Instruction on Sight Word Automaticity

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Instruction on Sight Word Automaticity Running head: INSTRUCTION ON SIGHT WORD AUTOMATICITY THE EFFECTS OF SIGHT WORD WRITING INSTRUCTION ON SIGHT WORD AUTOMATICITY IN THE READING MODE A Research Project Presented to the Faculty of the School of Education Viterbo University Jill M. Peterson, Ed. D Jill M. Peterson, Ed. D. Research Advisor Susan R. Hughes, Ed. D. Coordinator of Graduate Research in Education Tracy Stewart, Ph. D. Vice President for Academic Affairs In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Education By Bethany Noble August 2019 INSTRUCTION ON SIGHT WORD AUTOMATICITY 2 Abstract The purpose of this action research study is to investigate the connection between reading and writing through the impact of sight word writing instruction on sight word reading automaticity. This twelve-week study was conducted in a rural school district in a Midwest community at the elementary building. The participants in the study included two first grade students that had previously qualified for Title 1 reading services. Each student was given pretests and posttests that consisted of sight word assessments in the reading and writing modes. After the pretest, both students were given almost daily sight word writing instruction that included sight word review and learning to write one sight word. The results indicated positive growth in sight word automaticity in the reading mode and written sight word knowledge for both participants. By explicitly teaching sight words in the writing mode, students may acquire sight word automaticity in the reading mode. Further research is needed to study the effect of sight word writing instruction with students in various elementary settings and with larger sample sizes. A comparable study that utilizes a control and treatment group may also be necessary. INSTRUCTION ON SIGHT WORD AUTOMATICITY 3 The Effects of Sight Word Writing Instruction on Sight Word Automaticity in the Reading Mode Over the past six years, I have taught literacy to young students ranging from kindergarten through second grade. Many early literacy teachers may agree that reading is complex and needs a combination of acquired skills in order to be successful, yet a student’s goal is to understand the text. To do this, students need to have reading instruction in phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, and comprehension (National Reading Panel, 2017). Fluency plays an essential part in students’ ability to comprehend the text. “Without fluency, the student cannot comprehend the material: too much attention must be devoted to decoding individual words” (Fasko & Fasko, 2010, p. 61). One of the components of early literacy that builds on decoding and fluency is sight word recognition. “Sight word reading is particularly important for reading English because one third of written words in English contain letters that do not follow the letter sound rules” (McArther, Castles, Kohnen, Larsen, Jones, Anandakumar, & Banales, 2015, p. 393). Ehri (2005) suggested a focus on sight word instruction for beginning readers, and educators have placed it as an integral part of early reading. However, I have struggled with the best practices surrounding this instruction. Our students need to be given the opportunities to internalize their sight word learning and apply it to their writing. “An early reader who builds automaticity with a bank of known words will read and write more fluently” (Richardson, 2016, p. 111). Problem Statement As an early elementary teacher, I have observed many students struggle while reading unknown words that cannot be phonetically decoded. Additionally, the students have not built enough meaning in the text to strategically solve the unknown word. These sight words, or high INSTRUCTION ON SIGHT WORD AUTOMATICITY 4 frequency words, can become endless hurdles when students are reading and writing. Yaw, Skinner, Orsega, Parkhurst, Booher, and Chambers (2012) suggested if students begin to learn sight words, their confidence in reading can grow and less frustration is observed. I have tried many sight word instructional practices that range from flashcard drill to reading words in sentences or with animal voices. Although I have observed students’ motivation and engagement through this instruction, there is limited observation of these practices building the automaticity students need to become successful readers and writers. Richardson (2016) writes about the connection between reading and writing in her suggested framework. “Reading and writing are interwoven in the emergent guided reading lesson framework because they are reciprocal processes” (Richardson, 2016, p. 88). The problem that I identified in my practice was that students were not building their sight word recognition in the reading mode in order to maintain the knowledge and apply it to the writing mode or a variety of reading texts. Purpose Statement My concern for students to build an appropriate sight word recognition base in the reading mode has grown deeper. Students need to have the ability to apply their sight word knowledge to writing and other subject areas. Educators have used a variety of sight word instructional strategies in their teaching practices. Some of these strategies include traditional flashcard methods, multisensory approaches, and technology-based learning (Broz, Blust, & Berteisen, 2016; Musti-Rao, Lo, & Plati, 2014; Philips, 2012). However, recognizing the link between reading and writing can have positive affects for students learning (Anderson & Briggs, 2011; Ehri, 2014; Jones & Reutzel, 2015; McNaughton, 2014; Parr & McNaughton, 2014). “Making this connection has important implications for all readers, and particularly for those INSTRUCTION ON SIGHT WORD AUTOMATICITY 5 who experience difficulty in learning to read and write” (Anderson & Briggs, 2011, p. 546). Richardson (2016) states, “By teaching children how to write these words and not just read them, you help them control visual scanning and improve visual memory, which promotes better spelling skills” (p. 130). The specific purpose of this action research study is to investigate this connection through the effect of sight word writing instruction on sight word reading automaticity. Research Questions and Sub-Questions Based on the problem that I identified in my practice and my desire to study my practice, I designed a study that addressed the following primary question: What is the effect of sight word instruction in the writing mode on sight word automaticity in the reading mode? In addition to the primary question for the study, I identified and posed several sub-questions that included: (a) what is the importance of reading fluency on student achievement? (b) what are the effects of sight word instructional studies? (c) what is the connection between writing and reading? Definitions This action research has terms that require further explanation. Sight words, also known as high frequency words, can be defined as “a sizable portion of the text students encounter in reading. Many of these words do not conform to phonetic rules” (Musti-Rao, Lo, & Plati, 2015, p. 154). Fasko and Fasko (2010) suggested, “any word that is read sufficiently often becomes a sight word that is read from memory” (p. 39). In this study, sight words were referred to as the school building’s first grade list of words that students commonly see in texts and in their writing (see Appendix A). Sight word reading is defined when “readers read familiar words by INSTRUCTION ON SIGHT WORD AUTOMATICITY 6 accessing them in memory” (Ehri, 2005, p. 167). Sight word automaticity was described as “reading words from memory accurately and automatically in or out of text” (Ehri, 2014, p. 5). The definition of the sight word writing instruction that the participants of the study received was influenced by Richardson’s (2016) emergent and early guided-reading lesson plans (see Appendix B). The following description was replicated by Richardson’s (2016) work. Sight word instruction for this study was conducted approximately daily. The same word was taught for approximately two days before introducing a new word. Each instructional day, I began the new sight word learning by introducing the new word on a dry-erase board. I slid an index card left to right across the word as students examined it. After the introduction of the word, the following activities were conducted in this order: What’s Missing, Mix & Fix, Table Writing, and Write It (and Retrieve it). “The activities use a gradual release of responsibility, with you [teacher] doing more of the work at first and students writing independently by the end” (Richardson, 2016, p. 78). The first activity, What’s Missing, was defined as “erase a letter…show the board to students, and ask them to tell you the missing letter” (Richardson, 2016, p. 78). The second routine, Mix and Fix, gave students the opportunity to make the word with magnetic letters. Students would “slide each letter … then mix the letters and remake (fix) the word from left to right” (Richardson, 2016, p. 79). The third activity, Table Writing, had “students use their index finger to ‘write’ the word on the table” (Richardson, 2016, p. 79). The final activity, Write It (and Retrieve It), required three steps. Students wrote the new word, wrote a very familiar word, and finished with writing the new word again. The teacher dictated the sequence of writing (Richardson, 2016, p. 79). Limitations INSTRUCTION ON SIGHT WORD AUTOMATICITY 7 Although the findings of this study suggested a positive change in the students’ sight word automaticity in the reading modes, there were limitations within the research. The first limitation was time. The data from the action research project was gathered during a 12-week time frame. In order to fully observe if students maintained their sight word knowledge, an extended study would be needed. Researchers may find further information of students’ internalized sight word recognition after extended school breaks such as winter or summer break. An additional limitation was the study’s sample size. The experiment took place in a Title 1 reading setting with pullout services.
Recommended publications
  • Fry 1000 Instant Words: Free Flash Cards and Word Lists for Teachers
    Fry 1000 Instant Words: Free Flash Cards and Word Lists For Teachers Fry 1000 Instant Words Bulletin Board Display Banner and 26 Letter Cards The Fry 1000 Instant Words are a list of the most common words used for teaching reading, writing, and spelling. These high frequency words should be recognized instantly by readers. Dr. Edward B. Fry's Instant Words (which are often referred to as the "Fry Words") are the most common words used in English ranked in order of frequency. In 1996, Dr. Fry expanded on Dolch's sight word lists and research and published a book titled "Fry 1000 Instant Words." In his research, Dr. Fry found the following results: 25 words make up approximately 1/3 of all items published. 100 words comprise approximately 1/2 of all of the words found in publications. 300 words make up approximately 65% of all written material. Over half of every newspaper article, textbook, children's story, and novel is composed of these 300 words. It is difficult to write a sentence without using several of the first 300 words in the Fry 1000 Instant Words List. Consequently, students need to be able to read the first 300 Instant Words without a moment's hesitation. Do not bother copying these 3 lists. You will be able to download free copies of these lists, plus 7 additional lists that are not shown (words 301 - 1000), using the free download links that are found later on this page. In addition to these 10 free lists of Fry's sight words, I have created 1000 color coded flashcards for all of the Fry 1000 Instant Words.
    [Show full text]
  • Sight Word Poems Easy-To-Read Reproducible Poems That Target & Teach 100 Words from the Dolch List by Rosalie Franzese
    10 0 Super Sight Word Poems Easy-to-Read Reproducible Poems That Target & Teach 100 Words From the Dolch List by Rosalie Franzese Edited by Eileen Judge Cover design by Maria Lilja Interior design by Brian LaRossa ISBN: 978-0-545-23830-4 Copyright © 2012 by Rosalie Franzese All rights reserved. Published by Scholastic Inc. Printed in the U.S.A. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 40 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 100 Super Sight Word Poems © Rosalie Franzese, Scholastic Teaching Resources Introduction .................... 4 Our Class (we) ................. 32 Teaching Strategies .............. 5 Where Is My Teacher? (she) ...... 33 Activities ....................... 8 Love, Love, Love (me) ........... 34 Meeting the Common Core What Can I Be? (be) ............ 35 State Standards .................11 Look in the Sky (look). 36 References .....................11 The Library (at) ................ 37 Dolch Word List ................ 12 Look at That! (that) ............. 38 I Ran (ran) .................... 39 POEMS In the Fall (all) ................. 40 A Park (a) ..................... 13 You and Me (you) .............. 41 Me (I) ........................ 14 Do You? (do) .................. 42 The School (the) ............... 15 Setting the Table (here) ......... 43 I Go (go) ...................... 16 You Are My Puppy (are) ......... 44 Where To? (to) ................. 17 In My Room (there) ............. 45 I See the Animals (see) .......... 18 Where, Oh, Where? (where) ...... 46 My Room (my) ................. 19 Going, Going, Going (going) ...... 47 Feelings (am) .................. 20 What Is It For? (for) .............48 I Go In (in) .................... 21 What Is It Good For? (good) ...... 49 Here I Go! (on). 22 Come With Me (come) .......... 50 My Family (is) .................. 23 My Halloween Party (came) ...... 51 What Is It? (it) .................
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Sight Word Instruction on the Reading Achievement of Second Grade Students Mary Riscigno Submitted in Partial Fulf
    The Effect of Sight Word Instruction on the Reading Achievement of Second Grade Students Mary Riscigno Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Education May 2019 Graduate Programs in Education Goucher College Table of Contents List of Tables i Abstract ii I. Introduction 1 Statement of the Problem 1 Statement of Research Hypothesis 2 Operational Definitions 2 II. Review of the Literature 3 Early Literacy Development 3 Importance of Foundational Skills in Reading Instruction 5 Sight Word Instruction 7 III. Methods 10 Design 10 Participants 10 Instruments 11 Procedure 11 IV. Results 13 V. Discussion 16 References 20 List of Tables 1. t-test for Difference in Sample Mean Pretest Scores 13 2. t-test for Difference in Sample Mean Posttest Scores 14 3. t-test for Difference in Sample Mean Pre-to-Post Gains 15 i Abstract The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of sight word instruction on reading fluency for second grade students. The participants in this study were second grade students enrolled in a Baltimore County public school during the 2018-2019 school year. The students were randomly divided into two groups. The treatment group received small group guided instruction with a focus on sight word fluency four days a week for four weeks in addition to traditional whole group reading lessons. The control group received regular small group guided reading instruction and traditional whole group reading lessons. The results of the study indicated that both groups increased their reading levels, however, the hypothesis that sight word instruction would increase reading achievement was not supported when looking at the data.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is a Lexile? Decatur County School System for Parents 2015-2016 Modified for WBE PTO Mtg
    What is a Lexile? Decatur County School System For Parents 2015-2016 Modified for WBE PTO Mtg. (Jan. 2016) • A Lexile is a standard score that matches a student’s reading ability with difficulty of text material. • It is a measure of text complexity only. It does not address age-appropriateness of the content, or a reader’s interests. • A Lexile can be interpreted as the level of book that a student can read with 75% comprehension, offering the reader a certain amount of comfort and yet still offering a challenge. What is a Lexile? • K, 1st and 2nd Grade • Istation Reports provide Lexile scores • 3rd and 4th Grade: • Georgia Milestones Parent Reports provide Lexile scores Where do I find my student’s Lexile? • Lexile Targets by grade level are shown below. • College and Career Readiness – Goals for grade level CCRPI Grade Target 3 650 5 850 8 1050 11 1275 What does my student’s Lexile score tell me about his or her reading ability compared to other students in Georgia? • To calculate your student’s Lexile range, add 50 to the student’s reported Lexile score and subtract 100 (Example: 200 L = Range of 100-250L) • The range represents the boundaries between the easiest kind of reading material for your student and the hardest level at which he/she can read successfully. • Consider your child’s interest in topics and the age-appropriateness of the book’s content. Now that I know my student’s Lexile score, what do I do with it? A Quick Walk-Thru: Locating Books within Lexile Ranges @ https://lexile.com/ Enter Lexile Score How to Read a Book
    [Show full text]
  • Reading, Spelling, and Language: Why There Is No Such Thing As a “Sight” Word
    7/26/2018 Reading, Spelling, and Language: Why There is No Such Thing as a “Sight” Word DIBELS Super Summit July 9, 2018 Louisa Moats, Ed.D. 1 Topics For This Talk • Prevalence of beliefs about reading and spelling as “visual memory” activities • Common indicators of these beliefs in our classrooms • Review of evidence that orthographic memory relies on language processes and is only incidentally visual • Characteristics of instruction that are aligned with a linguistic theory of reading and spelling 2 Izzy, 2nd Grade Reading, 16th %ile Spelling, 12th %ile Vocabulary, 98th %ile 3 Louisa Moats, Ed.D., 2018 1 7/26/2018 How can teachers help Izzy? 4 Classroom Practices Reflecting the Belief That Reading is Primarily “Visual” • Vision therapy or colored overlays are often recommended when kids can’t read • High frequency words are treated as “sight words,” learned by rote repetition – 100 flash card words required in K – Texts written with high frequency words rather than with pattern-based words – Spelling taught by visual memory (write the word 10 times…) 5 Have You Seen This Conceptual Model of Word Recognition? Graphophonic/ Visual Semantic Syntactic “The Three Cueing Systems” 6 Louisa Moats, Ed.D., 2018 2 7/26/2018 How Reading and Spelling are Treated as “Visual” Skills • “Visual” cueing errors, along with meaning and structure errors, are a category in scoring Running Records • In the cueing systems model, the word “visual” is used interchangeably with “graphophonic” • Phonology has no role in the model 7 Everyday Practice: The Alphabetic
    [Show full text]
  • Teaching Sight Words According to Science ODE Literacy Academy 2019 Why Are We D O in G T H Is ? Alig N Men T W It H Ohio’S Plan T O Raise Literacy Achievement
    Teaching Sight Words According to Science ODE Literacy Academy 2019 Why are we d o in g t h is ? Alig n men t w it h Ohio’s Plan t o Raise Literacy Achievement 3 Why are we here? Ac c o r d in g t o t h e 2 0 17 Na t io n a l Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) scores, 37% of our nation’s fou rth - grade students were proficient readers. Why are we here? ● Nearly 30 percent of Ohio’s K- 3 students are reading below grade level. ● Nearly 40 percent of students in grades 3- 8 are not proficient on the OST in ELA. ● More than 50 percent of graduating seniors taking the ACT do not meet the college and career readiness benchmark for reading. Today’s Outcomes P a r t ic ip a n t s w ill… ● Ap p ly t h e t h e o r e t ic a l m o d e ls o f t h e S im p le Vie w o f Re a d in g and Seidenburg’s 4 Part Processing System for Word Recognition to instructional practices to teach words “by s ig h t .” ● Understand the connection between phonology and orthography when storing words for automatic retrieval. ● Demonstrate instructional strategies. Word Language Reading Recognition X Comprehension = Comprehension Phonological Background Awareness Knowledge Decoding (Phonics, Vocabulary Advanced Phonics) Language Sight Word Based on the Simple View of Recognition Verbal Reasoning Reading by Gough and Tunmer, 1986 Th e Simple View of Read in g 7 Wh a t is a Sig h t Wo r d ? A sight word is any word that is recognized in s t a n t ly a n d e ffo r t le s s ly , b y s ig h t , w h e t h e r it is s p e lle d r e g u la r ly o r ir r e g u la r ly BeAr bear bEaR bear Bear bear bear bEa r Sig h t w or d vocabulary is NOT based on visu al memory / visu al sk ills! - Dr .
    [Show full text]
  • Expanding Vocabulary and Sight Word Growth Through Guided Play in a Pre-Primary Classroom
    South African Journal of Childhood Education ISSN: (Online) 2223-7682, (Print) 2223-7674 Page 1 of 9 Review Article Expanding vocabulary and sight word growth through guided play in a pre-primary classroom Authors: Background: This article is based on a study that aimed at finding out how pre-primary 1 Annaly M. Strauss teachers integrate directed play into literacy teaching and learning. Play is a platform through Keshni Bipath2 which young children acquire language. Affiliations: Aim: This study uses an action research approach to understand how guided play benefits 1Faculty of Education, International University of incidental reading and expands vocabulary growth in a Chinese Grade K classroom. Management, Windhoek, Namibia Method: Data collection involved classroom observations, document analysis, informal and focus group discussions. 2Department of Early Childhood Development, Results: The results revealed the key benefits of play-based learning for sight word or University of Pretoria, incidental reading and vocabulary development. These are: (1) teacher oral and written Pretoria, South Africa language learning, (2) learners’ classroom engagement is promoted, (3) learners were actively engaged in learning of orthographic features of words, (4) learners practised recognising the Corresponding author: Annaly Strauss, visual or grapho-phonemic structure of words, (5) teacher paced teaching and (6) teacher [email protected] assesses miscues and (7) keep record of word recognition skills. Dates: Conclusion: In the light of the evidence, it is recommended that the English Second Language Received: 16 Feb. 2019 (ESL) curriculum for pre-service teachers integrate curricular objectives that promote practising Accepted: 24 July 2020 playful learning strategies to prepare teachers for practice.
    [Show full text]
  • GCS Read-At-Home Plan
    Family Read-At-Home Plan Parents, You are your child’s first teacher and reading with your child is a proven way to promote early literacy. Helping to make sure your child is reading on grade level is one of the most important things you can do to prepare him/her for the future. By reading with your child for 20 minutes per day and making a few simple strategies a part of your daily routine, you can make a positive impact on your child’s success in school. Five Areas of Reading Phonemic Awareness Phonics Phonemic awareness is the ability to hear and Phonics is the ability to understand the relationship distinguish sounds. between letters and the sounds they represent. This includes: This includes: -Recognizing sounds, alone and in words -Recognizing print patterns that represent sounds -Adding sounds to words -Syllable patterns -Taking apart words and breaking them into their -Word parts (prefixes, suffixes, and root words) different sounds -Moving sounds Common Consonant Digraphs and Blends: bl, br, ch, ck, cl, cr, dr, fl, fr, gh, gl, gr, ng, ph, pl, pr, qu, sc, sh, sk, sl, sm, sn, sp, st, sw, th, tr, tw, wh, wr Common Consonant Trigraphs: nth, sch, scr, shr, spl, spr, squ, str, thr Common Vowel Digraphs: ai, au, aw, ay, ea, ee, ei, eu, ew, ey, ie, oi, oo, ou, ow, oy Fluency Comprehension Fluency is the ability to read with sufficient speed to Comprehension is the ability to understand and draw meaning support understanding. from text. This includes: This includes: -Automatic word recognition -Paying attention to important information -Accurate word recognition -Interpreting specific meanings in text -Use of expression -Identifying the main idea -Verbal responses to questions -Application of new information gained through reading Vocabulary Vocabulary is students’ knowledge of and memory for word meanings.
    [Show full text]
  • Vocabulary Builder Guide
    Vocabulary Builder Guide Vocabulary Builder Guide Acknowledgement Marianne Cameron, Software Engineer Tracy Custer, M. Ed., SLP, Regional Consultant Cindy Halloran, OTR/L, Director of Center for AAC and Autism John Halloran, M.S., CCC-SLP, Senior Clinical Associate Jennifer Herzog, B.S., Regional Consultant Tim Hurn, Technical Service Rep Janet Lehr, M.S., CCC-SLP, Regional Consultant Verda McGraw, M. Ed., MAPs Manager Sarah Wilds, M.S., CCC-SLP, Regional Consultant PRC prentrom.com Page 1 Vocabulary Builder Guide 15252 V1.0 Vantage, Vanguard, Language Activity Monitor (LAM), Memory Transfer Interface (MTI) and Picture Prediction are trademarks of Prentke Romich Company (PRC). Minspeak and Unity are registered trademarks of Semantic Compaction Systems, Inc. in the United States and in other countries. Semantic Compaction is a trademark of Semantic Compaction Systems, Inc. PCS Symbols are a product of Mayer-Johnson. Fonix-DECtalk Text-to-Speech software is a copyright of Fonix Corporation; all rights reserved. RealSpeak Text-to-Speech is a trademark of Nuance Communications, Inc. Acapela speech technologies licensed from the Acapella Group. April 2011 PRC · 1022 Heyl Rd · Wooster, Ohio 44691 Telephone: (800) 262-1984 Fax: (330) 263-4829 E-Mail Address: [email protected] Web Site Address: prentrom.com PRC prentrom.com Page 2 Vocabulary Builder Guide 15252 V1.0 VOCABULARY BUILDER GUIDE Table of Contents Section 1: INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................ 5 Section 2: UNITY
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Educational Games on Sight Word Reading Achievement and Student Motivation
    Journal of Language and Literacy Education Vol. 13 Issue 2—Fall 2017 Effects of Educational Games on Sight Word Reading Achievement and Student Motivation Justine M. Gibbon, Stacy Duffield, Jeanette Hoffman, & Justin J. Wageman Abstract: Nine first-grade children at risk for reading failure were selected to participate in remedial reading interventions. These first-grade students scored below benchmark target and grade level expectancies on measures of early reading skills, including AIMSweb Nonsense Word Fluency and Northwest Evaluation Association Measures of Academic Progress (NWEA MAP ) for Primary Reading, or received a teacher referral based on oral reading fluency scores. Interventions occurred four times a week for 11 weeks, and included 10 minutes of direct phonemic awareness instruction and& 10 minutes of sight word games. Data were collected on students’ oral reading fluency, sight word identification, and motivation to read. Students were found to be more engaged and voiced a preference for sight word card and board games over the tablet apps. Results indicated that games as interventions can accelerate sight word learning and are highly effective for sight word achievement in first-grade students when combined with direct instruction on phonemic awareness. The participants improved at a rate double that of previously studied, remedial first-grade students. Results also indicated that intervention games in remedial reading programs have a direct impact on student engagement, but not necessarily on a student’s self-concept as a reader. Keywords: at risk, beginning reader, sight words, sight word games, motivation Justine M. Gibbon is a Title 1 Reading Teacher at Kindred, North Dakota.
    [Show full text]
  • Put Reading First 2006
    Put Reading First Kindergarten Through Grade 3 The Research Building Blocks For Teaching Children to Read ThirdThird Edition The Research Building Blocks for Teaching Children to Read Put Reading First Kindergarten Through Grade 3 Writers: Bonnie B. Armbruster, Ph.D., University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Fran Lehr, M.A., Lehr & Associates, Champaign, Illinois, Jean Osborn, M.Ed., University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Editor: C. Ralph Adler, RMC Research Corporation Designer: Lisa T. Noonis, RMC Research Corporation Contents i Introduction 1 Phonemic Awareness Instruction 11 Phonics Instruction 19 Fluency Instruction 29 Vocabulary Instruction 41 Text Comprehension Instruction This publication was developed by the Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement (CIERA) and was funded by the National Institute for Literacy (NIFL) through the Educational Research and Development Centers Program, PR/Award Number R305R70004, as administered by the Office of Educational Research and Improvement (OERI), U.S. Department of Education. However, the comments or conclusions do not necessarily represent the positions or policies of NIFL, OERI, or the U.S. Department of Education, and you should not assume endorsement by the Federal Government. The National Institute for Literacy The National Institute for Literacy, an agency in the Federal government, is authorized to help strengthen literacy across the lifespan. The Institute works to provide national leadership on literacy issues, including the improvement of reading instruction for children, youth, and adults by sharing information on scientifically based research. Sandra Baxter, Director Lynn Reddy, Deputy Director The Partnership for Reading This document was published by The Partnership for Reading, a collaborative effort of the National Institute for Literacy, the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and the U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Long-Term Effects of Synthetic Versus Analytic Phonics Teaching on the Reading and Spelling Ability of 10 Year Old Boys and Girls
    Long-term effects of synthetic versus analytic phonics teaching on the reading and spelling ability of 10 year old boys and girls Rhona S. Johnston, Sarah McGeown, Joyce E. Watson Abstract A comparison was made of 10-year-old boys and girls who had learnt to read by analytic or synthetic phonics methods as part of their early literacy programmes. The boys taught by the synthetic phonics method had better word reading than the girls in their classes, and their spelling and reading comprehension was as good. In contrast, with analytic phonics teaching, although the boys performed as well as the girls in word reading, they had inferior spelling and reading comprehension. Overall, the group taught by synthetic phonics had better word reading, spelling, and reading comprehension. There was no evidence that the synthetic phonics approach, which early on teaches children to blend letter sounds in order to read unfamiliar words, led to any impairment in the reading of irregular words. Keywords Synthetic phonicsAnalytic phonicsOpaque orthography The English spelling system has an opaque orthography; although it is an alphabetic system, some spellings have inconsistent grapheme- phoneme connections, e.g., ‘aisle’. This inconsistency in English spelling has led to models of adult reading such as the dual route model, where the pathways envisaged for the reading of words with irregular versus regular spelling-sound correspondences are seen as largely independent (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001). A substantial literature has examined whether individuals take a phonological approach to reading English, determined by whether their responses to irregular words are slower and less accurate than to regular words; it has been found that these effects are shown in both children and adults (e.g., Waters, Seidenberg, & Bruck, 1984).
    [Show full text]