Type 2 Rising A Contribution to a DG Account of Discontinuities

Timothy Osborne [email protected]

that crossing lines obtain in the tree. The tree in Abstract (1b) shows the manner in which the crossing lines are “remedied”. The displaced unit takes This contribution examines discontinuities in on a word as its head that is not its governor. DG. Discontinuities are addressed in terms of The tree conventions shown in (1b) follow catenae and rising. The catena is defined as A Groß and Osborne (2009). The dashed depen- WORD OR A COMBINATION OF WORDS THAT IS dency edge indicates the presence of rising by CONTINUOUS WITH RESPECT TO DOMINANCE. The definition identifies any tree or subtree of which the discontinuity is overcome; the under- a tree as a catena. Rising occurs when a gov- line marks the displaced unit; the g subscript ernor fails to dominate one (or more) of its marks the governor of the displaced unit; and the governees. Two sorts of rising are distin- italics mark the chain (=catena) of words the end guished: type 1 and type 2. Type 1 rising ob- points of which are the displaced unit and the tains when the risen catena is a constituent, governor of the displaced unit. These conven- whereas type 2 rising obtains when the risen tions will become clear as the discussion contin- catena is a non-constituent. The Rising Prin- ues. ciple expresses the main trait of instances of The flattening of structure illustrated in (1b) rising. Discontinuity sorts (e.g. wh-fronting, represents a widespread approach to discontinui- , , , NP- internal displacement) are classified in terms ties in DGs, although the terminology certainly of type 1 and type 2 rising. varies: Hudson (2000:32) employs the term “raising” to address such constellations; Duchier 1 Introduction and Debusmann (2001) use the term “climbing”; Gerdes and Kahane (2001) assume “emancipa- Many dependency grammars (DGs) address dis- tion”; Bröker (2003:294) posits “lifting”. Eroms continuities in terms of a flattening of structure. and Heringer (2003:26) suggest movement and A displaced unit takes on a word as its head that “adjunction”; and Groß and Osborne (2009) po- is not its governor. Example (1a) illustrates a sit “rising”. This contribution follows the termi- standard wh-discontinuity and example (1b) nology of the latter. Discontinuities are ad- shows the manner in which the discontinuity is dressed in terms of rising. While the accounts of “overcome”: these linguists certainly vary, the underlying (1) does idea pursued is consistent. This idea is that a flattening of structure occurs in order to over- constellation show come projectivity violations. What this While there seems to be a measure of a. What does this constellation show? agreement concerning the manner in which DGs should address discontinuities like the one does shown in (1), there are other structures involving What constellation showg discontinuities that pose major challenges and this for which there seems to be much less consensus about the correct analysis. Consider, for in- b. What does this constellation show? stance, the structural analysis of the following Tree (1a) illustrates a typical projectivity viola- example involving a relative clause: tion (=discontinuity). The fronted wh-element is separated from its governor in such a manner

38 (2) structures be controversial for DG theory, this contribution the are focuses more on it. The data examined are most- ly from English and German. that we examiningg a. the structures that we are examining 2 Catenae The arrow dependency edge identifies an ad- Before exploring the distinction between type 1 junct (as opposed to an ). While the and type 2 rising, the fundamental unit of syn- tree conventions shown again follow Groß and tactic analysis assumed in the current DG must Osborne (2009), the actual hierarchy of words be established. Following O‟Grady (1998), Os- assumed is similar to proposal by Kunze borne (2005), and Osborne et al. (in press), the (1975:160); the finite verb is seen as the root of catena (Latin for „chain‟, plural catenae) is po- the relative clause (not the relative pronoun).1 sited as the fundamental unit of syntactic analy- The difficulty with the analysis in (2a) is sis.2 The catena is defined as A WORD OR A that there are indications that the relative pro- COMBINATION OF WORDS THAT IS CONTINUOUS noun should be the root of the relative clause, WITH RESPECT TO DOMINANCE. This definition not the finite verb. In German for instance, the identifies any dependency tree or any subtree of presence of a relative pronoun evokes VF (=verb a dependency tree as a catena. final) order just like subordinators do. Since The catena unit is illustrated using the fol- subordinators are unanimously viewed as the lowing abstract structure: root of the clause they introduce, the inference is (3) B that relative pronouns should also be the roots of A D the clauses that they introduce. This insight mo- C E tivates the following structural analysis of (2): A B C D E (2) structures the that The capital letters represent words. The follow- are ing 17 combinations qualify as catenae: A, B, C, D, E, AB, BD, CD, DE, ABD, BCD, BDE, we examining g CDE, ABCD, ABDE, BCDE, and ABCDE. The b. the structures that we are examining following 14 combinations, in contrast, qualify as non-catenae: AC, AD, AE, BC, BE, CE, The relative pronoun is now the root of the rela- ABC, ABE, ACD, ACE, ADE, BCE, ABCE, tive clause. The major difference between (2a) and ACDE. As the number of words increases, and (2b) is that the displaced unit, i.e. that, in the percentage of non-catena combinations in- (2a) is a constituent (=a complete subtree), whe- creases. reas it alone is a non-constituent in (2b) (because Given a theory neutral definition of the con- it dominates other words). stituent (=A WORD/NODE PLUS ALL THE This contribution argues that the analysis in WORDS/NODES THAT THAT WORD/NODE DOMI- (2b) should be preferred over the analysis in NATES), there are five constituents in (3): A, C, (2a). This situation necessitates that the theory E, CDE, and ABCDE. Examining the combina- distinguish between two types of discontinuities. tions that qualify as catenae and that qualify as Discontinuities like the one in (1b) are instances constituents, one sees that every constituent is a of type 1 rising, whereas discontinuities like the catena, but many catenae are not constituents. one in (2b) are instances of type 2 rising. The Thus THE CONSTITUENT IS A SUBTYPE OF THE defining trait of type 1 rising is that the risen CATENA. unit is a constituent (=a complete subtree), whe- reas the risen unit of type 2 rising is a non- 2 O‟Grady (1998), Osborne (2005), and Groß and Osborne constituent. Since type 2 rising is more likely to (2009) employed the term “chain” (instead of “catena”). Osborne et al. (in press), however, replace the term “chain” 1 The term “root” is used throughout to denote the one with “catena” in order to avoid confusion coming from word in a given unit (e.g. constituent, catena) that is not constituency-based derivational grammars, where “chain” dominated by any other word in that unit. has a much different meaning.

39 The detailed discussions of the catena unit in (6) Kann the sources cited at the beginning of this section uns jemand helfeng establish the validity and importance of the con- cept. The discussion below can therefore take Kann uns jemand helfen? the concept for granted and base its account of can us someone help discontinuities thereupon. „Can someone help us?‟ (7) dass 3 Type 1 rising hat

Type 1 rising occurs when the risen catena is a uns das überraschtg constituent. A number of discontinuity types involve Type 1 rising (e.g. wh-fronting in Eng- dass uns das überrascht hat lish and German, scrambling in German, and that us that surprised has extraposition in English and German). This sec- „that that surprised us‟ tion briefly illustrates these discontinuity types (8) dass and in so doing, establishes the particular termi- versuchen nology of the current DG theory of discontinui- ties. The tree conventions introduced with tree wir das zu versteheng (1b) are again employed. dass wir das versuchen zu verstehen Example (1b) illustrated wh-fronting in that we that try to understand English. The next two examples illustrate w- „that we tried to understand that‟ fronting rising and topicalization rising in Ger- man: And the following examples illustrate extraposi- tion rising: (4) habt (9) has Ideen ihr gefundeng attempt occurred to Wessen gut g An avoid Wessen Ideen habt ihr gut gefunden? confusion whose ideas have you good found „Whose ideas did you find good?‟ An attempt has occurred to avoid confusion. (5) muss (10)dass

verstehen man könneng hat

Idee sie gesagtg dass Die komme Die Idee verstehen muss man können. sie the idea understand must one can dass sie gesagt hat, dass sie komme „One has to be able to understand the idea.‟ that she said has that she comes As mentioned above, the dashed dependency „that she said that she will come‟ edge indicates the presence of rising, the under- These instances of rising all show the major trait lined unit is the risen catena, the g subscript of type 1 rising. This trait is that the risen catena marks the governor of the risen catena, and the is a constituent (as defined above). While there italicized words constitute what is now called are many aspects of these discontinuity types the rising catena. that deserve attention, the main point that is per- The following examples illustrate scram- tinent for the account of type 2 rising below has bling rising in German (Scrambling does not now been established. This point is that the risen exist in English, of course): catena of type 1 rising is a constituent.

40 4 Type 2 rising and the Rising Principle (15) was happy boy The instance of type 2 rising illustrated with (2b) shows the risen catena as a non-constituent. This How (of) the aspect of type 2 rising allows one to easily dis- childg tinguish the two types of rising. Any instance of a rising where the risen catena is a non-constituent is type 2 rising. Type 2 rising occurs with How happy (of) a child was the boy? wh-fronting in subordinate clauses (in indirect (16) has questions), with relative pronouns of all types, She big and with NP-internal displacement. The following trees illustrate type 2 rising in too (of) indirect questions: mouthg (11) asked a They what She has too big (of) a mouth. wanted These two examples show type 2 rising within we to an NP. The parentheses indicate that the appear- ance of the preposition of in each case is option- claimg They asked what we wanted to claim. al. The risen adjective is focused by the adverb, i.e. by how and too. When the adjective is fo- (12) wissen cused in this manner, it must be fronted within Wir was the NP. Interestingly, this sort of type 2 rising is istg completely absent from German. The pertinent worden observation in this regard that there are numer- gesagt ous discontinuity types, and languages vary with Wir wissen was gesagt worden ist respect to the inventory of discontinuities that We know what said become is they allow. „Whe know what has been said.‟ The tree conventions in these instances of type 2 rising remain consistent. The risen catena The following two examples illustrate type 2 in each case is underlined; the governor of the rising in relative clauses: risen catena carries the g subscript, and the ris- (13) place ing catena is in italics. Two things should be the where acknowledged about type 2 rising: again that the risen catena is a non-constituent and that the root like of the risen catena necessarily dominates its we to governor.

sleepg Comparing the instances of type 1 rising in (4-10) with the instances of type 2 rising in the place where we like to sleep (11-16), one sees that in cases of type 1 rising, (14) Menschen the head of the risen catena dominates the gov- 3 die denen ernor of the risen catena, whereas with type 2 rising, the root of the risen catena itself domi- helfeng nates the governor of that risen catena. These wir gerne two observations exhaust the possibilities, and die Menschen, denen wir gerne helfen they motivate the Rising Principle: the people who we gladly help „the people who we like to help‟

And the following two examples illustrate type 2 3 rising inside NPs in English: The head of a given catena is the one word (outside of that catena) that dominates that catena.

41 Rising Principle If type 1 rising were the only type of rising that The head or the root of the risen catena the theory had at its disposal, accommodating must dominate the governor of the risen the contrast in a principled manner would be catena. difficult. The SV order of indirect questions is also The examples above all obey this principle. Ei- seen in relative clauses of all sorts. This fact ther the head of the risen catena dominates the supports the type 2 rising analysis of these governor of the risen catena (=type 1 rising) or clauses. the root of the risen catena dominates the gover- nor of the risen catena (=type 2 rising). The Ris- (18) claims ing Principle is the major guideline that all dis- the that continuities must obey. It helps limit the discon- have tinuities that can obtain. Since many DGs address discontinuities in they been terms of a mechanism like type 1 rising, type 1 denyingg rising should not be too controversial. Type 2 the claims that they have been denying rising, however, is unique to the current DG. To S V my knowledge, no other DG has proposed some- (19) left thing similar. Furthermore, there are some as- pects of type 2 rising that generate questions He which about the nature of discontinuities and head- didn’t dependent relations in general. For these rea- she dog sons, the following subsections motivate the cur- rent understanding of type 2 rising. He left, which she didn’t do. S V 3.1 SV order The same SV order seen in the indirect questions The first observation that supports type 2 rising is present in relative clauses like these. The comes from word order across direct and indi- combination SV-order plus pronoun fronting is rect clauses in English. Direct constituent ques- thus an indication of type 2 rising. tions in English can have VS order, where V is Beyond the VS vs. SV distinction, subcate- an auxiliary. In indirect questions in contrast, SV gorization considerations support type 2 rising. order obtains. These facts are illustrated with the Question verbs (e.g. ask, wonder, inquire, know, following examples: etc.) subcategorize for an indirect question, whe- reby the question word is the most distinctive (17) will trait of a question (direct or indirect). And re-

What you thinkg garding relative clauses, the relative pronoun is the most distinctive word of a relative clause, so a. What will you think? it makes sense that it should be the root of the V S relative clause. asked She what 3.2 VF order will VF order in German subordinate clauses pro- you think g vides similar support for type 2 rising. Type 2 b. She asked what you will think? rising in many subordinate clauses in German S V provides a principled means of accounting for The direct question in (17a) has VS order, where VF (=verb final) order. An initial observation in V is an auxiliary verb. The indirect question this regard is that the appearance of a typical (17b), in contrast, has SV order. Both sentences subordinator evokes VF order, e.g. necessarily involve a discontinuity. By assuming the distinction between type 1 and type 2 rising, the VS vs. SV distinction can be accommodated.

42 (20) dass posal, there would be no way to establish the kann desired parallelism across all VF clauses. The same observation speaks for type 2 ris- das geschehen ing in relative clauses in German. The appear- dass das geschehen kann ance of the relative pronoun evokes VF order, VF which means that the relative proform should that that happen can appear in a position where it can have this im- „that that can happen‟ pact on the clause it introduces, e.g.4 (21) bevor (24) Grund sind der weswegen wir angekommen ist

bevor wir angekommen sind das gescheheng VF der Grund, weswegen das geschehen ist before we arrived are VF „before we arrived‟ the reason why that happened is The subordinators dass „that‟ and bevor „before‟ „the reason why that happened‟ evoke VF order, which means the finite verb (25) Leute appears in clause-final position. Since this word die die order occurs with indirect questions and relative clauses as well, one can assume that such subor- habeng dinate clauses should have a similar structure. verloren But only if type 2 rising is allowed can the Geld structure of all VF clauses be parallel. Examine viel the parallelism of structure across the following clauses: die Leute die viel Geld verloren haben VF (22) wenn the folks that much money lost have geht „the folks that lost a lot of money‟ er The relative proforms weswegen „why‟ and die (Ich bleibe nicht,) wenn er geht. „that/who‟ evoke VF order. They should there- I stay not if he goes fore appear in a position where they can exert „I won‟t stay if he goes.‟ this influence. Assuming type 2 rising allows (23) wann them to serve as the root of the clause that they gehtg introduce. er As mentioned above, distributional consi- derations provide a second source of support for (Ich weiß nicht,) wann er geht. type 2 rising in indirect questions and relative I know not when he goes clauses in German. The defining trait of these „I don‟t know when he is going.‟ clauses is the wh-element or relative proform. The closeness in form and meaning across the Since the presence of these elements influences two clauses suggests strongly that they should greatly the distribution of the clauses in which have similar structures. The subordinator wenn they appear, granting them root status in the „when/if‟ and the interrogative proform wann clause is appropriate. „when‟ convey similar meanings and they both evoke VF order. Type 2 rising allows for the parallelism to be acknowledged in the structure. 4 The dependency arrow connecting weswegen to ist indi- If type 1 rising were all the theory had at its dis- cates that weswegen is an adjunct. Since the arrow always points away from the adjunct towards the governor of the adjunct, the arrow points downwards in this case.

43 3.3 Constituency-based hierarchies now proposed assumes that wh-features and relative proform features can in a sense percolate A third observation supporting type 2 rising is of up a catena to a higher node. a much different nature (from the previous two Interrogative verbs subcategorize for a word observations). This third observation concerns with a wh-feature. When pied-piping occurs, this the analysis of indirect interrogative clauses and feature has percolated upward to the root of the relative clauses in constituency grammars (as pied-piped catena, e.g. opposed to in DGs). Constituency grammars of the GB/MP tradition see the wh-element or rela- (27) asked tive proform occupying the head position of the He bicycle[wh] clause, e.g. the C position of CP. The type 2 ris- which [wh] like ing analysis therefore mirrors this analysis of the g GB/MP tradition. you The point is illustrated with the following He asked which bicycle you like. GB analysis of a simple indirect question. The wh-feature associated with which percolates (26) CP to the root node of the risen catena. How exactly spec C this percolation should be conceived of is not C IP clear at this point, but that some sort of percola- tion mechanism is necessary is apparent. In fact Spec I regardless of the particular approach at hand, I VP this passing of information up the structure is Spec V needed for pied-piping in general, in matrix as V … well as in embedded clauses. Two more examples involving relative pro- (She asked) what3 he1 said2 t1 t2 t3 nouns further illustrate the mechanism: The details of this analysis (e.g. the traces) are (28) event not important for the matter at hand. What is the weight[rel] important is the surface hierarchy shown. The wh-element what occupies C, whereby CP, the the of isg maximal projection of C, is the root node of the which[rel] unclear entire structure. This constituency-based analy- the event the weight of which is unclear sis is therefore analogous to the DG type 2 rising analysis now under consideration. (29) Arbeiter The type 2 rising analysis therefore opens die mit[rel] the door to the massive body of literature on denen[rel] wird subordinate clauses in constituency-based sys- tems. Many of the insights gained by the consti- verhandeltg tuency-based systems are now applicable to de- die Arbeiter, mit denen verhandelt wird pendency-based systems (that assume type 2 the workers with whom negotiated are rising). A bridge of sorts now spans the two tra- „the workers with whom one is negotiating‟ ditions (at least in this one area). In these cases, the feature [rel] (indicating the 3.4 Pied-piping presence of a relative pronoun) must percolate to Pied-piping in subordinate clauses presents a the root node of the clause. By doing so, this difficulty for the current analysis in terms of feature is in a position to elicit the obligatory SV type 2 rising. The seriousness of this difficulty order of English or VF order of German asso- should not, however, be overestimated, since ciated with relative clauses. pied-piping challenges most analyses regardless Worth emphasizing again is that no matter of whether (something like) type 2 rising is as- the approach, some sort of percolation mechan- sumed or not. The analysis of pied-piping that is ism is needed to accommodate pied-piping. This

44 necessity is consistent across matrix and embed- the embedded clause to a hierarchical position ded clauses. that allows the subcategorization requirement of the matrix predicate to be satisfied. 4 Rising in other languages The following example contains a standard relative clause: The discussion so far has focused on type 2 ris- ing in English and German. The question arises (31) client as to whether type 2 rising exists in other lan- le que guages. I believe that it does. Any time a wh- reconnaissez word or relative proform (or the encompassing g pied-piped expression) must introduce a clause, vous- one can make a case for type 2 rising. The dis- a. le client que vous reconnaissez cussion now briefly considers examples from the client that you recognize French and Russian. These languages also exhi- bit type 2 rising. b. *le client vous reconnaissez que. The following example from French illu- As in English and German, the relative pronoun strates type 2 rising in an embedded interroga- must undergo type 2 rising. If it does not (i.e. it tive clause: remains in situ), the result is clearly unaccepta- ble, as example (31b) shows. Based on such da- (30) veux ta, one can conclude that type 2 rising is occur- Je- savoir ring consistently in the same environments où across English, German, and French (and cer- tainly across many other languages as well). est The following example provided by an il allég anonymous reviewer illustrates an embedded a. Je- veux savoir où il est allé. interrogative in Russian: I want to.know where he is gone (30) Skazhi emu, „I want to know where he went.‟ tell him b. *Je veux savoir il est allé où ? kakuju c. *Je veux savoir où est-il allé. vzjala The lack of vertical projection edge but presence studentka knigug iz of a hyphen on Je- identifies Je- as a . This biblioteki aspect of the tree is not pertinent to the point at kakuju vzjala studentka knigu iz biblioteki hand and is therefore taken for granted. -ACC -NOM -ACC The question word où is fronted in the which lent student book from library relative clause in (30a). When this fronting fails to occur, the result is bad, as illustrated in (30b). „Tell him which book the student checked out of And sentence (30c) demonstrates that fronting is the library.‟ incompatible with subject-auxiliary inversion of The interrogative element kakuju „which‟ must the sort that one encounters in matrix questions be fronted within the embedded interrogative in French. These data can be accommodated if clause, a fact that is consistent with an analysis type 2 rising is seen as obligatory in embedded in terms of type 2 rising. interrogative clauses in French (just like it is in The interesting aspect of this example is such clauses in English and German). Note as that the interrogative word kakuju fails to pied- well that subcategorization requirements in pipe its governor knigu. Note that in English, French in such cases are the same as in English French, and German, such sentences are bad, and German. Since the matrix predicate subcate- e.g. *Tell him which the student checked out gorizes for an interrogative element, it makes book from the library. This contrast across the sense to view the wh-element as having risen in languages is explained in terms of Ross (1967)

45 left branch condition. Languages like English, References German, and French cannot extract an element Bröker, N. 2003. Formal foundations of dependency on a left branch out of an NP. Such cases require grammar. Ágel, V. et al. (eds), Dependency and pied-piping, e.g. Tell him which book the student valency: An international handbook of contempo- checked out of the library. Apparently, the left rary research, vol. 1, 294-310. Berlin: Walter de branch condition is not in force in Russian. This Gruyter. fact should perhaps not be a surprise, since the word order of Slavic languages like Russian is Duchier, D. and R. Debusmann. 2001. Topology de- pendency trees: a constraint based account of li- known to be much freer than that of the German- th ic (and Romance) languages. near precedence. Proceedings from the 39 an- In sum, there is evidence that type 2 rising nual meeting of the Association Computational Linguistics (ACL) 2001, Toulouse, France, 180- is the key to producing a principled DG analysis 187. of many embedded clauses across numerous languages. Eroms, H.-W. and H. J. Heringer. 2003. Dependenz und lineare Ordnung. Ágel, V. et al. (eds.), De- 7 Conclusion pendency and valency: An international handbook of contemporary research, vol. 1, 247-262. Ber- This contribution has provided a DG account of lin: Walter de Gruyter. discontinuities in English and German. Dis- placed units are addressed in terms of catenae Gerdes, K. and S. Kahane. 2001. Word order in Ger- and rising. Two types of rising are acknowl- man: A formal using a to- edged: type 1 and type 2. Since many DGs posit pology model. Proceedings from the 39th annual some mechanism akin to type 1 rising, it should meeting of the Association Computational Lin- not be too controversial. Type 2 rising, however, guistics (ACL) 2001, Toulouse, France, 220-227. is unique to the current DG. Type 2 rising occurs when the risen catena is a non-constituent. Groß, T. and T. Osborne 2009. Toward a practical DG theory of discontinuities. Sky Journal of Lin- By acknowledging type 2 rising, DG is in a guistics 22. 43-90. position to address all discontinuities in a prin- cipled fashion. All displacement obeys the Ris- Hudson, R. 2000. Discontinuities. Kahane, S. (ed.), ing Principle, which requires that either the head Les grammaires de dépendance (Dependency (type 1) or the root (type 2) of a risen catena grammars), Traitement automatique =des langues dominate the governor of that risen catena. This 41, 7-56. Paris: Hermes. principle significantly limits the type of discon- tinuities that the grammar allows. The second Kunze, J. 1975. Abhängigkeitsgrammatik. Studia half of the discussion concentrated on aspects of Grammatica 12. Berlin: Akademie Verlag. type 2 rising. Word order considerations (SV, O‟Grady, W. 1998. The of idioms. Natural V2, VF) provide the primary support for type 2 Language and Linguistic Theory 16:79-312. rising. Finally, something should be said about ris- Osborne, T. 2005. Beyond the constituent: A DG ing catenae. This concept was introduced and analysis of chains. Folia Linguistica 39(3-4). 251- shown in the trees (via italics), but almost noth- 297. ing has been said about why the concept is im- portant. A more comprehensive account of dis- Osborne, T., M. Putnam and T. Groß. (forthcoming). continuities would show that each specific dis- Catenae: Introducing a novel unit of syntactic continuity type (wh-fronting, topicalization, analysis. Syntax. scrambling, extraposition, NP-internal displace- Ross, John R. 1967. Constraints on Variables in Syn- ment) can be described and explained in terms tax. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT. of the rising catenae that each allows. Since the concept is important in this regard, drawing at- tention to it here was warranted.

46