Down Contrast Sensitivity Control

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Down Contrast Sensitivity Control Electrophysiological Events Related to Top- Down Contrast Sensitivity Control by Bratislav Mišić A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Graduate Department of Psychology University of Toronto © Copyright by Bratislav Mišić, 2009 Electrophysiological Events Related to Top-Down Contrast Sensitivity Control by Bratislav Mišić Master of Arts, Graduate Department of Psychology University of Toronto, 2009 Abstract Stimulus-driven changes in the gain of sensory neurons are well-documented, but relatively little is known about whether analogous gain-control can also be effected in a top-down manner. A recent psychophysical study demonstrated that sensitivity to luminance contrast can be modulated by a priori knowledge (de la Rosa et al., in press). In the present study, event-related potentials were used to resolve the stages of information processing that facilitate such knowledge-driven adjustments. Groupwise independent component analysis identified two robust spatiotemporal patterns of endogenous brain activity that captured experimental effects. The first pattern was associated with obligatory processing of contextual information, while the second pattern was associated with selective initiation of contrast gain adjustment. These data suggest that knowledge-driven contrast gain control is mediated by multiple independent electrogenic sources. page ii Acknowledgments First and foremost, I would like to thank my mentor, Anthony R. McIntosh for his patience, support and guidance. I would also like to thank Bruce Schneider and Claude Alain for generously providing their time and expertise as members of my thesis committee; Natasa Kovacevic for her invaluable technical help and all of the members of the McIntosh Lab and the Rotman Research Institute ERP Lab for providing a stimulating environment. Finally, I would like to thank the University of Toronto Department of Psychology and the Government of Canada for their financial support during my studies. page iii Table of Contents INTRODUCTION 1 Neural Manifestations of Contrast Sensitivity 2 Temporal Properties 5 Attention 6 Top-Down Control: de la Rosa, Gordon & Schneider 7 Top-Down vs. Bottom-Up: Neural Manifestations 8 MATERIALS AND METHODS 13 Experiment I 13 ERP Recording and pre-processing 15 Experiment II 17 Data Analysis 17 RESULTS 20 Experiment I 20 Experiment II 21 PLS Results 21 DISCUSSION 30 Behavior 30 Event-Related Potentials 30 REFERENCES 38 page iv List of Figures Figure 1. Control flow of Experiment I. 14 Figure 2. Cap configuration for the 76-channel ActiveTwo cap. 16 Figure 3. Mean percent accuracy for low-contrast trials for the three conditions in Experiment I. 14 Figure 4. Task-specific grand average ERP amplitudes at four representative electrodes. 14 Figure 5. Amplitude distributions for the Baseline and Valid-Cue conditions and their difference. 24 Figure 6. Task-related components. 25 Figure 7. Average amplitudes for cue- and task-specific conditions at one representative electrode. 26 Figure 8. Cue-type components common to all conditions. 27 Figure 9. Cue-type components unique to the Valid-Cue condition. 28 page v Introduction Successful object perception partly depends on the ability of the observer to parse a visual scene into objects. Objects can often be separated from their background and from each other as a result of subtle luminance and color contrasts along edges and mutual borders. These properties of the environment are therefore significant sources of information and sensitivity to contrasts is an important feature of the visual system. Please note that this literature review and research proposal specifically concern luminance contrast and not color contrast. Unless stated otherwise, the term ‘contrast’ refers to luminance contrast. Regardless of their specialized function or their station in the hierarchy of visual processing, most cortical visual neurons are sensitive to the level of contrast within their receptive field (RF) and their response profile varies somewhat predictably as a monotonic, increasing function of contrast (Albrecht & Hamilton, 1982). Response rates are accelerated and the response function is characterized by an expansive nonlinearity over low levels of contrast. As contrast increases, the response function becomes approximately linear. At high levels of contrast, the response rates saturate such that the response function has a second nonlinear region. Namely, the response function becomes compressed and asymptotes to some maximal response level. Despite considerable heterogeneity across individual neurons, the contrast- response function (CRF) generally maintains this sigmoidal shape (Albrecht & Hamilton, 1982) and can be fitted with the Naka-Rushton function (Naka & Rushton, 1966): n n n f(x) = rmaxx /(x + x50 ) where x represents contrast, rmax represents the firing rate at which the response saturates and x50 represents the semisaturation constant: the point at which 50% of the maximal response is attained. The slope of the function, which corresponds to the rate of change of the response, is represented by the exponent n. As will shortly become clear, it is useful to parameterize experimental contrast-response data in this way because it allows for page 1 page 2 most commonly observed phenomena to be conveniently described in terms of simple parameter manipulations. Contrast increments over the intermediate segment of the CRF elicit the greatest changes in the response and this segment is often termed the ‘dynamic range’. Therefore, the dynamic range of a given neuron is the range of contrasts over which the response is informative, but this range is considerably narrower than the range of contrasts likely to be encountered in the environment (Albrecht & Hamilton, 1982; Frazor & Geisler, 2006). To maintain sensitivity, gain is controlled and continuously adjusted in response to fluctuations of local contrast (Ohzawa, Sclar & Freeman, 1982; 1985). The precise means of gain-control is still subject to debate, but data from most studies have converged on two possibilities, both of which involve multiplicative mechanisms. The first is that the contrast necessary to elicit a given response is multiplied, thus changing the contrast-gain. This serves to center the CRF on the average ambient level of contrast (e.g. Gardner et al., 2005; Ohzawa et al., 1982), effectively shifting the dynamic range. Graphically, this corresponds to a lateral shift of the CRF, which can be modeled by changing the value of x50.The second is that response-gain is adjusted through a multiplication of the response by a constant factor proportional to the average ambient contrast (e.g. Geisler & Albrecht, 1992). This is equivalent to scaling the CRF and can be modeled by manipulating the rmax term of the Naka-Rushton function. Neural Manifestations of Contrast Sensitivity Thus far, the precise definition of ‘response’ has been left intentionally ambiguous and little attempt has been made to describe the nature of the processing units or their location. As previously noted, contrast sensitivity and gain-control are near-ubiquitous properties of visual neurons and typical CRFs can be acquired using a variety of recording techniques. Using single-cell recordings, they have been observed in cat striate cortex (Albrecht & Hamilton, 1982; Ohzawa et al., 1982; 1985; Sclar, Ohzawa & Freeman, 1982), monkey primary visual cortex (Albrecht & Hamilton, 1982; Albrecht et al., 2002; Sclar, Maunsell & Lonnie, 1990), area V4 (Cheng et al., 1994) and middle temporal area page 3 (MT; Cheng et al., 1994; Kohn & Movshon, 2003; Sclar et al., 1990). In these studies, the ‘response’ is the firing rate of an individual cell. Neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have also revealed contrast sensitivity and gain-control in the human visual cortex, including areas V1, V2, V3, V4 and MT (Avidan et al., 2001; Boynton et al., 1999; Gardner et al., 2005; Heeger et al., 2000; Tootell et al., 1995; 1997). In these studies, the ‘response’ variable is the hemodynamic or blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal that depends on the activity of many neurons. In addition, visually evoked potentials (VEPs) measured using electroencephalography (EEG) over the occipital lobe are also sensitive to stimulus contrast and standard CRFs can be computed using VEP amplitude as an index of the response (Spekreijse, Van der Tweel & Zuidema, 1973). These cortical responses are also subject to contrast sensitivity control mechanisms (Porciatti et al., 2000). Interestingly, VEP latency also depends on contrast, with phase advances resulting from increasing stimulus contrast (Porticatti et al., 2000; Spekreijse et al., 1973). Moreover, the neurophysiological measures provided by various methods are remarkably consistent with psychophysical contrast detection thresholds. For example, detection thresholds can be reliably predicted from single-cell responses (Geisler & Albrect, 1997), VEPs (Cannon, 1983) and BOLD responses (Boynton et al., 1999). This suggests a relatively direct link between neural activity and perceived contrast. Despite their substantial dependence on the level of local contrast and their response saturation at higher contrasts, neurons in the visual cortices typically maintain their functional selectivity when contrast is varied. For example, orientation (Sclar & Freeman, 1982) and direction selectivity (Sclar et al., 1990; Tootell et al., 1995; 1997) remain invariant with stimulus contrast.
Recommended publications
  • Simultaneous EEG-Fmri Reveals Attention-Dependent Coupling of Early Face Processing with a Distributed Cortical Network
    Simultaneous EEG-fMRI reveals attention- dependent coupling of early face processing with a distributed cortical network Article Published Version Creative Commons: Attribution 4.0 (CC-BY) Open Access Bayer, M., Rubens, M. T. and Johnstone, T. (2018) Simultaneous EEG-fMRI reveals attention-dependent coupling of early face processing with a distributed cortical network. Biological Psychology, 132. pp. 133-142. ISSN 0301-0511 doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.12.002 Available at http://centaur.reading.ac.uk/74377/ It is advisable to refer to the publisher’s version if you intend to cite from the work. See Guidance on citing . To link to this article DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.12.002 Publisher: Elsevier All outputs in CentAUR are protected by Intellectual Property Rights law, including copyright law. Copyright and IPR is retained by the creators or other copyright holders. Terms and conditions for use of this material are defined in the End User Agreement . www.reading.ac.uk/centaur CentAUR Central Archive at the University of Reading Reading’s research outputs online Biological Psychology 132 (2018) 133–142 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Biological Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Simultaneous EEG-fMRI reveals attention-dependent coupling of early face T processing with a distributed cortical network ⁎ Mareike Bayera,b, , Michael T. Rubensb, Tom Johnstoneb a Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany b Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, The University of Reading, Reading, UK ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: The speed of visual processing is central to our understanding of face perception.
    [Show full text]
  • Electrophysiological Time-Course and Neural Sources
    Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2014) 14:951–969 DOI 10.3758/s13415-014-0262-2 Feeling happy enhances early spatial encoding of peripheral information automatically: electrophysiological time-course and neural sources Naomi Vanlessen & Valentina Rossi & Rudi De Raedt & Gilles Pourtois Published online: 26 February 2014 # Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014 Abstract Previous research has shown that positive mood Introduction may broaden attention, although it remains unclear whether this effect has a perceptual or a postperceptual locus. In this The broaden-and-build effects of positive emotions study, we addressed this question using high-density event- related potential methods. We randomly assigned participants The importance of positive emotions in psychological well- to a positive or a neutral mood condition. Then they per- being has increasingly gained researchers’ interest since formed a demanding oddball task at fixation (primary task Fredrickson published her influential broaden-and-build the- ensuring fixation) and a localization task of peripheral stimuli ory (Fredrickson, 2001, 2004). At the heart of this theory lies shown at three positions in the upper visual field (secondary the idea that positive and negative emotions exert opposite task) concurrently. While positive mood did not influence influences on cognitive functions: Whereas negative mood behavioral performance for the primary task, it did facilitate would trigger a narrowing of the attentional scope and behav- stimulus localization on the secondary task. At the electro- ioral repertoire, positive mood, on the other hand, would fuel physiological level, we found that the amplitude of the C1 broader thought–action tendencies and expand the attentional component (reflecting an early retinotopic encoding of the focus (Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005).
    [Show full text]
  • Multiple Mechanisms Link Prestimulus Neural Oscillations to Sensory
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Multiple mechanisms link prestimulus neural oscillations to sensory responses Luca Iemi1,2,3*, Niko A Busch4,5, Annamaria Laudini6, Saskia Haegens1,7, Jason Samaha8, Arno Villringer2,6, Vadim V Nikulin2,3,9,10* 1Department of Neurological Surgery, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York City, United States; 2Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany; 3Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russian Federation; 4Institute of Psychology, University of Mu¨ nster, Mu¨ nster, Germany; 5Otto Creutzfeldt Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Mu¨ nster, Mu¨ nster, Germany; 6Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt- Universita¨ t zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany; 7Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands; 8Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, United States; 9Department of Neurology, Charite´-Universita¨ tsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany; 10Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Berlin, Germany Abstract Spontaneous fluctuations of neural activity may explain why sensory responses vary across repeated presentations of the same physical stimulus. To test this hypothesis, we recorded electroencephalography in humans during stimulation with identical visual stimuli and analyzed how prestimulus neural oscillations modulate different stages of sensory processing reflected by distinct components of the event-related potential (ERP). We found that strong prestimulus alpha- and beta-band power resulted in a suppression of early ERP components (C1 and N150) and in an *For correspondence: amplification of late components (after 0.4 s), even after controlling for fluctuations in 1/f aperiodic [email protected] (LI); signal and sleepiness.
    [Show full text]
  • Dynamics of the Straight-Ahead Preference in Human Visual Cortex Olena Bogdanova, Volodymyr Bogdanov, Jean-Baptiste Durand, Yves Trotter, Benoit Cottereau
    Dynamics of the straight-ahead preference in human visual cortex Olena Bogdanova, Volodymyr Bogdanov, Jean-Baptiste Durand, Yves Trotter, Benoit Cottereau To cite this version: Olena Bogdanova, Volodymyr Bogdanov, Jean-Baptiste Durand, Yves Trotter, Benoit Cottereau. Dynamics of the straight-ahead preference in human visual cortex. Brain Structure and Function, Springer Verlag, 2020, 225 (1), pp.173 - 186. 10.1007/s00429-019-01988-5. hal-03016204 HAL Id: hal-03016204 https://hal-cnrs.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03016204 Submitted on 20 Nov 2020 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Brain Structure and Function (2020) 225:173–186 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01988-5 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Dynamics of the straight‑ahead preference in human visual cortex Olena V. Bogdanova1,2,3 · Volodymyr B. Bogdanov1,2,4 · Jean‑Baptiste Durand1,2 · Yves Trotter1,2 · Benoit R. Cottereau1,2 Received: 30 May 2019 / Accepted: 14 November 2019 / Published online: 2 December 2019 © The Author(s) 2019 Abstract The objects located straight-ahead of the body are preferentially processed by the visual system. They are more rapidly detected and evoke stronger BOLD responses in early visual areas than elements that are retinotopically identical but located at eccentric spatial positions.
    [Show full text]
  • Dissertation Residual Effects Of
    DISSERTATION RESIDUAL EFFECTS OF CANNABIS ON ATTENTION TOWARD AND AWARENESS OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL STUDIES Submitted by Robert D. Torrence Department of Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Colorado State University Fort Collins, Colorado Fall 2018 Doctoral Committee: Advisor: Donald C. Rojas Co-Advisor: Lucy J. Troup Janice Nerger Agnieszka Burzynska Copyright by Robert D. Torrence 2018 All Rights Reserved ABSTRACT RESIDUAL EFFECTS OF CANNABIS ON ATTENTION TOWARD AND AWARENESS OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS: EVENT-RELATED POTENTIAL STUDIES Cannabis use has increased since legalization in various states within the United States of America. Although much of the research on the neurological and psychological effects of cannabis has been on non-human animals, the current research suggests that it can have anxiolytic effects but also decrease some cognitive functioning (e.g. memory, emotional processing, etc.). Individuals with high anxiety has been suggested to have increased attentional bias towards threat-related stimuli. The purpose of the current two studies was to examine the residual effects cannabis has on attentional bias towards and awareness of emotional facial expressions. Both experiments used event-related potential (ERP) to measure brain activity related to attentional processing. Experiment 1 used a dot-probe task with fearful and neutral facial expression to examine attentional bias. The second experiment used a backward masking paradigm to restrict awareness of facial expressions (i.e. fearful, happy, and neutral). The results indicated that cannabis use was associated with differences in attentional processing. Specifically, experiment 1 suggested cannabis users had reduced attentional bias towards fearful facial expressions as compared to non-users.
    [Show full text]
  • Multiple Mechanisms Link Prestimulus Neural Oscillations to Sensory Responses
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Multiple mechanisms link prestimulus neural oscillations to sensory responses Luca Iemi1,2,3*, Niko A Busch4,5, Annamaria Laudini6, Saskia Haegens1,7, Jason Samaha8, Arno Villringer2,6, Vadim V Nikulin2,3,9,10* 1Department of Neurological Surgery, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York City, United States; 2Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany; 3Centre for Cognition and Decision Making, Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, National Research University Higher School of Economics, Moscow, Russian Federation; 4Institute of Psychology, University of Mu¨ nster, Mu¨ nster, Germany; 5Otto Creutzfeldt Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Mu¨ nster, Mu¨ nster, Germany; 6Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt- Universita¨ t zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany; 7Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands; 8Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, United States; 9Department of Neurology, Charite´-Universita¨ tsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany; 10Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Berlin, Germany Abstract Spontaneous fluctuations of neural activity may explain why sensory responses vary across repeated presentations of the same physical stimulus. To test this hypothesis, we recorded electroencephalography in humans during stimulation with identical visual stimuli and analyzed how prestimulus neural oscillations modulate different stages of sensory processing reflected by distinct components of the event-related potential (ERP). We found that strong prestimulus alpha- and beta-band power resulted in a suppression of early ERP components (C1 and N150) and in an *For correspondence: amplification of late components (after 0.4 s), even after controlling for fluctuations in 1/f aperiodic [email protected] (LI); signal and sleepiness.
    [Show full text]
  • ERP): Exploring the Time-Course of Visual Emotion Processing Using Topographic and Principal Component Analyses
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by RERO DOC Digital Library Brain Topogr (2008) 20:265–277 DOI 10.1007/s10548-008-0053-6 ORIGINAL PAPER Beyond Conventional Event-related Brain Potential (ERP): Exploring the Time-course of Visual Emotion Processing Using Topographic and Principal Component Analyses Gilles Pourtois Æ Sylvain Delplanque Æ Christoph Michel Æ Patrik Vuilleumier Accepted: 4 February 2008 / Published online: 13 March 2008 Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008 Abstract Recent technological advances with the scalp and qualitative changes in the electric field configuration EEG methodology allow researchers to record electric recorded at the scalp level, which are not apparent when fields generated in the human brain using a large number of using conventional ERP analyses. Additional information electrodes or sensors (e.g. 64–256) distributed over the gained from these approaches include the identification of a head surface (multi-channel recording). As a consequence, sequence of successive processing stages that may not fully such high-density ERP mapping yields fairly dense ERP be reflected in ERP waveforms only, and the segregation of data sets that are often hard to analyze comprehensively or multiple or partly overlapping neural events that may be to relate straightforwardly to specific cognitive or emo- blended within a single ERP waveform. These findings tional processes, because of the richness of the recorded highlight the added value of such alternative analyses when signal in both the temporal (millisecond time-resolution) exploring the electrophysiological manifestations of com- and spatial (multidimensional topographic information) plex and distributed mental functions, as for instance domains.
    [Show full text]
  • Event-Related Potential
    Event-related potential techniques. In 1935–1936, Pauline and Hallowell Davis recorded the first known ERPs on awake humans and their findings were published a few years later, in 1939. Due to World War II not much research was conducted in the 1940s, but research focusing on sensory issues picked back up again in the 1950s. In 1964, research by Grey Walter and colleagues began the modern era of ERP component discoveries when they reported the first cognitive ERP component, called the contingent nega- tive variation (CNV).[3] Sutton, Braren, and Zubin (1965) made another advancement with the discovery of the P3 component.[4] Over the next fifteen years, ERP compo- nent research became increasingly popular. The 1980s, with the introduction of inexpensive computers, opened up a new door for cognitive neuroscience research. Cur- A waveform showing several ERP components, including the rently, ERP is one of the most widely used methods in N100 and P300. Note that the ERP is plotted with negative volt- cognitive neuroscience research to study the physiological ages upward, a common, but not universal, practice in ERP re- correlates of sensory, perceptual and cognitive activity as- search sociated with processing information.[5] An event-related potential (ERP) is the measured brain response that is the direct result of a specific sensory, cognitive, or motor event.[1] More formally, it is any 2 Calculation stereotyped electrophysiological response to a stimulus. The study of the brain in this way provides a noninvasive means of evaluating brain functioning in patients with ERPs can be reliably measured using cognitive diseases.
    [Show full text]
  • Early Event Related Brain Potentials
    Biological Psychology 107 (2015) 31–43 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Biological Psychology jo urnal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho Early event-related brain potentials and hemispheric asymmetries reveal mind-wandering while reading and predict comprehension ∗ James M. Broadway , Michael S. Franklin, Jonathan W. Schooler University of California, Santa Barbara, USA a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Article history: The electroencephalogram (EEG) of mind-wandering (MW) was examined in event-related potentials Received 31 August 2014 (ERPs) and pre-stimulus alpha (8–12 Hz), over lateral-posterior sites of left and right brain hemispheres, Accepted 20 February 2015 while individuals read text passages. After controlling for individual differences in general intelligence Available online 1 March 2015 (g), P1-asymmetry was greater (right-minus- left) and N1 amplitudes were more negative, when indi- viduals were not MW (i.e., they were reading attentively). Approximately 82% of variance in reading Keywords: comprehension was accounted for by the predictors: g, pre-stimulus alpha, left- and right-hemisphere Attention P1, and left-hemisphere N1 (when individuals were not MW). Together, individual differences in MW- Event-related potentials sensitive ERPs uniquely accounted for approximately 38% of the variance in reading comprehension, over Hemispheric asymmetry and above prediction by g and pre-stimulus alpha. The within-person effect of MW on P1-asymmetry Individual differences Language was estimated to account for an additional 4.6% of criterion variance. Implications for EEG/ERP research Mind-wandering into attention, language processing, hemispheric asymmetries, and individual differences are discussed.
    [Show full text]
  • Multisensory Interactions in Early Evoked Brain Activity Follow the Principle of Inverse Effectiveness
    NeuroImage 56 (2011) 2200–2208 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Multisensory interactions in early evoked brain activity follow the principle of inverse effectiveness Daniel Senkowski a,b,c,⁎,1, Dave Saint-Amour d,e,1, Marion Höfle b, John J. Foxe a,f,⁎⁎ a The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research Program in Cognitive Neuroscience and Schizophrenia, 140 Old Orangeburg Road Orangeburg, N.Y. 10962, USA b Department of Neurophysiology and Pathophysiology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistr. 52, 20246 Hamburg, Germany c Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy Charité, University Medicine Berlin St. Hedwig Hospital, Große Hamburger Str. 5-11 10115 Berlin, Germany d Centre de Recherche, CHU Sainte-Justine, 3175, Côte-Sainte-Catherine Montreal, H3T 1C5, Canada e Département de Psychologie, Université du Québec à Montréal, C.P. 8888, Montreal, H3C 3P8, Canada f The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Children's Evaluation and Rehabilitation Center (CERC) Departments of Pediatrics and Neuroscience, Albert Einstein College of Medicine Van Etten Building, Wing 1C, 1225 Morris Park Avenue, Bronx, NY 10461, USA article info abstract Article history: A major determinant of multisensory integration, derived from single-neuron studies in animals, is the Received 22 December 2010 principle of inverse effectiveness (IE), which describes the phenomenon whereby maximal multisensory Revised 9 March 2011 response enhancements occur when the constituent unisensory stimuli are minimally effective in evoking Accepted 28 March 2011 responses. Human behavioral studies, which have shown that multisensory interactions are strongest when Available online 8 April 2011 stimuli are low in intensity are in agreement with the IE principle, but the neurophysiologic basis for this finding is unknown.
    [Show full text]
  • Changes in Early Cortical Visual Processing Predict Enhanced Reactivity in Deaf Individuals
    Changes in Early Cortical Visual Processing Predict Enhanced Reactivity in Deaf Individuals Davide Bottari1,2*, Anne Caclin3,4, Marie-He´le`ne Giard3,4, Francesco Pavani1,2 1 Department of Cognitive Sciences and Education, University of Trento, Trento, Italy, 2 Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Trento, Italy, 3 INSERM, U1028; CNRS, UMR5292; Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, Lyon, France, 4 University Lyon 1, Lyon, France Abstract Individuals with profound deafness rely critically on vision to interact with their environment. Improvement of visual performance as a consequence of auditory deprivation is assumed to result from cross-modal changes occurring in late stages of visual processing. Here we measured reaction times and event-related potentials (ERPs) in profoundly deaf adults and hearing controls during a speeded visual detection task, to assess to what extent the enhanced reactivity of deaf individuals could reflect plastic changes in the early cortical processing of the stimulus. We found that deaf subjects were faster than hearing controls at detecting the visual targets, regardless of their location in the visual field (peripheral or peri- foveal). This behavioural facilitation was associated with ERP changes starting from the first detectable response in the striate cortex (C1 component) at about 80 ms after stimulus onset, and in the P1 complex (100–150 ms). In addition, we found that P1 peak amplitudes predicted the response times in deaf subjects, whereas in hearing individuals visual reactivity and ERP amplitudes correlated only at later stages of processing. These findings show that long-term auditory deprivation can profoundly alter visual processing from the earliest cortical stages.
    [Show full text]
  • ENGAGEMENT with ANGRY FACES DURING ATTENTIONAL BIAS MODIFICATION: INSIGHTS from the N2pc
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by ResearchArchive at Victoria University of Wellington ENGAGEMENT WITH ANGRY FACES DURING ATTENTIONAL BIAS MODIFICATION: INSIGHTS FROM THE N2pc BY LISA MICHELLE HUNKIN A thesis submitted to the Victoria University of Wellington in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Cognitive and Behavioural Neuroscience Victoria University of Wellington 2014 ENGAGEMENT WITH THREAT IN ATTENTIONAL BIAS MODIFICATION i Abstract Healthy individuals show an attentional bias toward threat, and this bias is exaggerated in anxious individuals. Recent studies have shown that training anxious individuals to attend to neutral information can reduce their threat bias and anxiety levels. This training is called attentional bias modification (ABM). However, despite the large literature on ABM, it is still unclear how ABM achieves its effects. Two mechanisms – facilitated engagement with threat, and delayed disengagement from threat – are thought to be involved in the threat bias. In this thesis, I investigated the effects of ABM on engagement with angry faces. First, in Experiment 1 I developed an ABM task to train healthy individuals to attend to either angry or neutral faces. Participants completed a dot-probe task in which they saw two faces – one angry and one neutral – followed by a target that appeared more often in the location of either the angry or neutral face (depending on their respective training condition). Experiment 1 was successful at inducing a bias. Next, Experiment 2 used this task to investigate the effects of ABM on event-related potentials before, during, and after training.
    [Show full text]