Investigating Human Diversity in the Twenty-First Century

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Investigating Human Diversity in the Twenty-First Century Investigating human diversity in the twenty-first century Julien Delhez Anthropological Review • Vol. 83(3), 317–325 (2020) Investigating human diversity in the twenty-first century Julien Delhez Seminar für Ägyptologie und Koptologie, Georg-August-Universität Göttingen, Germany ABSTRACT: In recent years, there has been renewed academic and public debate on the topic of race. The present essay compares two books dealing with this subject. Charles Murray’s Human Diversity states that the social sciences are permeated by a rigid orthodoxy that puts unnecessary strain on researchers working on sex differences, race differences, and individual differences. Far from being scary, Murray argues, these differences are interesting and can be lived with. Adam Rutherford’s How to Argue With a Racist examines various claims with respect to race. It includes four sections dedicated, respectively, to skin color, ancestry, sport, and intelligence. The essay assesses the authors’ factual claims as well as their tone, their general mindset, and their personal attitude toward science. KEY WORDS: human differences, race, genetic variation, ancestral populations Introduction gue With a Racist: History, Science, Race and Reality (Rutherford 2020). While both Research on human evolution and ge- books have already been reviewed sever- netics has greatly changed the way hu- al times by academics and by journalists, mans see themselves. While helping to I believe a comparative book review has understand the past and the present of several merits. Firstly, both authors are our species, research has also led spe- informed observers and have witnessed cialists and the broader public to revise the same scientific revolution, and yet their thoughts on some highly sensitive came out with very different conclusions; topics. it may be interesting to explore why. In this essay, I discuss two books ad- Secondly, an assessment of the authors’ dressing recent studies on human differ- viewpoints and writing may reveal many ences and what they mean for society: differences beyond factual disagreement. Charles Murray’s Human Diversity: The Thirdly and importantly, when one uses Biology of Gender, Race, and Class (Murray each book to cast light on the strengths 2020), and Adam Rutherford’s How to Ar- and weaknesses of the other book, it be- Review Article Received: July 28, 2020; Revised: XXXX XX, XXXX; Accepted: August 25, 2020 DOI: 10.2478/anre-2020-0024 © 2020 Polish Anthropological Society 318 Julien Delhez comes easier to overcome the temptation tackles sex differences in what Murray of selecting the best arguments on one calls “cognitive repertoires” (Murray side while ignoring valuable points on 2020: 8), i.e. differences in personality, the other side. All in all, the main lesson mental abilities, and behavior. To enable I have drawn from comparing these two the reader to understand how large an books is that the topic of race requires a average difference needs to be in order great deal of intellectual humility. to matter for society, Murray provides Human Diversity is a thick volume sum- an interlude explaining what Cohen’s d marizing the results of many recent stud- consists of (Murray 2020: 23), and he ies on sex differences, race or population points out one of the most important differences (see below for the distinction facts about sex differences in cognitive between race and population), and individ- repertoires: even if the vast majority of ual differences in relation to class struc- effect sizes for such differences are tiny, ture. The book does not require advanced it does not mean than such differences training in genetics or psychometrics. are socially irrelevant, because “in the Nonetheless, given how complex the is- real world, it is taken for granted that sues dealt with are, one can expect that small differences add up” (Murray 2020: it will mainly find readers among those 28). A very convincing example of this who are willing to familiarize themselves is given by the comparison between fac- with technical notions. Despite its as- es of both sexes: for isolated traits, it is sumed goal of challenging “an orthodoxy often difficult to perceive any difference that is scared stiff of biology” (Murray between the female face and the male 2020: 2), Human Diversity is noticeable for face, but when one looks at a face in its its non-confrontational tone. entirety, most of the time, it isn’t difficult How to Argue With a Racist is a short to find out whether it is male or female book which focuses exclusively on the (see Murray 2020: 29). topic of race. Compared to Human Di- The next three chapters tackle sex versity, How to Argue With a Racist has a differences in personality, neurocogni- more combative approach, as Rutherford tive functioning and abilities, and voca- (2020: 3) explains: “This book is a weap- tional choices. Murray generally focuses on.” The book has neither footnotes nor on the issues that are least contentious endnotes, only a list of forty references among psychologists and neuroscien- (Rutherford 2020: 189–194). tists. The difficult question of whether Before comparing the arguments the sexes differ in g (the general factor of both books on the topic of race, it is of intelligence) is treated very briefly, worth presenting Murray’s claims on the with a footnote allowing curious read- two other sensitive issues he deals with: ers to find articles from both sides of the sex differences and class. debate (Murray 2020: 386–387, n. 61). For sex differences in vocational choices, Latest research on human sex Murray relies on the results of the Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth differences (SMPY). This study is especially help- ful for assessing sex differences in life A substantial part of Human Diversi- choices insofar as all participants, male ty (Murray 2020: 11–127 & 337–372) and female, had the ability to embrace Investigating human diversity in the twenty-first century 319 a career in engineering or the physical (2020) wrote: “Murray leans hard on the sciences. Even within the SMPY sample, work of Cambridge psychologist Simon substantial sex differences in life prefer- Baron-Cohen. But while Baron-Cohen ences have been found: among others, is well respected, some of his published on average, females were more likely to claims have not been replicated (that is, choose a part-time employment (either confirmed by subsequent studies). This temporarily or definitely), and males doesn’t mean they are wrong, but that were more likely to give weight to a “sal- Murray’s confidence is unwarranted.” ary that is well above the average per- In fact, Murray (2020: 35) clearly urg- son” (Murray 2020: 69). Females were es caution about one of Baron-Cohen’s also more likely to end up in the human- most publicized studies: “It is a single, ities. Importantly, both sexes viewed unreplicated study with a sample of 102, their lives as equally satisfying, which no proof to take to the bank, but its find- suggests “that there are multiple ways ing was in line with many other studies to construct a meaningful, productive, that have found personality sex differ- and satisfying life” (Lubinski et al. 2014 ences in infants.” cited in Murray 2020: 76). Another chapter deals with the differ- Class and individual differences ences that have been observed, on brain scans, between brains of both sexes. For Human Diversity’s section about class the time being, the most important thing mainly presents conclusions that are al- to remember is that this research field is ready accepted among behavioral geneti- still in its infancy (Murray 2020: 125). cists. I will summarize them very briefly. Appendix 2 deals with medical condi- The author accepts neither the pessimis- tions that may constitute exceptions to tic view that socio-economic class is pri- the sexual dimorphism of the human marily a function of economic or ethnic species. Appendix 3 tackles sex differ- privilege, nor the optimistic view that ences in brain size, whose implications “people can become anything they want are not yet well understood, and the phe- to be if they try hard enough” (Murray, nomenon known as “greater male vari- 2020: 203). Instead, Murray (2020: 204) ance” (see Murray 2020: 357). states: “Class is a function of the genet- Are there additional topics the au- ic lottery plus character, determination, thor should have covered? In the spirit hard work, and idiosyncratic circum- that a chain is as strong as its weakest stances.” To clarify the role of the genet- link, Murray has willfully refrained from ic lottery, Murray outlines his argument discussing the findings of evolution- in three steps. Firstly (chapter 11), in an ary psychology (see Murray 2020: 6–7) overview of solidly established findings and studies on rodents in relation to of behavioral genetics, he points out that biological sex differences in the brain all behavioral traits are heritable, and the (see Murray 2020: 103). Some readers g factor substantially so, while the shared will probably miss a discussion of such environment (parental environment) has studies. In the author’s defense, though, little impact on the children’s cognitive one cannot say that wariness about un- repertoires. Secondly (chapter 12), he fair critics was not warranted: in a re- provides evidence as to the crucial role view for the New Statesman, Philip Ball of g in determining educational perfor- 320 Julien Delhez mance, job performance, income, and Importantly, while the number of groups occupation. Thirdly and finally (chapter (K) is decided by the analyst, analyses 13), he deals with various claims about conducted with different numbers offer allegedly substantial effects of exter- results that are consistent with one an- nal interventions. The sections on the other: “different values of K do not pro- growth mindset (Murray 2020: 255–258) duce a radically different pattern of re- and epigenetics (Murray 2020: 260–268) sults.
Recommended publications
  • Future Efforts in Flynn Effect Research: Balancing Reductionism with Holism
    J. Intell. 2014, 2, 122-155; doi:10.3390/jintelligence2040122 OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Article Future Efforts in Flynn Effect Research: Balancing Reductionism with Holism Michael A. Mingroni Newark, Delaware, USA; E-Mail: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-302-753-3533 External Editor: Joseph L. Rodgers Received: 13 February 2014: in revised form 2 October 2014 / Accepted: 2 October 2014 / Published: 15 October 2014 Abstract: After nearly thirty years of concerted effort by many investigators, the cause or causes of the secular gains in IQ test scores, known as the Flynn effect, remain elusive. In this target article, I offer six suggestions as to how we might proceed in our efforts to solve this intractable mystery. The suggestions are as follows: (1) compare parents to children; (2) consider other traits and conditions; (3) compare siblings; (4) conduct more and better intervention programs; (5) use subtest profile data in context; and (6) quantify the potential contribution of heterosis. This last section contains new simulations of the process of heterosis, which provide a plausible scenario whereby rapid secular changes in multiple genetically influenced traits are possible. If there is any theme to the present paper, it is that future study designs should be simpler and more highly focused, coordinating multiple studies on single populations. Keywords: Flynn effect; intelligence; secular trend; heterosis 1. Introduction It has been nearly thirty years since James Flynn brought widespread attention to the occurrence of rapid gains in IQ test scores [1,2]. However, in honestly assessing the situation today one would have to conclude that we are not much closer to identifying the cause than we were three decades ago.
    [Show full text]
  • The Black-White Test Score Gap: an Introduction
    CHRISTOPHER JENCKS MEREDITH PHILLIPS 1 The Black-White Test Score Gap: An Introduction FRICAN AMERICANS currently score lower than A European Americans on vocabulary, reading, and mathematics tests, as well as on tests that claim to measure scholastic apti- tude and intelligence.1 This gap appears before children enter kindergarten (figure 1-1), and it persists into adulthood. It has narrowed since 1970, but the typical American black still scores below 75 percent of American whites on most standardized tests.2 On some tests the typical American black scores below more than 85 percent of whites.3 1. We are indebted to Karl Alexander, William Dickens, Ronald Ferguson, James Flynn, Frank Furstenberg, Arthur Goldberger, Tom Kane, David Levine, Jens Ludwig, Richard Nisbett, Jane Mansbridge, Susan Mayer, Claude Steele, and Karolyn Tyson for helpful criticisms of earlier drafts. But we did not make all the changes they suggested, and they are in no way responsible for our conclusions. 2. These statistics also imply, of course, that a lot of blacks score above a lot of whites. If the black and white distributions are normal and have the same standard deviation, and if the black-white gap is one (black or white) standard deviation, then when we compare a randomly selected black to a randomly selected white, the black will score higher than the white about 24 percent of the time. If the black-white gap is 0.75 rather than 1.00 standard deviations, a randomly selected black will score higher than a randomly selected white about 30 percent of the time.
    [Show full text]
  • The Flynn / Non-Flynn Phenomenon and the Digitized Educational System
    ERD 2016: Education, Reflection, Development, Fourth Edition The Flynn / Non-Flynn Phenomenon and the digitized Educational System Claudia Talasman-Chioreana* * Corresponding author: Claudia Talasman-Chiorean, [email protected] aPhD Research Assistant, College of Political Sciences, Babes-Bolyai University, str. Traian Mosiu, nr. 71, Cluj-Napoca, Romania Abstract http://dx.doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2016.12.12 Forced to adapt to new realities, the human mind has evolved in a qualitatively superior way from a historical stage to another. This principle is affirmed and demonstrated in several studies known as The Flynn Phenomenon. As a response to these principles, The Non-Flynn Phenomenon claims that in our modern heavily digitized society, IQ has decreased due to the minimal contribution of the human mind in dealing with the new existential problems of humanity. The current trend of substitution of specific human elements by gadgets, software tools or created digital spaces is increasingly obvious. Between The Flynn and non-Flynn Phenomenon, education plays an important role. Education aims efficient use innovative technical means. And this trend may be the solution for the continuous development of IQ. The new technology involves increasing interest, maintaining motivation, widening of the informational horizon, if used properly, without excesses, in combination with other methods and modern and applied educational resources. © 2016 Published by Future Academy www.FutureAcademy.org.uk Keywords: The Flynn Phenomenon, The Non-Flynn Phenomenon, digitized society, IQ, education. 1. IQ and Human Evolution Throughout human evolution, progress has been one of the functional, adaptive constants that man has consistently promoted. Whether we talk about progress at the technical, cultural, social or civic level, the human being has constantly been trying to find new, useful, easy resources in order to get a ratio of maximum benefit and minimal effort.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Intelligence?
    WHAT IS INTELLIGENCE? By one of the most creative and influential psychologists in the field of intelligence comes an unrivalled ‘must-read’ …a brilliant interweaving of data and argument. Intrigued? Read on to find out more about Flynn’s inspiring new book October 2007 • 226 pages • 5 tables • 7 figures Hardback 978-0-521-88007-7 c. £14.99 Do IQ gains mean that each generation is more intelligent than the last? Do they suggest how each of us can enhance our own intelligence? For over twenty years, psychologists have struggled to understand the implications of IQ gains. Professor James Flynn, one of the most creative and influential psychologists in the field of intelligence, is finally ready to give his own views. His answer is both surprising and illuminating. Flynn’s unrivalled book makes an important contribution to our understanding of human intelligence. Contents 1. A bombshell in a 6. IQ gains can kill letter box 7. What if the gains are 2. Beyond the Flynn Effect over? 3. Towards a new theory 8. Knowing our ancestors of intelligence Appendix I: Tables; 4. Testing the Dickens/ Appendix II: Declaration Flynn model in a capital case. 5. Why did it take so long? Find out more or order online at www.cambridge.org/flynn WHAT THE EXPERTS SAY James Flynn has been a saintly anomaly in Flynn provides the first satisfying explanation this contentious debate. His latest book is a classic of the massive rise in IQ test scores... expression of the man – advancing a particular Professor N. J. Makintosh,University of Cambridge point of view, but committed to following the data, responding to criticism with reason and evidence, and unfailingly good-humored.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Ability William T
    Forthcoming in The New Palgrave Dictionary of Economics, Steve Durlauf ed. Cognitive Ability William T. Dickens Senior Fellow The Brookings Institution ABSTRACT Modern psychological theory views cognitive ability as multidimensional while acknowledging that the many different abilities are themselves positively correlated. This positive correlation across abilities has led most psychometricians to accept the reality of a general cognitive ability that is reflected in the full scale score on major tests of cognitive ability or IQ. This entry provides an introduction to the history of cognitive testing and some of its major controversies. Evidence supporting the validity of measures of cognitive ability is presented and the nature and implications of group differences is discussed along with evidence on its malleability. Some people obviously and consistently understand new concepts quicker, solve unfamiliar problems faster, see relationships that others don’t and are more knowledgeable about a wider range of topics than others. We call such people smart, bright, quick, or intelligent. Psychologists have developed tests to measure this trait. Originally called IQ tests (for Intelligence Quotient because the measures were constructed as the ratio of mental age to chronological age multiplied by 100) that name has fallen out of favor. Instead, such tests are now often referred to as tests of cognitive ability. Although the term IQ is still sometimes used to refer to what such tests measure, none construct a ratio. History Spearman (1904) first popularized the observation that individuals who do well on one type of mental task also tend to do well on many others. For example, people who are good at recognizing patterns in sequences of abstract drawings are also good at quickly arranging pictures in order to tell a story, telling what three dimensional shapes drawn in two dimensions will look like when rotated, tend to have large vocabularies and good reading comprehension, and are quick at arithmetic.
    [Show full text]
  • The G-Factor of International Cognitive Ability Comparisons: the Homogeneity of Results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-Tests Across Nations
    European Journal of Personality Eur. J. Pers. 21: 667–706 (2007) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/per.634 The g-Factor of International Cognitive Ability Comparisons: The Homogeneity of Results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-Tests Across Nations HEINER RINDERMANN* Institute of Psychology, Otto-von-Guericke-University Magdeburg, Germany Abstract International cognitive ability and achievement comparisons stem from different research traditions. But analyses at the interindividual data level show that they share a common positive manifold. Correlations of national ability means are even higher to very high (within student assessment studies, r ¼ .60–.98; between different student assessment studies [PISA-sum with TIMSS-sum] r ¼ .82–.83; student assessment sum with intelligence tests, r ¼ .85–.86). Results of factor analyses indicate a strong g-factor of differences between nations (variance explained by the first unrotated factor: 94–95%). Causes of the high correlations are seen in the similarities of tests within studies, in the similarities of the cognitive demands for tasks from different tests, and in the common developmental factors at the individual and national levels including known environmental and unknown genetic influences. Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Key words: intelligence; knowledge; education; student assessment; educational achievement; OECD; IEA INTRODUCTION The past century of empirical research on cognitive abilities has convincingly demonstrated their relevance for both individuals and societies across a wide variety of criteria. For individuals, both IQ (Schmidt & Hunter, 2004) and literacy (OECD, 2000) have been linked with job performance, longevity (Gottfredson & Deary, 2004; OECD, 2000), and low criminality (Herrnstein & Murray, 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • Malcolm Gladwell Looks at Intelligence and IQ Tests
    Malcolm Gladwell Looks at Intelligence and I.Q. Tests In this fascinating New Yorker article, author Malcolm Gladwell begins by describing what he calls the I.Q. fundamentalists – those who believe that (a) intelligence tests measure an identifiable mental trait that predicts the quality of our thinking, and (b) that intelligence is innate – it’s determined by our genes and can’t be changed after birth. Among the fundamentalists are H. H. Goddard (who coined the word moron and wrote, “The people who are doing the drudgery are, as a rule, in their proper places”); Lewis Terman (who predicted that California children with the highest I.Q. scores would grow up to be the most successful in every profession); Arthur Jensen (who said that Head Start wouldn’t work because I.Q. was mostly genetic); Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray (authors of The Bell Curve); Richard Lynn (who believes that Asians are the most intelligent race because of the Himalayas, really cold weather, pre-modern hunting practices, brain size, and specialized vowel sounds); and James Watson (who recently told an English newspaper that he was “inherently gloomy” about the prospects for Africa because its people consistently score lower than Europeans on intelligence tests). But those who believe that intelligence is inborn and fixed, says Gladwell, haven’t contended with some stubborn facts. The most important is that I.Q. improves steadily from one generation to the next. For example, at the beginning of the nineteenth century, the children of U.S. immigrants from southern Italy scored in the high 70s and low 80s on I.Q.
    [Show full text]
  • Iq on the Rise: the Flynn Effect in Rural Kenyan Children
    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article IQ ON THE RISE The Flynn Effect in Rural Kenyan Children Tamara C. Daley,1 Shannon E. Whaley,2 Marian D. Sigman,1,2 Michael P. Espinosa,2 and Charlotte Neumann3 1Department of Psychology, 2Department of Psychiatry, and 3School of Public Health, University of California, Los Angeles Abstract—Multiple studies have documented significant IQ gains dren in eastern Tennessee (Wheeler, 1970). Specifically, youths’ expo- over time, a phenomenon labeled the Flynn effect. Data from 20 in- sure to a more technological and complex visual world through toys, dustrialized nations show massive IQ gains over time, most notably in games, television, and computers may affect performance on IQ tests. culturally reduced tests like the Raven’s Progressive Matrices. To our Images typically seen on cereal boxes, restaurant placemats, and com- knowledge, however, this is the first study to document the Flynn effect puter games resemble the patterns on tests such as the Raven’s matri- in a rural area of a developing country. Data for this project were col- ces, and exposure to these materials may improve performance on lected during two large studies in Embu, Kenya, in 1984 and 1998. tests of visual-spatial abilities (Williams, 1998). Most recently, Flynn Results strongly support a Flynn effect over this 14-year period, with (2000) has argued that environmental and social factors serve as multi- the most significant gains found in Raven’s matrices. Previously hy- pliers of certain cognitive skills and that a model of reciprocal causal- pothesized explanations (e.g., improved nutrition; increased environ- ity therefore best accounts for the massive gains on IQ tests.
    [Show full text]
  • The Theory of Intelligence and Its Measurement
    Intelligence 39 (2011) 171–177 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence The theory of intelligence and its measurement A.R. Jensen ⁎ Univ. of California, Berkeley and The Institute of Mental Chronometry, 3330 Southlake Drive, Kelseyville CA 95451, United States article info abstract Article history: Mental chronometry (MC) studies cognitive processes measured by time. It provides an Received 8 January 2011 absolute, ratio scale. The limitations of instrumentation and statistical analysis caused the early Received in revised form 11 February 2011 studies in MC to be eclipsed by the ‘paper-and-pencil’ psychometric tests started by Binet. Accepted 11 March 2011 However, they use an age-normed, rather than a ratio scale, which severely limits the ability of Available online 22 April 2011 IQ tests to probe the physical basis of differences in cognition. For this reason, Arthur Jensen reinitiated mental chronometry in the 1970s. He designed an apparatus that measures reaction Keywords: time to a task known as the Hick paradigm that requires the testee to respond to a display of 1 Flynn Effect (FE) to 8 lights. Faster decision times were related to psychometric g, with theoretically important Galton Intelligence consequences. He was able to do this, where many other studies had failed, mainly because his IQ apparatus clearly separated movement (MT) from reaction time (RT, also called ‘decision Mental chronometry time’.) Interestingly, while RT is clearly related to IQ, MT is not. Principal components analysis Movement time (MT) reveals RT to be a cognitive variable and MT a motor variable. Failure to distinguish between Nisbett them drastically obscures the correlation between composite RT (i.e., RT+MT) and cognitive Psychometrics variables.
    [Show full text]
  • Flynn Et Al. 2014
    Intelligence 44 (2014) 1–10 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses James R. Flynn a,⁎,1, Jan te Nijenhuis b,1, Daniel Metzen b a Departments of Political Studies and Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand b Work and Organizational Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands article info abstract Article history: When IQ tests are ranked by the magnitude of their score gains over time, this hierarchy lacks a Received 20 October 2013 positive correlation with the same tests ranked by their g loadings. Therefore, Jensen declared Received in revised form 29 January 2014 IQ gains “hollow” and, by implication, extended this judgment to score gains that indicated Accepted 30 January 2014 that blacks had made IQ gains on whites. We offer four exploratory meta-analyses that apply Available online 25 February 2014 Jensen's method to the subtest score differences between normal subjects and those suffering from certain afflictions: iodine deficiency (K =6,N = 196), prenatal cocaine exposure (K =2, Keywords: N = 215), fetal alcohol syndrome and degree of fetal alcohol syndrome (respectively, K =1, Flynn effect N =110;andK =3,N = 125), and traumatic brain injury (K =14,N = 629). All of these Jensen effect create a substantial cognitive deficit in those afflicted. However, the correlations between Method of correlated vectors subtest score differences and g loadings run from −0.23 to +0.12, with an unweighted Significance of IQ gains over time Significance of black IQ gains on whites average of 0.00.
    [Show full text]
  • Life, Death, and IQ
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Cleveland-Marshall College of Law Cleveland State University EngagedScholarship@CSU Cleveland State Law Review Law Journals 2011 Life, Death, and IQ: It's Much More than Just a Score: Understanding and Utilizing Forensic Psychological and Neuropsychological Evaluations in Atkins Intellectual Disability/Mental Retardation Cases John Matthew aF bian William W. Thompson Jeffrey B. Lazarus Follow this and additional works at: https://engagedscholarship.csuohio.edu/clevstlrev Part of the Criminal Law Commons, and the Law and Psychology Commons How does access to this work benefit oy u? Let us know! Recommended Citation John Matthew Fabian, William W. Thompson, and Jeffrey B. Lazarus, Life, Death, and IQ: It's Much More than Just a Score: Understanding and Utilizing Forensic Psychological and Neuropsychological Evaluations in Atkins Intellectual Disability/Mental Retardation Cases, 59 Clev. St. L. Rev. 399 (2011) available at https://engagedscholarship.csuohio.edu/clevstlrev/vol59/iss3/7 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Law Journals at EngagedScholarship@CSU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Cleveland State Law Review by an authorized editor of EngagedScholarship@CSU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. LIFE, DEATH, AND IQ: IT’S MUCH MORE THAN JUST A SCORE: UNDERSTANDING AND UTILIZING FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGICAL AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATIONS IN ATKINS INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY/MENTAL RETARDATION CASES JOHN MATTHEW FABIAN, PSY. D., J.D, ABPP* WILLIAM W. THOMPSON, IV, ASSISTANT PUBLIC DEFENDER+ JEFFREY B. LAZARUS, ATTORNEY AT LAW∇ I. STANDARDS FOR ATKINS HEARINGS....................................
    [Show full text]
  • Appendix to the Petition for a Writ of Certiorari
    No. ------ 3Jn tbe ~upreme <!Court of tbe fflniteb ~tate~ KENNETH DARCELL QUINCE A/K/A RA.SIKH ABDUL-HAKIM, Petitioner, V. STATp OF FLORIDA, Respondent. ON PETITION FOR A WRIT OF CERTIORARI TO THE SUPREME COURT OF FLORIDA APPENDIX TO THE PETITION FOR A WRIT OF CERTIORARI DEA TH PENALTY CASE RAHEELA AHMED COUNSEL OF RECORD FLORIDA BAR No. 0713457 [email protected] MARIA CHRISTINE PERINETTI FLORIDA BAR No. 0013837 [email protected]. US LISA MARIE BORT FLORIDA BAR NO; 0119074 [email protected] ASSISTANT CCRCs LAW OFFICE OF THE CAPITAL COLLATERAL REGIONAL COUNSEL - MIDDLE REGION 12973 NORTH TELECOM PARKWAY, TEMPLE TERRACE, FLORIDA 33637-0907 (813) 558-1600 Counsel for the Petitioner, Kenneth D. Quince. CONTENTS OF APPENDIX INDEX TO APPENDICES Appendix A. Revised opinion of the Supreme Court of Florida affirming the lower court's denial of relief. Quince v. State, Appeal No. SCI 7-127 (April 12, 2018). Appendix B. Order of the Supreme Court of Florida granting the State's motion for clarification and denying Quince's motion for rehearing. Quince v. State, Appeal No. SCI 7-127 (April 12, 2018). Appendix C. Quince's motion for rehearing filed before the Supreme Court of Florida. Quince v. State, Appeal No. SCI 7-127 (February 1, 2018). Appendix D. State of Florida's motion for clarification filed before the Supreme Court of Florida. Quince v. State, Appeal No. SCI 7-127 (January 23, 2018). Appendix E. Initial opinion of the Supreme Court of Florida affirming the lower court's denial ofrelief. Quince v.
    [Show full text]