Frederick Le Play, the Developmental Paradigm, Reading History Sideways, and Family Myths

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Frederick Le Play, the Developmental Paradigm, Reading History Sideways, and Family Myths Preliminary Draft1 Frederick Le Play, the Developmental Paradigm, Reading History Sideways, and Family Myths by Arland Thornton Population Studies Center, Survey Research Center, and Department of Sociology The University of Michigan Ann Arbor, MI 48109 December 2005 Working Paper, Population Studies Center, the University of Michigan 1 This paper is a preliminary draft that has not gone through final proofing and verification. Readers who find errors are invited to inform me of the mistakes so that they can be corrected in future revisions. Preliminary Draft Frederick Le Play, the Developmental Paradigm, Reading History Sideways, and Family Myths In 1969 the Cambridge Group for the History of Population and Social Structure held an international conference on the comparative history of household and family. The major issue of the conference was household and family structure and size in the past. The conference participants considered a substantial body of data casting severe reservations toward the “assumption that in the past the household was universally large and complicated, and that historical progress has always been from big and complex to small and simple”. In fact, the conference apparently manifested “…a certain animus against ‘the myth of the extended family’…” (Laslett, 1974/l972:ix-x). Frederick Le Play was undoubtedly not invited to the Cambridge conference since he had died in 1882. However, his ghost apparently attended the conference since his ideas and legacy dominated the book that reported the conference proceedings. Peter Laslett, editor of the conference volume, asserted that Le Play “…was, and perhaps still is, the strongest single influence on the historical study of the family” (Laslett, 1974/l972:16). In the preface and introduction to the volume, Laslett repeatedly referred to Le Play and his pivotal role in the establishment of the idea of extended families in the past. Laslett also discussed briefly data collected and reported by Le Play in his book The Workers of Europe. An implicit, if not explicit, theme of Laslett’s introductory essay is a sense of bewilderment about how Le Play, given his own data and the data assembled for the Cambridge conference, could have reached his conclusion about the historical transition from extended to nuclear families in Western societies. Writing nearly two decades after this Cambridge Conference, Laslett (l987:271) displayed a sense of frustration about the inability of present-day historians to dispel the myth of extended families in the past. He wrote that it had been “…a relatively straightforward task to 1 Preliminary Draft expose the misconceptions,” yet misinformation about the past continued. “Hard as we have worked on these points in the last fifteen or twenty years,” Laslett complained, “some of the final consequences of Le Play’s having taken the wrong turning in the l850s still remain.” So who was this Frederick Le Play? Who was this man who dominated historical family scholarship for more than a century after his death? What theoretical assumptions and methods caused him to take a wrong turn and become the central architect of the myth that families had been extended in the past and had changed to nuclear as a result of the transition from a rural agricultural to an urban industrial society? In researching Frederick Le Play I discovered one of the giants of social science history. Le Play began his career as a metallurgist and industrial engineer and later became interested in human behavior and family relations.2 In the middle of the nineteenth century he carried out and reported in The Workers of Europe one of the most monumental and successful studies ever conducted in the social sciences. One of Le Play’s contributions in The Workers of Europe was a system for categorizing family types that still informs the work of family scholars today. A more important contribution was his description of the geographical distribution of family types in mid-nineteenth century Europe—a description that has been verified by a large battery of family research conducted in the second half of the twentieth century. As I argue later in this 2 Le Play, who was born in Normandy in 1806 and died in 1882 in Paris, began his very successful career in the physical sciences and engineering. As Kellner (1972) wrote, “his life before 1855 would provide enough achievement to fill four life-long careers”. Among other things, he conducted and published research in mining, metallurgy, the steel industry, and industrial relations. He was a professor at the Ecole des Mines, sat on three governmental commissions, and managed a mining and industrial operation in Russia (Brooke, 1970). From the beginning Le Play displayed a strong interest in the human side of mining and industrial operations (Brooke, 1970) and in human welfare more generally (Le Play, 1937/1879: 365-367). He also traveled extensively in his numerous roles as university professor, engineering researcher, government commissioner, and industrial manager, which undoubtedly increased his interest in the issues of social science. Le Play wrote that his social science apprentice was well advanced by 1843, from 1848 to 1855 his metallurgical career dwindled as he devoted more and more time to his social science work, and after the publication of his book in 1855, he devoted himself entirely to social science (Le Play, 1937/1879: 573-575). 2 Preliminary Draft paper, Le Play went wrong only when he utilized the developmental paradigm and reading history sideways to draw conclusions about social history and processes from his international comparative family data. That is, he used information from societies he believed to be less developed to proxy for the pasts of societies he perceived as more advanced. Studying Families by Direct Observation It is important to note that this metallurgist-turned-sociologist was committed to an empirical approach to the study of human affairs. He strongly believed that social science conclusions should be arrived at inductively through the observation of human experience rather than deductively through reliance on preconceived theoretical orientations. He wrote that: In scientific matters, only direct observation of facts can lead to rigorous conclusions and to their acceptance. This principle is acknowledged today in the physical sciences, but it is still unrecognized in social science. The practitioners of social science are generally inspired by preconceived idea… People imbued with such biases tend to disdain the facts and the conclusions which can be induced from them. Social science thus remains in a situation comparable to that of the physical sciences when they were based on the conceptions of astrology and alchemy; social science will not be established until it is founded on observation” (Le Play, 1982/1862:179). So how did Le Play plan to advance social science beyond astrology and alchemy? It was through the direct observation of individuals and their families. Although Le Play directed a government statistical agency from 1830 to 1848, he was skeptical of the value of statistics in the description and understanding of society (Silver 1982). He believed that statistics were too isolated from the real world and could not provide reliable information concerning people. He wanted detailed and direct observations of individuals and their families rather than the second- hand data of statistics. Because of his distrust of statistics and his reliance on direct observation 3 Preliminary Draft of individuals and families, Le Play, unlike some other researchers in the 1800s, did not appear to use in his research the limited statistical data available (for a discussion of the use of statistics by Malthus and Westermarck, see Thornton 2005b, 2005c). Although Le Play did not use standard sampling techniques, he was aware of the need to choose families carefully in order for them to represent the larger population. As he put it, “we proceed like the zoologist who applies the investigative techniques of anatomy and physiology to a few individuals in order to describe an entire species” (Le Play, 1982/1862:179-180). Families rather than individuals were chosen as the unit of analysis because of Le Play’s conviction that families were the true social building blocks of society. In practice, Le Play followed a purposive strategy to select families—where invidiual families were chosen directly rather than through a random procedure. Le Play believed that he could choose average families in a particular area who could be representative of a larger group of families. As he put it: “In any social class, it is important to select a family native to the area and of nearly average conditions, that is, neither superior nor inferior to others in respect to its material situation or its morality” (Le Play 1982/1862, pages 180-181). Le Play believed that in order for valid information to be collected from the families selected, the investigator needed two essential attributes. The first was “a sincere love of science, which leads one to seek the truth and record the facts with scrupulous accuracy” (Le Play 1982/1862, page 181). He believed that such scrupulous attention to truth and direct recording of observations could overcome any biases a person might bring to a research project. The second essential condition for accurate evaluation was the ability to gain the confidence of the family under observation. This confidence was necessary because Le Play’s method required extensive interaction between the investigator and the family studied. 4 Preliminary Draft Once the families were selected for study they were subjected to an intense and multifaceted data collection effort, what Le Play called the monographic method. Le Play suggested that the field worker should spend from eight days to a month with each family participating in the study (Higgs, 1890:423). During this time the investigator observed the family and the activities of its individual members.
Recommended publications
  • Integrating Play and Family Therapies to Help Children with Anxiety
    Integrating Play and Family Therapies to Help Children with Anxiety | EMAN TADROS, MS, MFT 16 | PLAYTHERAPY | March 2018 | www.a4pt.org COMMENTS BY CLINICAL EDITOR: Suggestions for helping children with anxiety through family play therapy. raditional family therapy approaches are significantly Combining Play and Family Therapies geared more towards adolescents and adults, though Keith and Whitaker (1981) posited that there are many parallels Lund, Zimmerman, and Haddock (2002) argued that between the process of play therapy and family therapy, notably, family therapy “can become child friendly with a little that structure is critical, scope is increased through magic and adaptation and creativity” (p. 448). Lund and colleagues rituals, play constantly weaves the symbolic and the real, and body (2002) reported many barriers that prevent therapists language is always implicit. They proposed that play therapy utilizes from including young children in family therapy, including a belief T a “parental surrogate” to help children adjust on biopsychosocial that it was acceptable to exclude children from family therapy levels to different settings in their world, such as home, school, sessions if the therapist was uncomfortable with their presence. and playground (Keith & Whitaker, 1981, p. 244). Play therapists are Willis, Walters, and Crane (2014) offered that marriage and family already familiar with how the interplay between symbolic and real therapists (MFT) “tend to view child-focused work as the realm is freely exhibited in the child’s self-expression (Landreth, 2012), as of child or play therapists rather than family therapists” (p. 288). well as how structure is crucial to the process in both directive and I would argue that child-focused work is the duty of all therapists non-directive approaches.
    [Show full text]
  • Play Parent Workbook
    PL Physical Literacy AY Assessment for Youth CS4L PHYSICAL LITERACY parentCS4L PLAYparent Workbook 1 TABLE OF CONTENTS What is Physical Literacy? 4 What is PLAY? 5 What is PLAYparent? 6 PLAYparent Workbook 7 Physical Literacy Visual Analog Scale (VAS) 8 Cognitive Domain: Confdence, Motivation and Comprehension 9 Framework 9 Question 1. Confdence to participate in physical activity and sport 9 Question 2. Motivation to participate in physical activity and sport 10 Question 3. Understands movement terms like skip, gallop, hop and jump 10 Question 4. Desire to participate in activities alone 11 Question 5. Desire to participate in activities with others or in groups 11 Question 6. Knowledge related to healthy physical activity 12 Scoring & Calls-to-action 13 Motor Competence 14 Locomotor 14 Framework 14 Question 7. Coordination when moving 14 Question 8. Safety while moving in the environment relative to others 15 Question 9. Number of movement skills acquired 15 Question 10. Ability to balance during movement 15 Question 11. Ability to run 16 Question 12. Ability to start, stop and change directions 16 Scoring & Calls-to-action 17 Object Control 18 Framework 18 Question 13. Ability to use hands to throw, catch and carry objects 18 Question 14. Ability to use feet to kick or move objects 19 Question 15. Left side is as capable as the right side 19 Scoring & Calls-to-action 21 Environment 22 Framework 22 Question 16. Amount of participation in water activities 22 Question 17. Amount of participation in indoor activities 22 Question 18. Amount of participation in outdoor activities 23 Question 19.
    [Show full text]
  • Attachment in the Classroom
    Educ Psychol Rev (2009) 21:141–170 DOI 10.1007/s10648-009-9104-0 REVIEW ARTICLE Attachment in the Classroom Christi Bergin & David Bergin Published online: 21 May 2009 # Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2009 Abstract Attachment influences students’ school success. This is true of students’ attachment to their parents, as well as to their teachers. Secure attachment is associated with higher grades and standardized test scores compared to insecure attachment. Secure attachment is also associated with greater emotional regulation, social competence, and willingness to take on challenges, and with lower levels of ADHD and delinquency, each of which in turn is associated with higher achievement. These effects tend to be stronger for high-risk students. In this era of accountability, enhancing teacher–student relationships is not merely an add-on, but rather is fundamental to raising achievement. Understanding the role of attachment in the classroom will help educators be more effective, particularly with challenging students. Twelve suggestions to improve teacher–student relationships and school bonding are provided. Keywords Attachment . Achievement . School bonding . Teacher–student relationship Children’s socioemotional well-being is critical to school success, and attachment is the foundation of socioemotional well-being. Because of this, educators—from preschool to high school—can be more effective if they understand how attachment influences their students. Attachment influences school success through two routes: indirectly through attachment to parents, and directly through attachment to teachers and schools. In this article, we briefly review basic attachment concepts for readers who are not familiar with attachment research. We then show how attachment to parents is linked to school success, and we summarize the limited research on attachment-like relationships with teachers.
    [Show full text]
  • Active Play: Simple Games and Activity Ideas
    Active Play: Simple Games and Activity Ideas Children have different needs and abilities depending upon their age. Children need a safe environment that promotes and encourages their physical, intellectual, emotional and social growth. Keep the ages and needs of the children in your care in mind when planning games and activities. Younger children have shorter attention spans, and are just learning how to coordinator their bodies, while older children may get bored with not enough of a “challenge” in the game or activities. Read more about children’s developmental ages and stages: Growth and Development Child’s Development Ages & Stages Go, Go, Stop Pick a leader, and start and end spots. Have children line up at the start and the leader stand at the end with his back to the children. The object is to get to the end where the leader is. Have the leader yell go, go, go for a few seconds (i.e., 2 second, 10 seconds, etc.) and then yell stop. All children stop and freeze. If the leader turns around and sees any child moving, that child has to go back to the starting spot. The game ends when a child touches the leader. Follow the Leader To begin the game, you may want an adult to lead. Children line up behind the leader. The leader decides the actions for children to follow. The children mimic the leader's actions. Any players who fail to follow or do what the leader does are out of the game. Change leaders and give every child time to lead the others.
    [Show full text]
  • Scaling-Up Playful Parenting
    SCALING-UP PLAYFUL PARENTING The LEGO Foundation and UNICEF in partnership with the Early Childhood Development Action Network (ECDAN), Africa Early Childhood Network (AfECN), International Step by Step Association (ISSA) and Aga Khan Development Network (AKDN) Parents and primary caregivers have an important role to play in their children’s development. Their responsive caregiving sets a critical trajectory for brain growth in young children, which lays a solid foundation for healthy development and lifelong learning. Evidence from a range of countries shows that when caregivers engage in quality play and reading with their children, it can result in improved learning outcomes that persist throughout the life course. On October 29th, 2019, the LEGO Foundation announced a new partnership with UNICEF to scale parenting interventions that promote playful interactions, increase global knowledge and buy-in for playful parenting programs, and bolster primary caregivers’ demand for playful learning during the early years. This partnership aligns with the Foundation’s 2019 commitment which pledged to bring to scale impactful programs that support parents and caregivers to strengthen their children’s development through play. While ample evidence confirms the benefits of Playful Parenting programs, such efforts have rarely expanded beyond the local level. The LEGO Foundation, by way of its Playful Parenting initiative, is investing in bringing programs to scale and developing learning to help ensure sustainability and continued expansion. “Stimulating
    [Show full text]
  • IS NATURE OR NURTURE to BLAME? NO, IT’S the BRAIN! Revista Iberoamericana De Psicología Del Ejercicio Y El Deporte, Vol
    Revista Iberoamericana de Psicología del Ejercicio y el Deporte ISSN: 1886-8576 [email protected] Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria España Barreiros, André; Abreu, Ana Maria SPORTS EXPERTISE: IS NATURE OR NURTURE TO BLAME? NO, IT’S THE BRAIN! Revista Iberoamericana de Psicología del Ejercicio y el Deporte, vol. 12, núm. 2, 2017, pp. 307-312 Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, España Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=311151242014 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative REVISTA IBEROAMERICANA DE PSICOLOGÍA DEL EJERCICIO Y EL DEPORTE Vol. 12 nº 2 pp. 307-312 ISSN 1886-8576 SPORTS EXPERTISE: IS NATURE OR NURTURE TO BLAME? NO, IT’S THE BRAIN! André Barreiros1,2 & Ana Maria Abreu3 Universidade Lusófona do Porto1, Portugal; Universidade do Porto2, Portugal; Universidade Europeia, Laureate International Universities3, Lisboa, Portugal ABSTRACT: For long, people have wondered about the reasons for the superior performance of elite athletes. As it seems, researchers have been divided between reasons that pertain to nature and those that pertain to nurture. More recently, more complex interactionist theories have come to light. These theories posit that both genes and environment contribute to the development of motor expertise in a non-linear way. It is possible that this discussion might never be resolved. Here, we propose that instead of concentrating on the reasons “why”, we concentrate on the “how”, i.e., brain function associated to motor expertise.
    [Show full text]
  • Bf Skinner's Theory and Education
    441 Institute for Christian Teaching Education Department of Seventh-day Adventists B. F. SKINNER'S THEORY AND EDUCATION: A CHRISTIAN CRITIQUE by Ademola Stephen Tayo Babcock University Ilishan-Remo, Nigeria 482-00 Institute for Christian Teaching 12501 Old Columbia Pike Silver Spring, MD 20904 USA Prepared for the 28th International Faith and Learning Seminar held at Babcock University, Nigeria June 17-28, 2001 442 INTRODUCTION 1 The use of rewards to alter classroom behavior is well established in literatme • One of the leading proponents of the behavioristic school ofthought is B. F. Skinner. His theory 2 3 has been used with great success among substance abusers , hearing impaired children and 4 the mentally handicapped • Christian teachers have equally adopted the technique in 5 6 encouraging evangelis~ , memorization and recitation of Bible verses , the discipline of students in the classroom, 7 as well as in enhancing academic performance. 8 Skinner's operant conditioning has come under criticism by both Christians and non­ Christians alike. The validity of his experimental procedures has been challenged. 9 Others question his assumption about human nature. Furtherstill, some claim that his method may cause students to become dependent on extrinsic rather than intrinsic rewards. The purpose of this essay is to look at the life of B. F. Skinner and circumstances in his life that shaped his idea, examine his theoretical framework as it relates to education, and finally set forth a Christian response to the theory as it bears on the educational goals and objectives, nature of students, teaching methodology, and the teaching-learning environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 11: Behaviorism
    Chapter 11: Behaviorism Megan Davis, Nicole Pelley and Stephanie Quinlan Behaviorism (1892-1956) ● Psychology has been the study of the mind since the Greeks ○ The definition of the mind has been debated extensively ○ 20th century: Shift from what the mind was to what it did ■ Mind causes behavior ● New field of research ○ Psychology was redefined with help from animal psychology ○ People started believing humans evolved from animal forms ○ Had to rethink Descartes’ definition of the mind New Directions in Animal Psychology New Directions in Animal Psychology Animal psychology as Romanes begun it, used 2 methods: 1. Anecdotal Method → Collect data 2. Method of Inference → Interpret data Close examination in late 19th, early 20th century. Anecdote → Experiment From Anecdote to Experiment ● Experiment replaced anecdotes and informal, naturalistic experiments ● Aim of animal psychology - produce natural science and anecdote not the path to science ● Two important research programs: ○ Thorndike ○ Pavlov From Anecdote to Experiment Edward Lee Thorndike (1874-1949): ● Initially wanted to study children ● Not many readily available, took up animals ● Studied with William James ● Developed “connectionism” ○ Methodological and theoretical approach to animal learning ○ Formulation of an S-R psychology he called “connectionism” ○ Anecdotal method overestimated animal intelligence From Anecdote to Experiment Thorndike’s Puzzle Boxes ● Trap cat inside box ● Each box opened by cat in different way ● Rewarded with salmon for escaping ○ Ex. of instrumental
    [Show full text]
  • Playpandemic-Newer
    #PlayPandemic DON’T JUST SURVIVE... THRIVE Move over, Corona, there’s a new pandemic in town. It’s Play and it’s spreading like wildfire throughout the globe. I know, Covid-19 is no laughing matter - my heart #PlayPandemic aches for all those suffering from it’s far reaching effects. We’re going to meet the panic, fear and uncertainty with passion, purpose and joy. TABLE OF CONTENTS: What follows on these pages is an offering WELCOME / PAGE 3 of guidance, reassurance and practical steps to transform your forced-seclusion and social EXTRA RESOURCES / PAGE 4 distancing into a true family retreat. PLAY TIME / PAGE 8 This guide, and all the materials linked in it - are aimed to serve you. Pick and choose what works FOCUS TIME / PAGE 10 for you and release the rest. This is a #guiltfree and #judgementfree zone - we believe there is no one- MESSY TIME/ PAGE 13 size-fits-all and no such thing as “behind”. MOVEMENT TIME/ PAGE 16 So dive right in, make it your own and share it out. Let’s spread the #PlayPandemic like a wild fire. QUIET TIME/ PAGE 18 FAMILY TIME/ PAGE 20 SCREEN TIME / PAGE 22 DAILY FLOW SHEET / PAGE 27 Hey, I would love the chance to support you further and connect with you on a deeper and more personal level. Connect With Me Via Email I’ll share Live Q&A’s, updated play ideas and behind the scenes snippets... 2 WELCOME! At home all day with your kids? Social distancing? Now comes the tough love part: If you’re “stuck” at home with your Anxiety?...in this uncertain time I know you kids either way for the foreseeable future you have two choices: probably have so many questions, just like all the A) Suffer through it, waiting for it to end, perhaps crippled by anxiety other parents who reach out to me asking: How to or bursts of rage..
    [Show full text]
  • 1 a Study of Nature Versus Nurture, While Diagnosing
    1 A STUDY OF NATURE VERSUS NURTURE, WHILE DIAGNOSING ANXIETY OR CONDUCT DISORDERS IN ADOLESCENTS By Austin Laura Schnur Advised by Dr. Johnston SOC 461, 462 Senior Project Social Science Department College of Liberal Arts CALIFORNIA POLYTECHNIC STATE UNIVERSITY Winter, 2017 Nature vs. Nurture While Diagnosing Anxiety or Conduct Disorders 2 Table of contents Research Proposal………………………………………………………….3 Annotated Bibliography…………………………………………………...5 Outline………………………………………………………………………11 Text……………………………………………………………………………. I. Introduction……………………………………………………...13 II. Definitions, Symptoms, and Examples………………………...15 III. Literature Reviews……………………………………………..23 IV. Discussion……………………………………………………….30 V. Personal Experience……………………………………………..33 VI. Conclusion………………………………………………………36 Works Cited…………………………………………………………………38 3 Research Proposal Nature vs. Nurture While Diagnosing Anxiety or Conduct Disorders in Adolescents Purpose or Goal of the Project The purpose of this senior project is to learn more about the influences, whether environmental or genetic, on mental disorders such as anxiety and conduct disorders. Goals include researching about environmental factors that come into play with diagnosing these mental disorders, as well as learning more about the genetic factors that take part. This research will also help me come to personal conclusions on whether nature, such as genetic and biological factors, or nurture, such as social and environmental interactions are more central when, adolescents develop anxiety or conduct behaviors. How this Project will be Accomplished The study on nature versus nurture while diagnosing mental disorders in adolescents will be accomplished through review of journal articles and research data. This research will include studying certain theories in relation to how adolescents develop anxiety or conduct behaviors, considering different standpoints and opinions from research articles, and understanding how both factors can take part in a diagnosis.
    [Show full text]
  • Play (Activity)
    Play (activity) 1 Definitions Child playing with bubbles Pen spinning The seminal text in the field of play studies is the book Homo Ludens first published in 1944 with several subse- quent editions, in which Johan Huizinga defines play as [2]:13 In psychology and ethology, play is a range of voluntary, follows: intrinsically motivated activities normally associated with “Summing up the formal characteristic of recreational pleasure and enjoyment.[1] Play is most com- play, we might call it a free activity standing monly associated with children and their juvenile-level quite consciously outside 'ordinary' life as be- activities, but play can also be a useful adult activity, and ing 'not serious’ but at the same time absorbing occurs among other higher-functioning (non-human) an- the player intensely and utterly. It is an activ- imals as well. ity connected with no material interest, and no Many of the most prominent researchers in the field profit can be gained by it. It proceeds within its of psychology (including Melanie Klein, Jean Piaget, own proper boundaries of time and space ac- William James, Sigmund Freud, Carl Jung and Lev cording to fixed rules and in an orderly manner. Vygotsky) have viewed play as endemic to the human It promotes the formation of social groupings species. These psychologists all had strong beliefs on how that tend to surround themselves with secrecy important play was on human development. Many re- and to stress the difference from the common search methods were performed to prove their theories. world by disguise or other means.” Play is often interpreted as frivolous; yet the player This definition of play as constituting a separate and inde- can be intently focused on their objective, particularly pendent sphere of human activity is sometimes referred when play is structured and goal-oriented, as in a game.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Play Therapy Is Appropriate for Children with Symptoms of PTSD: 6 Reasons Why Play Therapy Is an Effective Treatment Choice for Children with Trauma
    Why Play Therapy is Appropriate for Children with Symptoms of PTSD: 6 Reasons Why Play Therapy is an Effective Treatment Choice for Children with Trauma BY ASSOCIATION FOR PLAY THERAPY BOARD OF DIRECTORS, APRIL 2020 lay therapy is a developmentally congruent intervention that helps children build coping skills, enhance their capacity for self-regulation, and promotes positive self-esteem (Bratton et al., 2005; Lin & Bratton, 2015; Ray et CN 6JG#UUQEKCVKQPHQT2NC[6JGTCR[ PF FGƒPGURNC[VJGTCR[CUūVJGU[UVGOCVKEWUGQHCVJGQTGVKECN model to establish an interpersonal process wherein trained Play Therapists use the therapeutic powers of RNC[VQJGNRENKGPVURTGXGPVQTTGUQNXGRU[EJQUQEKCNFKHƒEWNVKGUCPFCEJKGXGQRVKOCNITQYVJCPFFGXGNQROGPVŬ P (para. 3). Play therapy has a longstanding history as a treatment for children who have experienced trauma or who display symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder (Shelby, 2000; Vicario et al., 2013). In play therapy, children explore feelings and thoughts VJTQWIJU[ODQNKETGRTGUGPVCVKQPOGFKCVKPIVJGPGWTQNQIKECNCPFFGXGNQROGPVCNKORCEVQHVTCWOCCPFCVVCEJOGPVFGƒEKVU (Gaskill & Perry, 2017; Gil, 2017; Goodyear-Brown, 2019; Lefebre, 2018). Young children rely on play and play therapy to GZRTGUUVJGKTGOQVKQPCNYQTNFTGUQNXGVJGKTEJCNNGPIGUCPFRTQDNGOUCPFFGXGNQROGCPKPIHWNTGNCVKQPUJKRUǡ 9KVJKPVJGƒGNFQHRNC[VJGTCR[NKEGPUGFOGPVCNJGCNVJRTQHGUUKQPCNUWUGFKTGEVKXGCPFPQPFKTGEVKXGOGVJQFUVQCFFTGUU children’s trauma symptoms. Directive methods involve structuring play activities to target emotions, cognitions, and behaviors while nondirective methods
    [Show full text]