), the National Science Foundation (NSF IIS-97-20333), the Office of Naval Research (ONR N00014-95-1-0657, N00014- 95-1-0409, and N00014-01-1-0624), and the Whitaker Foundation (RG-99-0186). Abstract Under natural viewing conditions, a single depthful percept of the world is consciously seen. When dissimilar images are presented to corresponding regions of the two eyes, binocular rivalry may occur, during which the brain consciously perceives alternating percepts through time. How do the same brain mechanisms that generate a single depthful percept of the world also cause perceptual bistability, notably binocular rivalry? What properties of brain representations correspond to consciously seen percepts? A laminar cortical model of how cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 generate depthful percepts is developed to explain and quantitatively simulate binocular rivalry data.
    [Show full text]
  • Depth Inversion Despite Stereopsis: the Appearance of Random-Dot Stereograms on Surfaces Seen in Reverse Perspective
    Perception, 1979, volume 8, pages 135-142 Depth inversion despite stereopsis: the appearance of random-dot stereograms on surfaces seen in reverse perspective John I Yellott, Jr, Jerry L Kaiwi Cognitive Science Group, School of Social Sciences, University of California at Irvine, Irvine, California 92717, USA Received 20 October 1977, in revised form 18 October 1978 Abstract. Inside-out relief masks of faces can be depth-inverted (i.e. seen in reverse perspective) during close-up binocular viewing. If a random-dot stereogram is projected onto such a mask, stereopsis can be achieved for the stereogram, and its depth planes are correctly seen while the mask itself, including the region covered by the stereogram, is simultaneously perceived as depth- inverted. This demonstration shows that binocular depth inversion cannot be explained by a complete loss of stereoscopic information (e.g. through monocular suppression), or by a process analogous to pseudoscopic viewing whereby retinal disparities are incorporated into perception, but with their signs uniformly reversed. 1 Introduction Reversible perspective is probably most often thought of in connection with ambiguous pictures like the Necker cube (figure la), but it is also well-known that depth reversals can sometimes be experienced with solid objects, and with far more dramatic perceptual consequences. [Gregory (1970) describes these in detail; Helmholtz (1925) reviews references back to 1712, and Hochberg (1972) and Robinson (1972) review the modern literature, which is surprisingly thin.] In this note we use 'depth inversion' to refer specifically to this second kind of reversible perspective, in which real objects (rather than depicted ones) are perceived, from the standpoint of depth, as inside-out, so that their concave surfaces appear convex and vice versa.
    [Show full text]
  • A Survey of 3DTV Displays: Techniques and Technologies
    IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON CIRCUITS AND SYSTEMS FOR VIDEO TECHNOLOGY, VOL. 17, NO. 11, NOVEMBER 2007 1647 A Survey of 3DTV Displays: Techniques and Technologies Philip Benzie, Member, IEEE, John Watson, Senior Member, IEEE, Phil Surman, Ismo Rakkolainen, Klaus Hopf, Hakan Urey, Member, IEEE, Ventseslav Sainov, and Christoph von Kopylow (Invited Paper) Abstract—The display is the last component in a chain of ac- both vertical and horizontal parallax and several viewers are able tivity from image acquisition, compression, coding transmission to see a 3-D image that exhibits no accommodation/convergence and reproduction of 3-D images through to the display itself. rivalry. However, the principal disadvantages of these displays There are various schemes for 3-D display taxonomy; the basic are: the images are generally transparent, the hardware tends categories adopted for this paper are: holography where the image to be complex and non-Lambertian intensity distribution cannot is produced by wavefront reconstruction, volumetric where the be displayed. Multiple image displays take many forms and it is image is produced within a volume of space and multiple image likely that one or more of these will provide the solution(s) for the displays where two or more images are seen across the viewing first generation of 3DTV displays. field. In an ideal world a stereoscopic display would produce Index Terms—Holography, TV, 3-D displays. images in real time that exhibit all the characteristics of the original scene. This would require the wavefront to be reproduced accurately, but currently this can only be achieved using holo- graphic techniques. Volumetric displays provide both vertical and I.
    [Show full text]
  • A Primer on Binocular Rivalry, Including Current Controversies
    A Primer on Binocular Rivalry, Including Current Controversies Randolph Blake Vanderbilt Vision Research Center Vanderbilt University Nashville TN 37240 USA running head: Binocular Rivalry email: [email protected] voice: 615-343-7010 FAX: 615-343-5029 Binocular Rivalry Abstract. Among psychologists and vision scientists, binocular rivalry has enjoyed sustained interest for decades dating back to the 19th century. In recent years, however, rivalry’s audience has expanded to include neuroscientists who envision rivalry as a “tool” for exploring the neural concomitants of conscious visual awareness and perceptual organization. For rivalry’s potential to be realized, workers using this “tool” need to know details of this fascinating phenomenon, and providing those details is the purpose of this article. After placing rivalry in a historical context, I summarize major findings concerning the spatial characteristics and the temporal dynamics of rivalry, discuss two major theoretical accounts of rivalry (“eye” vs “stimulus” rivalry) and speculate on possible neural concomitants of binocular rivalry. key words: binocular rivalry, suppression, conscious awareness, neural model, perceptual organization 1. Introduction The human brain has been touted as the most complex structure in the known universe (Thompson, 1985). This may be true, but despite its awesome powers the brain can behave like a confused adolescent when it is confronted with conflicting visual messages. When dissimilar visual stimuli are imaged on corresponding retinal regions of the two eyes, the brain lapses into an unstable state characterized by alternating periods of perceptual dominance during which one visual stimulus or the other is seen at a time. This confusion is understandable, for the eyes are signalling the brain that two different objects exist at the same location in space at the same time.
    [Show full text]
  • Cholinergic Modulation of Binocular Vision
    5208 • The Journal of Neuroscience, July 1, 2020 • 40(27):5208–5213 Development/Plasticity/Repair Cholinergic Modulation of Binocular Vision Yasha Sheynin,1 Pedro Rosa-Neto,3 Robert F. Hess,1* and Elvire Vaucher2* 1McGill Vision Research, McGill University, Montréal, Quebec H3G 1A4, Canada, 2Laboratoire de Neurobiologie de la Cognition Visuelle, École d’Optométrie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Quebec H3T 1P1, Canada, and 3Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montréal, Quebec H4H 1R3, Canada The endogenous neurotransmitter acetylcholine (ACh) is known to affect the excitatory/inhibitory (E/I) balance of primate visual cortex, enhancing feedforward thalamocortical gain while suppressing corticocortical synapses. Recent advances in the study of the human visual system suggest that ACh is a likely component underlying interocular interactions. However, our understanding of its precise role in binocular processes is currently lacking. Here we use binocular rivalry as a probe of interocular dynamics to determine ACh’s effects, via the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor (AChEI) donepezil, on the binocular visual system. A total of 23 subjects (13 male) completed two crossover experimental sessions where binocular rivalry measurements were obtained before and after taking either donepezil (5 mg) or a placebo (lactose) pill. We report that enhanced cholinergic potentiation attenuates perceptual suppression during binocular rivalry, reducing the overall rate of interocular competition while enhancing the visibility of superimposition mixed percepts. Considering recent evidence that perceptual suppression during binocular rivalry is causally modulated by the inhibitory neurotransmitter GABA, our results suggest that cholinergic activity counteracts the effect of GABA with regards to interocular dynamics and may modulate the inhibitory drive within the visual cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • ©Elliott King, 2018 “The Spectator Makes the Picture”: Optical Illusions
    ©Elliott King, 2018 “The Spectator Makes the Picture”: Optical illusions and Viewer Experience in Dalí’s and Duchamp’s Stereoscopic Works By Elliott H. King Abstract: Dalí and Duchamp shared a well-established interest in perception and optics that included a number of experiments with 3-D optical illusions. Using some of the same historical source materials, plausibly in dialogue, both artists created stereograms and anaglyphs, though ostensibly to different ends. Duchamp’s numerous stereoscopic works, beginning around 1918 and extending to his last pieces in 1968, have been credited variously to his erudite studies of perspective, the fourth dimension, and the viewer’s role in “completing” the artwork; Dalí’s 1970s stereoscopic canvases, by contrast, are generally framed facilely as somewhat gimmicky efforts at achieving heightened illusionism. This essay considers both artists’ stereo works in terms of the viewer experience Duchamp emphasized. I am especially interested in those pieces that create stereoscopic “dissonance” by combining two wholly dissimilar pictures. Though one might readily compare the resulting images to multiple-exposure photographs (as others have done), a viewer’s experience of “dissonant” stereoscopy and single-image photography is distinct. Arguably even more so than in traditional stereoscopy, these “dissonant” stereo works give rise to illusory colors and oscillating figures that underscore the role of the eye and brain in constructing (the illusion of) objective reality – a subject in which both artists, in their particular ways, were deeply invested. 1 Avant-garde Studies Issue 3, Spring/Summer 2018 ©Elliott King, 2018 Among a number of affinities that have come to light thanks especially to the 2017-2018 exhibition Dalí/Duchamp and its accompanying catalogue, both Salvador Dalí and Marcel Duchamp shared an avid interest in perception and 3-D optical illusions, specifically stereoscopy.
    [Show full text]
  • Configural Associations in Discrimination Learning
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Animal Behavior Processes 0097-7403/90,r$00.75 1990. Vol. 16, No. 3, 250-261 Configural Associations in Discrimination Learning John M. Pearce and Paul N. Wilson University of Wales, College of Cardiff, Cardiff, Wales In 4 experiments, Sprague-Dawley rats and homing pigeons received training with an A+ AB0 BC+ discrimination, in which food (+) accompanied trials with A and EtC. Food was not presented (0) on trials with the compound AB. Subsequent test trials revealed that responding during C by itself, or the compound ABC, was slower than during either A or BC. Responding during the ABC compound was also found to be slower after training with the A+ AB0 BC+ than an A0 AB+ BC+ discrimination. We argue that these findings demonstrate the importance of configural associations in discrimination learning. Two accounts for the way in which these associations exert their influence are considered. A major concern relating to theories of conditioning has Saavedra, 1975; Whitlow & Wagner, 1972; Woodbury, 1943~ been to identify the associations that are formed when a For example, animals can solve a negative patterning problem compound conditioned stimulus (CS) is paired with an un- in which the US follows two stimuli when they are presented conditioned stimulus (US). By far the most influential line of separately but not when they are presented together (Bel- theorizing has been to assume that this training provides the lingham et al., 1985; Rescorla, 1972; Woodbury, 1943). Ac- potential for each stimulus within the compound to enter cording to the Rescorla and Wagner (1972) model, without separately into an association with the US (Frey & Sears, the influence of confignral cues, this discrimination would be 1978; Mackintosh, 1975; Moore & Stickney, 1980; Pearce & insoluble, because it is impossible for a CS by itself to signal Hall, 1980; Rescorla & Wagner, 1972).
    [Show full text]