Genetic variation in light vision and light-dependent movement behaviour in the eyeless Collembola Folsomia candida Marta Gallardo Ruiz, Jean-François Le Galliard, Thomas Tully

To cite this version:

Marta Gallardo Ruiz, Jean-François Le Galliard, Thomas Tully. Genetic variation in light vision and light-dependent movement behaviour in the eyeless Collembola Folsomia candida. Pedobiologia, Elsevier, 2017, 61, pp.33-41. ￿10.1016/j.pedobi.2016.12.001￿. ￿hal-02344732￿

HAL Id: hal-02344732 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02344732 Submitted on 16 Oct 2020

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Genetic variation in light vision and light-dependent movement behaviour in the eyeless Collembola Folsomia candida

Marta Gallardo Ruiza (a) Jean-François Le Galliard (a,b), Thomas Tully (a,c)* a Sorbonne Université, CNRS, IRD, INRA, Institut d’écologie et des sciences de l’environnement (IEES-Paris), Paris, France b Ecole Normale Supérieure, PSL Research University, CNRS, Centre de recherche en écologie ex- périmentale et prédictive (CEREEP-Ecotron IleDeFrance), UMS 3194, 78 rue du château, 77140 Saint-Pierre-les-Nemours, France c Sorbonne Université, INSPE de l'académie de Paris, Paris, France * [email protected] Published in Pedobiologia in 2017. Gallardo Ruiz, M., Le Galliard, J.-F., & Tully, T. (2017). Genetic variation in light vision and light-depen- dent movement behaviour in the eyeless Collembola Folsomia candida. Pedobiologia, 61, 33-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pedobi.2016.12.001

ABSTRACT may show contrasting habitat prefer- ences depending on their sex, stage, size and can cope with spatiotemporal variation genetic background for instance (Stamps, 2001; in their environment through mobility and selec- Matthysen, 2012). This intra-specific variation of tive habitat choice. Intra-specific variation in behavioural responses may influence a wide habitat choice has been documented especially range of eco-evolutionary processes (Sih et al., for host plant preferences and cryptic habitat se- 2012; Ronce and Clobert, 2012; Edelaar and Bol- lection in insects. Here, we investigated the ge- nick, 2012). Inter-individual differences in habitat netic variation in light sensitivity and light-depen- choice behaviours may be genotype-dependent dent habitat choice in the eyeless Collembola and related to differential performance in specific Folsomia candida with a choice test under four habitats or niches (Sih et al., 2012; Edelaar and different lighting conditions (control dark condi- Bolnick, 2012; Hawthorne and Via, 2001; Cousyn tion, two simulations of undergrowth natural light et al., 2001; De Meester, 1996; Jaenike and Holt, conditions and red light). We tested twelve clonal 1991). Genetic variation in habitat choice behav- strains from diverse geographical origins that are iour is common in animals and has been well clustered in two evolutionary clades with con- documented for host plant preferences and cryp- trasting fast or slow life-history strategies. The tic habitat selection in insects (reviewed in clones differed in their mean movement probabili- Jaenike and Holt, 1991). ties in the dark treatment. These differences were One of the cues animals use to select their habi- related to the two different phylogenetic clades, tat is light which animals may be positively or where fast-life history clones are on average negatively attracted to. For example, negative more mobile than slow-life history counterparts phototaxis acts as a predator-avoidance mecha- as predicted by the ‘colonizer syndrome’ hypoth- nism in some aquatic organisms (De Meester, esis. We found behavioural avoidance of light in 1996; Cousyn et al., 2001; Michels and De the three light conditions. Moreover, photophobia Meester, 2004; Borowsky, 2011) and may help was stronger when the simulated light spectrum some soil organisms that are very sensitive to rel- was brighter and included non-red light. Photo- ative humidity to prevent desiccation by looking phobia was similar among all clonal lineages and for deeper and more humid soil layers (Salmon et between the two clades, which suggests that this al., 2014; Salmon and Ponge, 2012). Neverthe- behaviour is a shared behavioural trait in this less, different individuals or populations within species. We discuss the use of light as an envi- the same species may present different photo- ronmental cue for orientation, displacement and tactic preferences. For example, clonal popula- habitat choice under natural conditions. tions of Daphnia magna (a zooplancton species) 1. Introduction exposed to high levels of predatory pressure are more photophobic than clonal populations less Animals use various environmental cues for habi- exposed to predation (Cousyn et al., 2001; De tat choices, and different individuals from a single

1 Meester, 1996). In general, differences in pho- most studies using F. candida as a model species totaxis may have a heritable, genetic basis (e.g., have used parthenogenetic lineages (Fountain Markow and Smith, 1977; De Meester, 1996; and Hopkin, 2005). Earlier studies on several Cousyn et al., 2001) or could be the result of non- parthenogenetic lineages have uncovered sub- genetic, phenotypic plasticity and personality (is stantial intra-specific genetic and morphological this too anthropomorphic for Daphnia or spring- polymor- phism (Chenon et al., 2000; Tully et al., tails?) differences among individuals (e.g.,Kain et 2006; Tully and Potapov, 2015). Intra-specific di- al., 2012). Quantifying sources of variation in versity is organised in two major evolution- ary phototaxis is therefore important to understand clades (Tully et al., 2006; Tully and Potapov, the evolution of this wide- spread behavioural 2015), and life history studies have shown that trait. Here, we investigate the genetic variation in two contrasted biodemographic strategies light sensitivity and light-dependent movement evolved along the divergence of these two clades behaviour in the eye-less Folsomia can- (Tully and Ferriere, 2008; Tully, 2004; Tully and dida Willem 1902 (Collembola, ), an Lambert, 2011; Mallard et al., 2015). One clade hemi-edaphic and cosmopolitan soil organism in- has a high reproductive potential: when sufficient habiting various habitats such as caves, forest lit- food is available, these produce on av- ter and man- made habitats (Fountain and Hop- erage larger clutches than the ones from the oth- kin, 2005). er clade (Tully and Ferriere, 2008), but they have shorter mean lifespans than the less fecund clade The degeneration or even loss of the visual sys- and also reach a smaller adult body size (Tully tem is a convergent and frequent evolutionary and Ferriere, 2008; Tully and Lambert, 2011; Mal- phenomenon in soil- dwelling and cave animals lard et al., 2015). These two groups of clonal lin- (Christiansen, 2005). Nevertheless, even eyeless eages fit well to the typical slow (A) and fast (B) and eye-reduced species often retain some sen- life history syndromes (or r-K life histories, see sitivity to the ambient light level through extra-oc- Reznick et al., 2002). But, until now, the ecologi- ular photoreceptors (EOP, Taddei-Ferretti and cal conditions in which they have evolved and the Musio, 2000; Ullrich-Lüter et al., 2011), which are time elapsed since the divergence of the two useful for the maintenance of circadian rhythms clades remain to be determined. Intra-specific (Friedrich, 2013) or for orientation and habitat variation in habitat choice behaviour and mobility choice (Timmermann and Plath, 2008; Borowsky, has so far neither been examined in this species 2011). Indeed, previous works strongly suggest nor in other Collembola. Instead, the few works that F. candida is sensitive to light despite being that relate the habitat preference or distribution eyeless. In choice-test experiments, F. candida and colonization ability of Collembola with their avoids UV light moving to warmer locations ex- morphological and life history traits are focused posed to white light, prefers darkness over cool on the study of collembolan community composi- white light (Fox et al., 2007), and displays a dose- tion (Salmon et al., 2014; Ponge and Salmon, response avoidance of UV-B light relative to 2013; Huebner et al., 2012; Salmon and Ponge, darkness (Beresford et al., 2013). Yet, to our 2012; Ponge et al., 2006). Intraspecific variation knowledge, no study has examined the wave- in phototactic behaviour and life history traits has lengths of maximum sensitivity of the ocular or been well investigated in Daphnia magna. In this extra-ocular photoreceptors (EOPs)of these ani- species, positive phototactic clones present a mals (Barra, 1971; Jordana et al., 2000; Fox et fast life history strategy whereas negative and in- al., 2007). In true insects (Pterygota), few species termediate phototactic clones present a slow life are able to detect wavelengths longer than 600 history strategy (e.g., De Meester, 1994). nm (red light), which suggests a red-blind com- mon ancestor (Briscoe and Chittka, 2001). In ad- We tested if springtail clonal variation in light-de- dition, the behavioural tests mentioned above pendent habitat choice exists using an experi- could not always prevent confounding effects of mental setup to control the lighting conditions differences in temperature or humidity associated while maintaining constant temperature and with the lighting treatment. This is of great impor- moisture. We tracked springtail movements un- tance, as F. candida needs a relative humidity der this setup to quantify their spatial preference close to saturation (Holmstrup, 2002; Waagner et for shaded versus illuminated areas as a measure al., 2011) and is very sensitive to temperature of the habitat choice behaviour. We first tested (Boiteau and Mackinley, 2012; Boiteau and whether F. candida can use light as an environ- MacKinley, 2013). mental cue for habitat choice under different lighting conditions, including natural shaded and Although sexual reproduction exists in some sunny understory spectra and an artificial red- populations of F. candida (Frati et al., 2004), this light spectrum. We measured springtail sensitivity species is generally recognized as asexual, and

2 to long wavelengths because these wavelengths plaster of Paris mixed with Indian ink to increase are dominant under the forest canopy (Smith, visual detectability of individuals (Tully and Fer- 1994) while red and far red are the principal riere, 2008). Populations were kept in incubators wavelengths that penetrate the soil (Bliss and at 20°C (+/-0.5°C) in the dark and fed with pellets Smith, 1985). We hypothesised that F. candida of a mixture of agar and dried yeast (Tully and should not detect or react to red and far-red light Ferriere, 2008). We established synchronised per se if the incapability to detect these wave- populations of each clone in the same way and at lengths was a shared condition of most true in- the same time by transferring 10–12 randomly sects and Collembola, even though most spring- chosen adult females from stock cultures to new tails are sensitive to heat generated by red light culture vials. Females were transferred to new (Briscoe and Chittka, 2001). We further studied vials every week and old vials were kept at 20 C the sensitivity to light of twelve clonal lineages of for laid eggs to hatch since this temperature is F. candida including eleven lineages belonging to optimal for all lineages. Then, vials were checked the two evolutionary clades described earlier (Tul- weekly for the presence of new-borns. If new- ly et al., 2006; Tully and Ferriere, 2008; Tully and borns were detected, ad libitum food was provid- Lambert, 2011; Tully and Potapov, 2015). We ad- ed weekly to ensure optimum body growth. dressed the following questions: Do light sensi- Therefore, the age of the experimental animals tivity and habitat choice behaviour vary between was known with a one-week accuracy. However, clonal lineages, as has been found in other taxa due to the low fecundity of some clones or acci- (Jaenike and Holt, 1991; De Meester, 1996; dental flooding in some vials, we had to add Cousyn et al., 2001)? If such clonal variation ex- medium size (~1.4mm) adults of unknown age ists, how is it organised relative to the phyloge- taken from the stock cultures in order to keep netic clades and what are the links between the balanced samples for all clones and treatments behavioural responses and the main life history (see Table S1 for details on sample size and indi- strategies of each clade? We predicted that lin- vidual characteristics). eages from the slow life history group would be more photophobic (De Meester, 1994, 1995), giv- 2.2. Measurement of natural light en that photophobia is likely to be associated conditions with life in more stable habitats, which usually se- At midday of a sunny day in June 2013, we lects for a slow life history (Pianka, 1970; Reznick measured 23 light spectra under the canopy of a et al., 2002). temperate forest located in the Foljuif field sta- tion, 80km South of Paris, France 2. Materials and methods (48 17013.96“N, 2 40040.34“E), where popula- 2.1. Maintenance and origin of the tions of F. candida have been previously found. We recorded 19 spectra in the shade and four studied springtails spectra on sunspots in the undergrowth. The cli- We used twelve clonal lineages of the Collembola mate conditions and plant community structure Folsomia candida labelled AP, BR, BV, DK, GB, of this forest have been studied extensively GM, HA, ME, PB, TO, US and WI (Tully et al., (Blandin et al., 1980). We used a handheld spec- 2006). Information about the phylogenetic rela- trome- ter parameterized for absolute irradiance tion- ships and habitat and geographical origin of measurements in the range 200–850 nm (Jaz Se- all strains except ME can be found in previous ries, JAZ-ULM-200, Ocean Optics, USA). We studies (Tully et al., 2006; Tully and Potapov, measured the incident light with a cosine-correct- 2015). Clones AP, BV, BR, GB and HA belong to ed probe (180 field of view) after dark calibration the “slow clade” A while the “fast clade” B com- and converted data into units of irradiance spec- prises the clones DK, GM, PB, TO, US and WI trum (mW cm 2) using factory calibration of the (Tully and Ferriere, 2008). The new clone ME was spectrophotometer. Measurements were taken at collected in November 2013 from some decaying ground levels under the canopy of different tree wood beams into an abandoned man-made and bush species, as well as under sunspots un- tunnel in the Mercantour French National Park der the trees and into tree stump holes to sample (South-East of France, 44°7.0260N, 7°16.7270E, different light spectra that natural populations of 1530 m). The life history strategy of this clone spring- tails are likely to encounter aboveground. and its phylogenetic relationships with the other clones are currently unknown. 2.3. Simulation of environmental conditions All clonal populations were reared in similar con- ditions in polyethylene vials (inner diameter 52 Artificial light and constant climate conditions mm, height 65 mm) filled with a 30 mm layer of were simulated in a controlled environment labo- ratory at the CEREEP Ecotron - Ile De France

3 (Saint-Pierre-les-Nemours, France). We used one centre, so that springtails could move freely between each 13 m3 controlled environment chamber of the side of the box. b) Photograph of the LEDs modules. Ecolab, where climate (temperature, relative hu- Light was provided by means of modular LED midity, rainfall) and lighting conditions can be (light emitting diodes) arrays (n = 40) allowing to simulated (Verdier et al., 2014). In the centre of turn on or off cool white LEDs (n=15 per array) the chamber, we installed vials for the characteri- and five different LED types with maximum emis- sation of photophobia in springtails (see below) sion wavelengths in the UV (370nm, n=3), green on a controlled water table to maintain constant (520nm, n=2), red (660nm, n=5), far red (740nm, humidity and temperature conditions in the vials n=7), and infrared (840 nm, n = 3). Thus, up to during the observations. Controlling for constant 36,864 combinations of LEDs parameters can be humidity and temperature was essential to test programmed. To automatise the simulation of the exclusively for the effect of light, since slight most appropriate lighting conditions, we wrote a changes in humidity and temperature could mod- procedure in R (The R and Development Core ify the springtails’ behaviour and thus affect their Team, 2012) to calculate the sum of squared dis- apparent or real photophobia (Holmstrup, 2002; tance between the reference spectrum and the Boiteau and Mackinley, 2012). Atmospheric tem- simulated spectrum, and to select the combina- perature in the chamber was maintained at 20°C tion of parameters that minimised this sum. We (+/-0.1°C) and temperature of the water table was simulated the “average undergrowth” light spec- set at 10°C. We used a cold water table to keep trum by producing a spectrum which best the temperature inside the vials slightly lower matched the mean spectra of the 23 light than the air temperature to prevent condensation measurements made at various places on the for- on the transparent lid that covered the vials (Fig. est floor (see Fig. 2a and Table S2 provided as 1). The temperature measured inside the vials Supplementary information). was 19°C and the relative humidity was 93% (DS-1923 iButton loggers) irrespective of the test conditions.

Figure 2: Light conditions used during the experiment. a) Natural and simulated average under-canopy spectra and experimental red light spectrum. The three spectra have equivalent total power. b) Natural and simulated sunspots under the canopy spectra. The two spectra have equivalent total power. We also produced a “maximum undergrowth spectrum”, using the four measurements made on sunspots, which should correspond to the strongest undergrowth light conditions that litter- dwelling springtails are susceptible to be ex- posed to in this forest (Fig. 2b and Table S2). We expected a stronger light-avoidance response under this condition compared to under the mean undergrowth light spectrum. In addition, we sim- ulated a “red light” spectrum whose power was Figure 1: Fig. 1. a) Photographs of the experimental boxes. Boxes were wrapped with a thin flexible black plastic sheet close to that of the mean undergrowth light spec- (1), and only exposed to light from above. Boxes were trum (Fig. 2a and Table S2). This red light spec- covered with a transparent plastic lid (2) to prevent desicca- trum has two irradiance peaks (red at 660 nm tion and disturbance due to airflow. The arrow shows the and far red at 740nm) and has no UV light that gap between the plaster of Paris floor (3) and the opaque could promote photophobia in springtails (Fox et black piece of plastic (4) that descended vertically along the al., 2007; Beresford et al., 2013). We further com-

4 pared these three lighting conditions with a con- scripts of the analysis, and figures are provided trol treatment where the room was maintained in as Supplementary information. We calculated the the dark. This condition was used to verify movement (or transition) probabilities (the proba- whether the experimental set-up in itself could bility that an individual has moved from one side affect the behaviour of the springtails and to to the other side of the vial after one hour), using compare the mobility of the different clones in the pairs of consecutive observations of the same in- dark. dividual to form a binomial variable which equals zero when the individual stayed in the same half 2.4. Experimental protocol of the vial and equals one when the had The dark, average undergrowth and red light moved to the other. This binomial variable was spectra were tested in June 2014, and the maxi- analysed with nested generalised linear mixed mum undergrowth spectrum was tested in De- models (GLMMs) for repeated measurements ap- cember 2014. We used 36 individuals of each proach with a binomial error distribution and logit clonal lineage in each light treatment (n = 1728), link function, using treatment (light spectrum and six individuals per clone were tested every type), position (dark or light) and clonal group as day. Springtails were photographed to measure the fixed factors, and individual as the random their length (mean = 1.41 mm, range = 0.93–2.19 factor. Observations were thus clustered in indi- mm) and kept isolated without food in rearing viduals and individuals were nested within vials the day before. These individuals were clones. In the full model, we analysed all main transferred the next morning to new rearing vials effects as well as two and three-way interactions. wrapped with a thin flexible black plastic sheet, We implemented the full model with the glmer and only exposed to light from above. Each plas- procedure in package lme4 for R 3.1.2 (Bates et tic box was filled with plaster of Paris and had a al., 2012; The R and Development Core Team, hole in the bottom to ensure that the plaster can 2012) and tested for the significance of fixed be kept constantly wet to saturation (Fig. 1). An effects with Chi-square tests. Since the three- opaque black piece of plastic covered half of the way interaction Spectrum*Position*Clone was vial’s opening and descended vertically along the significant (see Table S3 provided as Supplemen- centre of the vial down to 3 mm above the plaster tary information), we analysed each treatment in- level, in order to maintain in the dark half of the dependently to get a better understanding of the vial while allowing the springtail to move freely behavioural differences among clones under between the two halves of the vials. Preliminary each treatment. experiments showed that in total darkness For subsequent analyses we further included the springtail behaviour is very sensitive to drought body length of each individual in the analysis to and slight humidity differences between the two test for a size effect on individual movement sides of the vials. The vials were thus covered probability. Since mean body length can vary with transparent lids to prevent any desiccation. substantially among clones (Mallard et al., 2015), When being transferred, animals were separated we calculated for each treatment the corrected randomly in two groups of same size and re- body lengths as the sum of the overall mean leased in the bright and dark sides at the begin- body length and the body length residuals com- ning of the experiment (here called ‘position’) re- puted here as the difference between individual spectively, such that the proportion of animals in lengths and mean length of their clone. This each position was exactly 50% for each clone “corrected body length” was then used to study and treatment. One observer (MGR) studied the the effect of body length while controlling for springtails’ movements between the two sides of differences among clones. The full models in- the vials by recording the position of each animal cluded position, clone and corrected length as every hour during six hours (from 9 am to 5 pm). fixed factors, the two-way interactions and indi- For the control dark treatment, we observed the vidual identity as the random factor. We used a springtails’ positions as quickly as possible using backward stepwise selection to get final models a soft light head torch to minimise disturbance. that only included significant factors and interac- At the end of the experiment, we inspected all tions. We also report full models in Table S4 fol- vials to check that no individual had escaped or lowing Forstmeier and Schielzeth’s recommenda- deceased to ensure valid observations when the tion (Forstmeier and Schielzeth, 2011). For the full springtails did not move. and final models, we calculated marginal and 2.5. Statistical analyses conditional R2, which quantify the amount of We used the software R (The R and Development variance explained by the models (Nakagawa Core Team, 2012) for the statistical analysis and and Schielzeth, 2013). We additionally performed to produce the graphs. The raw data and the R- GLMMs with clade, position and their interaction

5 as fixed factors, and clone and individual within er propensity to switch between sides (0.44 per clone as nested random factors to test for phylo- hour) than clones with a slow life-history strategy genetic variation. The clone ME, whose evolutio- (0.34, Fig. 4a). Contrary to our expectations, F. nary clade has not yet been characterized, was candida proved to be slightly sensitive to red light excluded from these analyses. (Table 2), independently of their length: springtails preferentially moved from red light to dark (0.46 per hour) than from dark to red light (0.42, see Fig. 3). Clones also differed in their mean mobili- ty, but these differences were not due to clade (x21 = 0.07, p = 0.79, Fig. 4b).

Figure 3: Models that best describe transition probabilities in the dark treatment after backward selection (N = 431). Dropped variables are presented in order in which they were removed. Marginal and conditional R2 of the final model are 0.111 respectively.

Table 1: Models that best describe transition probabilities in the dark treatment after backward selection (N = 431). Dropped variables are presented in order in which they were removed. Marginal and conditional R2 of the final model are 0.111 respectively.

Figure 4: Mean movement probability between the two sides of the test boxes (95% CI) for each clone. Black sym- bols denote clones with a slow life-history strategy; white symbols denote clones with a fast life-history strategy; and the grey symbol denotes the clone ME whose life-history Table 2: Models that best describe transition probabilities in strategy remains uncharacterised. Mean values were aver- the red light treatment after backward selection (N = 432). aged over the two sides of the test boxes. Marginal and conditional R2 of the final model are 0.015 and 0.068 respectively. Springtails showed photophobia when exposed to average undergrowth light: they moved from 3. Results the light to the dark side significantly more often In the homogenous dark environment, the mobili- than in the reverse direction (0.43 versus 0.37, ty of spring- tails was not influenced by their po- see Fig. 3 and Table 3). The different clones (x211 sition in the vial (Table 1; Fig. 3) and was inde- = 18.09, p = 0.08) and the two clades (x21 = pendent of their body length. The individual 0.40, p = 0.40) had similar movement probabili- mobility (average transition probability) differed ties (Fig. 4c and Table 3). In this light condition, significantly among clones (Table 1, Fig. 4a), and movement probability varied with body length this variability was mostly due to difference be- depend- ing on position: while in the dark com- tween the two evolutionary clades (x21 = 15.86, partment the length did not influence mobility, p < 0.001). In complete darkness, the clones with but when exposed to light, smaller than average a fast life history (clade B) had on average a high- springtails were more prone to move than longer

6 ones (Fig. 5a). When exposed to maximum un- dergrowth light, springtails moved away on aver- age more often (0.45 per hour) than when they were in the dark side of the vials (0.36, see Fig. 3 and Table 4). Differences among clones (x211 = 15.67, p = 0.15) and clades were not significant (x21 = 1.04, p = 0.31, Fig. 4d), and there was a non-significant trend (Position* Corrected Length: x211 = 1.22, p = 0.27) for longer than average in- dividuals to move less often than smaller individ- uals, as observed in the average undergrowth light treatment (Table 4). However, there was a significant interaction between effects of clone and of corrected length: for most clones, size matters little and smaller individuals tended to move more, while for clone GB the mobility was positively influenced by relative length (x211 = 21.49, p = 0.03) (Fig. 5b) Clone GB was charac- terised by the slowest life history strategy.

Figure 5: a) Predicted mean movement probability between the two sides as a function of individuals’ length, for each position in the average undergrowth spectrum. White box- plots and dotted line denote that the individual was on the bright side prior to the observation, while grey boxplots and solid line denote that the individual was on the dark side. The dashed-line represent the predicted response of the in- dividuals that were on the bright side prior to the observa- tion, while the solid line represent the predicted response of the individual that were on the dark side. b) Predicted move- ment probability between the two sides as a function of indi- Table 3: Models that best describe transition probabilities in viduals’ length for each clone in the maximum undergrowth the average undergrowth spectrum after backward selection spectrum. (N = 432). Marginal and conditional R2 of the final model are Differences in temperature between the illuminat- 0.011 and 0.074 respectively. ed and dark sides of the boxes fell within the measurement error of our thermometer devices (<0.5°C). Thus, even though we cannot strictly exclude that the bright side may be slightly warmer than the dark side, the potential tempera- ture difference was so small that we could not detect it. Thus we interpret our result as a light sensitivity but one has to remember that temper- ature gradients could also play a role. For in- stance, temperature receptors such as micro se- tae on the antennas or legs may help springtails Table 4: Models that best describe transition probabilities in the maximum undergrowth spectrum after backward selec- to perceive small temperature gradients, in par- tion (N = 432). Marginal and conditional R2 of the final model ticular when doing ‘turning’ movements (loops), a are 0.039 and 0.089 respectively. frequent behaviour when in an unknown environ- ment (Bengtsson et al., 2004). 4. Discussion In addition, our experimental design is not suited 4.1. Limits of the experimental protocol to know how many times springtails moved be- Our experimental protocol allowed us to study tween sides during the one hour census interval. the behaviour of isolated individuals in darkness Our estimations of transition probabilities are and under three light conditions while maintaining based on the hypothesis that each springtail constant temperature and humidity. As the does no or only one move in an hour, which may springtails’ mobility is very sensitive to these en- be wrong given that some individuals may have vironmental factors, this represents a significant moved multiple times between two censuses. Fu- improvement to test for photophobia. ture studies should track more continuously indi- viduals through space to obtain better estimates

7 of dispersal distances and capacities. Wilkens and Larimer, 1976), remains an open question. 4.2. Light sensitivity and its ecological significance Photophobia may help springtails to avoid open spaces when they forage in the litter close to the We found that, despite being eyeless, F. candida surface, and together with positive geotaxis is slightly but significantly sensitive to light. Pho- (Boiteau and MacKinley, 2014), it may serve for tophobia was detected in the presence of the orientation in vertical movements. Being able to three light sources, including two light spectra avoid open spaces and the surface of the litter chosen to mimic natural conditions encountered may provide several benefits. First of all, all clonal by springtails. Hence, this study confirms earlier lineages tested here were non-pigmented and general findings of negative phototaxis in this thus little protected against the deleterious species (Fox et al., 2007; Beresford et al., 2013; effects of UV radiations. Long-term (two weeks) Boiteau and MacKinley, 2014; Salmon and exposure to UV-B light increases mortality and Ponge, 1998). But our study goes one step fur- causes DNA damage in this species (Beresford et ther by showing that the intensity of photophobia al., 2013). Behavioural tactics of habitat selection response varied depending on the light spec- for dark sheltered zones or deeper soil layers can trum: Light avoidance by springtails increased help springtails to escape from potentially dan- from the red light spectrum at an intensity equiv- gerous UV-radiation. This may also explain why alent to that of the average light spectrum under unpigmented or slightly pigmented Collembola the canopy, to average undergrowth spectrum, are preferentially distributed in edaphic or hemi- and to maximum undergrowth spectrum. F. can- edaphic rather than epigeal habitats and in dida is thus sensitive both to light intensity and to woodlands rather than in grasslands (Salmon et its quality. This suggests that despite being blind, al., 2014; Salmon and Ponge, 2012). However, these springtails can use light as an environmen- the photophobia of F. candida cannot be exclu- tal cue for orientation, movement and habitat sively related to UV-radiation avoidance, since F. choice under natural conditions. Light avoidance candida was also sensitive to red light. Long- behaviour was not very strong, because individu- waves are dominant wave- lengths under the for- als also moved from the dark to the illuminated est canopy (Smith, 1994), and thus sensitivity to side of the vials and switched their position fre- these wavelengths could be related to a more quently. This may be due to the fact that the in- general aboveground avoidance. Moreover, as tensity of the light spectrum tested was low com- ambient light level is often correlated with other pared for example to direct sunlight, but could climatic factors (notably temperature and humidi- also be related to the small spatial scale of the ty), photophobia is useful to target deeper, more setup and the short time scale of our tests. humid and more stable microhabitats preferred Multiple examples of eye-reduced and eyeless by most Collembola species, especially those species that retain brightness and colour sensi- that are the most vulnerable to desiccation tivity exist (e.g. Ramirez et al., 2011; Friedrich, (Salmon et al., 2014; Salmon and Ponge, 2012). 2013). Extra-ocular photoreceptors (EOP) or non- Another explanation is that photophobia may in- visual photoreceptors can be responsible of light directly help springtails to avoid predators. Being sensitivity when eyes are absent. EOP exist in the devoid of compound eyes F. candida probably form of dermal photoreceptors in invertebrates cannot form images and thus cannot see ap- (Tosini and Avery, 1996; Binder and McDonald, proaching predators (Hauzy et al., 2010), as it is 2008; Xiang et al., 2010; Ullrich-Lüter et al., the case of the sister eyeless species Folsomia fi- 2011), the sixth caudal ganglion in decapods mentaria (Baatrup et al., 2006). This species is (Wilkens and Larimer, 1976; Larimer, 1966), gen- thus very vulnerable in illuminated environments italia (Arikawa et al., 1980) and neurons of the op- to visually active-chasing predators such as tic lobes in butterflies (Lampel et al., 2005), and hunting ground-beetles, some of which are fero- photosensitive cells in the hydra (Taddei-Ferretti cious predators of springtails (Ernsting, 1977). and Musio, 2000). F. candida lacks external eye 4.3. Intra-specific variability in move- facets and any other external eye structures as revealed by scanning electron microscopy (Fox et ment and light sensitivity al., 2007). Whether F. candida is only facet-less In general, smaller individuals were more mobile but has internal photoreceptors where eyes than larger ones and we found that smaller should be or whether it uses other types of extra- springtails exposed to light were also more likely ocular photoreception such as a dispersed pho- to seek refuge in the dark side than the larger toreception dermal sense (Ramirez et al., 2011) or ones in the average undergrowth light treatment. neural photoreception (Lampel et al., 2005; The risk of staying in an illuminated environment

8 may not be the same for large and small individu- probabilities translate into clonal differences in als. For example, it has been shown that the long-distance movement behaviour and therefore carabid Notiophilus biguttatus a common visual dispersal distances. hunter and voracious predator of springtails Studies on the intraspecific variation of pho- preys more efficiently on small rather large totaxis are scarce but differences in phototaxis Collembola (Ernsting and Mulder, 1981). This in- among genetically distinct lineages have been re- terpretation is tenuous given that the effect of ported in several species. It has been shown that body size on movement was small, variable several Daphnia clonal populations differ in their across clones and not repeatable across light photophobia in response to contrasting predatory treatments. These variable effects of body size pressures (Cousyn et al., 2001; De Meester, are difficult to explain. The intensity of photopho- 1996). Artificially selected Drosphila strains also bia may also depend on other individual state pa- differ in their phototactic preferences both in rameters such as the nutritional status or the quality and degree as a result of different phase in the moulting and oviposition cycle. genotypes and the presence of specific muta- There could also be a trade-off between the pref- tions (Kain et al., 2012). But contrary to these erence for safer dark deep zones and the need findings, we did not find any significant differ- for foraging and dispersal at the illuminated sur- ences in our model organism in light sensitivity face (Dromph, 2003). among clones and clades for the two under- We searched for clonal variation in both the mean growth spectra, including when springtails were movement propensity and in the light sensitivity exposed to the brightest light spectrum. This of movement. We first found that the various lin- could indicate that photophobia is a shared, evo- eages moved differently in the dark: the average lutionary rigid behavioural trait in this species. It transition probabilities varied from 0.2 to almost also implies that other studies that failed to find 0.5 depending on the lineages. Quite remarkably, intraspecific variation in phototaxis may be miss- this genetic diversity was structured between the ing in the published bibliography. two previously described phylogenetic clades: In addition, when exposed to light, the genetic clones from the clade characterized by a fast life- differences in movement behaviour observed in history strategy had on average higher transition the dark vanish. Our observations also suggest probabilities than those with slow life-history that moving from one side of the vial to the other strategy, suggesting that they move faster and side in homogeneous dark conditions or in het- can potentially disperse over longer distances in erogeneous light conditions is associated with natural conditions. Even though we did not different underlying behaviours: exploration for measure dispersal per se (defined as the move- the former and escape for the latter. This is also ment of individuals or propagules that leads to supported by the fact that body length did not gene flow, see Clobert, 2012), mobility or ex- affect movement in the dark while small individu- ploratory activity has generally been positively als were found to be slightly more mobile than correlated to dispersiveness and can thus be larger ones in the heterogeneous environments. used as a rough proxy for dispersal in natural populations (Ronce and Clobert, 2012). The high- Acknowledgments er movement propensity of the fast life-history We want to thank to the staff members of the clade agrees with the evolutionary scenario of the Ecotron IleDe- France, especially Simon Chollet, ‘colonizer syndrome’ where fast life-history Mathieu Llavata and Florent Massol. This re- species or populations are more dispersed than search was supported by the Centre National de their slow life-history counterparts (Ronce and la Recherche Scientifique and a grant from the Clobert, 2012). The combination of increased dis- Ecole doctorale Diversité du Vivant at the Univer- persal, high fecundity but short lifespan and low sité Pierre and Marie Curie to M.G. R. (contract competitive ability allows fast individuals to 859/2013). This work was made possible thanks colonise new habitats more efficiently. Individuals to the CNRS Research Infrastructure Ecotrons with reduced movement propensity, low fecundi- supported by the Conseil régional d'Ile-de- ty and high competitive ability perform better at France (DIM R2DS I-05-098/R and 2011- the core range and in stable habitats, where pop- 11017735) and the Agence Nationale de la ulations have reached their carrying capacity and Recherche grant of the “Investissements where infraspecific competition is strong (Burton d'avenir” program (ANR-11-INBS-0001 AnaEE et al., 2010). Future studies should assess France). whether differences in short-distance movement

9 References

Arikawa, K., Eguchi, E., Yoshida, A., Aoki, K., 1980. Multiple De Meester, L., 1996. Evolutionary potential and local genet- extraocular photoreceptive areas on genitalia of butterfly Pa- ic differentiation in a phenotypically plastic trait of a cyclical pilio xuthus. Nature 288, 700– 702. parthenogen, Daphnia magna. Evol.; Int. J. Org. Evolution Baatrup, E., Bayley, M., Axelsen, J.A., 2006. Predation of the 1293–1298. mite Hypoaspis aculeifer on the springtail Folsomia fimetaria Dromph, K.M., 2003. Effect of starvation on phototaxis and and the influence of sex size, starvation, and poisoning. En- geotaxis of collembolans. Eur. J. Soil Biol. 39, 9–12. tomol. Exp. Appl. 118, 61–70. Edelaar, P., Bolnick, D.I., 2012. Non-random gene flow: an Barra, J.A., 1971. Les photorecepteurs des Collemboles, underappreciated force in evolution and ecology. Trends etude ultrastructurale. Z. Zellforsch. Mik. Ana. 117, 322–353. Ecol. Evol. 27, 659–665.Ernsting, G., Mulder, A.J., 1981. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., 2012. lme4: Linear Components of predatory behaviour underlying density-de- mixed-effects models using S4 classes [computer file]. pendent prey-size selection by Notiophilus biguttatus F. Bengtsson, G., Nilsson, E., Ryden, T., Wiktorsson, M., 2004. (Carabidae, coleoptera). Oecologia 51, 169–174. Irregular walks and loops combines in small-scale move- Ernsting, G., 1977. Effects of food deprivation and type of ment of a soil insect: implications for dispersal biology. J. prey on predation by Notiophilus biguttatus F. (Carabidae) Theoret. Biol. 231, 299–306. on springtails (Collembola). Oecologia 31, 13–20. Beresford, G.W., Selby, G., Moore, J.C., 2013. Lethal and Forstmeier, W., Schielzeth, H., 2011. Cryptic multiple hy- sub-lethal effects of UV-B radiation exposure on the collem- potheses testing in linear models: overestimated effect sizes bolan Folsomia candida (Willem) in the laboratory. Pedobi- and the winner's curse. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 47–55. ologia 56, 89–95. Fountain, M.T., Hopkin, S.P., 2005. Folsomia candida Binder, T.R., McDonald, D.G., 2008. The role of dermal pho- (Collembola): a standard soil . Annu. Rev. Entomol. toreceptors during the sea lamprey (Petromyzon marinus) 50, 201–222. spawning migration. J. Comp. Physiol. A. Neuroethol. Sens. Fox, G.L., Coyle-Thompson, C.A., Bellinger, P.F., Cohen, Neural. Behav. Physiol. 194, 921–928. R.W., 2007. Phototactic responses to ultraviolet and white Blandin, P., Abbadie, L., Courault, S., Garay, I., Geoffroy, light in various species of Collembola including the eyeless J.J., 1980. Etude d'un ecosysteme forestier mixte, 1: Climat, species, Folsomia candida. J. Insect Sci. 7, 1–12. structure de la vegetation et retombees de litieres [France, Frati, F., Negri, I., Fanciulli, P.P., Pellecchia, M., De Paola, V., Seine-et-Marne]. Rev. Ecol. Biol. Sol 17, 181–198. Scali, V., Dallai, R., 2004. High levels of genetic differentia- Bliss, D., Smith, H., 1985. Penetration of light into soil and tion between Wolbachia-infected and non- infected popula- its role in the control of seed germination. Plant Cell Environ. tions of Folsomia candida (Collembola, Isotomidae). Pedobi- 8, 475–483. ologia 48, 461–468. Boiteau, G., MacKinley, P., 2013. Role of avoidance behavior Friedrich, M., 2013. Biological clocks and visual systems in in the response of Folsomia candida to above-freezing cool- cave-adapted animals at the dawn of speleogenomics. Inte- ing temperatures. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 147, 50–60. gr. Comp. Biol. 53, 50–67. Boiteau, G., MacKinley, P., 2014. Geotaxis in the springtail Hauzy, C., Tully, T., Spataro, T., Paul, G., Arditi, R., 2010. Folsomia candida. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 1–7. Spatial heterogeneity and functional response: an experi- Boiteau, G., Mackinley, P., 2012. Locomotor response of Fol- ment in microcosms with varying obstacle densities. Oe- somia candida (Collembola: isotomidae) to cooling tempera- cologia 163, 625–636. tures. Environ. Entomol. 41, 916– 924. Hawthorne, D.J., Via, S., 2001. Genetic linkage of ecological Borowsky, B., 2011. Responses to light in two eyeless cave specialization and reproductive isolation in pea aphids. Na- dwelling amphipods (Niphargus ictus and Niphargus frasas- ture 412, 904–907. sianus). J. Crustacean Biol. 31, 613–616. Holmstrup, M., 2002. Strategies for Cold and Drought Toler- Briscoe, A.D., Chittka, L., 2001. The evolution of color vision ance in Permeable Soil Invertebrates. Ministry of Environ- in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46, 471–510. ment. National Environmental Research Institute. Burton, O.J., Phillips, B.L., Travis, J.M., 2010. Trade-offs and Huebner, K., Lindo, Z., Lechowicz, M.J., 2012. Post-fire suc- the evolution of life- histories during range expansion. Ecol. cession of collembolan communities in a northern hardwood Lett. 13, 1210–1220. forest. Eur. J. Soil Biol. 48, 59–65. Chenon, P., Rousset, A., Crouau, Y., 2000. Genetic Jaenike, J., Holt, R.D., 1991. Genetic variation for habitat polymorphism in nine clones of a parthenogenetic collem- preference: evidence and explanations. Amer. Nat. S67–90. bolan used in ecotoxicological testing. Appl. Soil Ecol. 14, Jordana, R., Baquero, E., Montuenga, L.M., 2000. A new 103–110. type of arthropod photoreceptor. Arthropod Struct. Dev. 29, Christiansen, K., 2005. Morphological adaptations. Ency- 289–293. clopedia of Caves. Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego Kain, J.S., Stokes, C., de Bivort, B.L., 2012. Phototactic per- (CA), pp. 386–397. sonality in fruit flies and its suppression by serotonin and Clobert, J., 2012. Dispersal Ecology and Evolution 1 st. white. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 19834– 19839. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Lampel, J., Briscoe, A.D., Wasserthal, L.T., 2005. Expression Cousyn, C., De Meester, L., Colbourne, J.K., Brendonck, L., of UV-, blue-, long- wavelength-sensitive opsins and mela- Verschuren, D., Volckaert, F., 2001. Rapid, local adaptation tonin in extraretinal photoreceptors of the optic lobes of of zooplankton behavior to changes in predation pressure in hawk moths. Cell Tissue Res. 321, 443–458. the absence of neutral genetic changes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Larimer, J.L., 1966. A functional caudal photoreceptor in Sci. U. S. A. 98, 6256–6260. blind cavernicolous crayfish. Nature 210, 204–205. De Meester, L., 1994. Life histories and habitat selection in Mallard, F., Farina, M., Tully, T., 2015. Within species varia- Daphnia: divergent life histories of D. magna clones differing tion in long-term trajectories of growth, fecundity and in phototactic behavior. Oecologia 97, 33– 341. mortality in the Collembola Folsomia candida. J. Evol. Biol. De Meester, L., 1995. Life history characteristics of Daphnia 1–9. magna clones differing in phototactic behavior. Hydrobiolo- Markow, T.A., Smith, W.L., 1977. Genetic analysis of photo- gia 307, 167–175. tactic behavior in Drosophila simulans. Genetics 85, 273–

10 278. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 230–242. Matthysen, E., 2012. Multicausality of dispersal: a review. Taddei-Ferretti, C., Musio, C., 2000. Photobehaviour of Hy- Dispersal Ecology and Evolution. Oxford University Press, dra (Cnidaria, Hydrozoa) and correlated mechanisms: a case Oxford, pp. 3–18. of extraocular photosensitivity. J. Photochem. Photobiol. B: Michels, E., De Meester, L., 2004. Inter-clonal variation in Biology 55, 88–101. phototactic behaviour and key life-history traits in a The R Development Core Team, 2012. R: A language and metapopulation of the cyclical parthenogen Daphnia am- environment for statistical computing [computer file]. Vienna, bigua: the effect of fish kairomones. Hydrobiologia 522, Austria,R foundation for statistical computing [distributor]. 221–233. Timmermann, M., Plath, M., 2008. Phototactic response and Nakagawa, S., Schielzeth, H., 2013. A general and simple light sensitivity in an epigean and a hypogean population of method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed- a barb (Garra barreimiae, Cyprinidae). Aquat. Ecol. 43, 539– effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 4, 133–142. 547. Pianka, E.R., 1970. R-Selection and K-Selection. Amer. Nat. Tosini, G., Avery, R.A., 1996. Dermal photoreceptors regulate 102, 592–597. basking behavior in the lizard Podarcis muralis. Physiol. Be- Ponge, J.F., Salmon, S., 2013. Spatial and taxonomic corre- hav. 59, 195–198. lates of species and species trait assemblages in soil inver- Tully, T., Ferriere, R., 2008. Reproductive flexibility: genetic tebrate communities. Pedobiologia 56, 129–136. Ponge, variation, genetic costs and long-term evolution in a collem- J.F., Dubs, F., Gillet, S., Sousa, J.P., Lavelle, P., 2006. De- bolan. PLoS One 3, 3207. creased biodiversity in soil springtail communities: the im- Tully, T., Lambert, A., 2011. The evolution of postreproduc- portance of dispersal and landuse history in heterogeneous tive life span as an insurance against indeterminacy. Evol.; landscapes. Soil Biol. Biochem. 38, 1158–1161. Int. J. Org. Evol. 65, 3013–3020. Ramirez, M.D., Speiser, D.I., Pankey, M.S., Oakley, T.H., Tully, T., Potapov, M., 2015. Intraspecific phenotypic varia- 2011. Understanding the dermal light sense in the context of tion and morphological divergence of strains of Folsomia integrative photoreceptor cell biology. Vis. Neurosci. 28, candida (Willem) (Collembola: isotomidae), the standard test 265–279. springtail. PLoS One 10, pe0136047. Reznick, D., Bryant, M.J., Bashey, F., 2002. R- and K-selec- Tully, T., D'Haese, C.A., Richard, M., Ferriere, R., 2006. Two tion revisited: the role of population regulation in life-history major evolutionary lineages revealed by molecular phyloge- evolution. Ecology 83, 1509–1520. ny in the parthenogenetic collembola species Folsomia can- Ronce, O., Clobert, J., 2012. Dispersal syndromes. Dispersal dida. Pedobiologia 50, 95–104. Ecology and Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. Tully T., 2004. Facteurs génétiques, maternels et envi- 119–138. ronnementaux de l’expression des traits d’histoire de vie Salmon, S., Ponge, P., 1998. Responses to light in a soil- chez le collembole. dwelling springtail. Eur. J. Soil Biol. 34, 199–201. Ullrich-Lüter, E.M., Dupont, S., Arboleda, E., Hausen, H., Salmon, S., Ponge, P., 2012. Species traits and habitats in Arnone, M.I., 2011. Unique system of photoreceptors in sea springtail communities: a regional scale study. Pedobiologia urchin tube feet. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 108, 8367– 55, 295–301. 8372. Salmon, S., Ponge, J.F., Gachet, S., Deharveng, L., Lefeb- Verdier, B., Jouanneau, I., Simonnet, B., Rabin, C., Van vre, N., Delabrosse, F., 2014. Linking species, traits and Dooren, T.J., Delpierre, N., Clobert, J., Abbadie, L., Ferriere, habitat characteristics of Collembola at European scale. Soil R.G., Le Galliard, J.F., 2014. Climate and atmosphere simu- Biol. Biochem. 75, 73–85. lator for experiments on ecological systems in changing en- Sih, A., Cote, J., Evans, M., Fogarty, S., Pruitt, J., 2012. Eco- vironments. Environ. Sci. Technol. 48, 8744–8753. logical implications of behavioural syndromes. Ecol. Lett. 15, Waagner, D., Bayley, M., Holmstrup, M., 2011. Recovery of 278–289. reproduction after drought in the soil living Folsomia candida Smith, H., 1994. Sensing the light environment: the functions (Collembola). Soil Biol. Biochem. 43, 690–692. of the phytochrome family. In: Kendrick, R.E., Kronenberg, Wilkens, L.A., Larimer, J.L., 1976. Photosensitivity in the G.H.N. (Eds.), Photomorphogenesis in Plants. Kluwer Acad- sixth abdominal ganglion of decapod crustaceans: a com- emic, Dordrecht, The Netherlands, pp. 377–416. parative study. J. Comp. Physiol. 106, 69–75. Stamps, J.A., 2001. Habitat selection by dispersers: inte- Xiang, Y., Yuan, Q., Vogt, N., Looger, L.L., Jan, L.Y., Jan, grating proximate and ultimate approaches. In: Clobert, J., Y.N., 2010. Light-avoidance- mediating photoreceptors tile Danchin, E., Dhondt, A.A., Nichols, J.D. (Eds.), Dispersal. the Drosophila larval body wall. Nature 468, 921– 926.

11 Tables Table S2. Summary of the total irradiance of nat- ural and simulated spectra.

Table S1. Sample size, age and body length per Spectrum Total irradi- ance clone studied in the experimental setup. (µW.cm-2) Total Number Mea Range Mean Range of Natural average undergrowth 31.5 num- of indi- n of age body length Simulated average undergrowth 37.3 ber of viduals age (days) length (mm) individ of un- (days (mm) Red and far-red 23.8 uals known ) Natural maximum undergrowth 70.5 age (or length) Simulated maximum undergrowth 52.0 AP 144 42 46.2 23-96 1.34 0.95-1.99 BR 144 42 32.3 17-76 1.41 0.99-2.02 Table S3. The influence of clone identity, position BV 144 24 68.4 23-117 1.50 1.02-1.98 (dark or light), spectrum type (four treatments) DK 144 26 62.7 23-110 1.29 0.97-1.76 and their two and three-way interactions (full GB 144 61 48.6 20-104 1.58 1.11-2.19 model) on transition probability of springtails be- tween each side of the experimental boxes GM 144 24 65.1 23-116 1.38 0.99-1.80 (N=1728 individuals). * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** HA 144 24 66.1 23-117 1.51 1.08-2.05 p<0.001. ME 144 24 69.7 12-117 1.41 0.97-1.99 Chi D. f. p-value PB 144 24 (1) 67.5 23-116 1.35 0.97-1.77 square TO 144 24 68.8 23-116 1.39 1.04-1.91 Clone 31.02 11 0.001 *** US 144 24 66.5 23-110 1.36 0.93-2.16 Position 26.98 1 <0.001 *** WI 144 24 60.8 23-109 1.40 1.06-1.89 Spectrum 9.09 3 0.028 * Clone*Position 8.03 11 0.710 Clone*Spectrum 47.87 33 0.045 * Position*Spectrum 8.33 3 0.039 * Clone*Position*Spectrum 47.51 33 0.049 *

12 Table S4. Full models describing the influence of clone, position, corrected length and their two-way interactions on transition probabilities of springtails between each side of the experimental boxes. Full models were fitted separately for each spectrum type corresponding to dark, red light, average undergrowth spectrum and maximum undergrowth spectrum treatments. Marginal and conditional R2 are provided. * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001.

Chi squared D.f. p-value R2m R2c Dark treatment (N=431) Clone 23.73 11 0.0139 * * 0.0037 0.1141 Position 0.74 1 0.3880 Residual Length 0.03 1 0.8547 Clone* Position 16.86 11 0.1120 Clone* Corrected Length 14.10 11 0.2273 Position* Corrected Length 0.66 1 0.4165 Red light treatment (N=432) Clone 20.21 11 0.0425 * * 0.0247 0.0731 Position 4.77 1 0.0290 * * Corrected Length 0.04 1 0.8472 Clone* Position 9.13 11 0.6095 Clone* Corrected Length 8.94 11 0.6276 Position* Corrected Length 0.0046 1 0.9462 Average undergrowth treatment (N=432) Clone 17.8053 11 0.0862 0.0384 0.0827 Position 9.4785 1 0.0021 ** ** Corrected Length 3.8588 1 0.0495 * * Clone* Position 17.5853 11 0.0917 Clone* Corrected Length 12.5436 11 0.3242 Position* Corrected Length 7.1203 1 0.0076 ** Maximum undergrowth treatment (N=432) Clone 15.4355 11 0.1634 0.04574 0.0961 Position 23.0414 1 <0.001 *** Corrected Length 0.0164 1 0.8981 Clone* Position 11.7662 11 0.3815 Clone* Corrected Length 21.7120 11 0.0267 * Position* Corrected Length 1.1535 1 0.2828

13