Shame and Nondisclosure: a Study of the Emotional Isolation of People

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Shame and Nondisclosure: a Study of the Emotional Isolation of People BritishJournal of MedicalPsychology (2001), 74, 1–21 Printedin GreatBritain 1 q 2001The British Psychological Society Shameandnon-d isclosure:Astudyofthe emotionalisolationof peopler eferredfor psychotherapy James Macdonald* SalomonsCentre for Applied Socialand PsychologicalDe velopment, Kent, UK Ian Morley Departmentof Psychology, University of Warwick, UK Thirty-fourpeople referred to an NHS psychotherapydepartment were given a modied formof Oatley and Duncan’ s (1992)emotion diary which included questions about whethereach recorded emotion had been subsequently disclosed to anyone (for example apartner,friend or professional). One week later the diaries were collected and participantsinterviewed. Interviews focused, among other things, on reasons for non- disclosureof recorded emotional experiences and the relationship between shame and non-disclosure.The results indicated that a majorityof the emotional incidents recordedin the diaries were not disclosed (68%). This result contrasts with studies onnon-clinical samples in which only approximately 10% of everyday emotions are keptsecret. Qualitative analysis of the interview data revealed that participants appearedto behabitual non-disclosers of emotional and personal experiences and that non-disclosurewas related to the anticipation of negative interpersonal responses to disclosure(in particular labelling and judging responses) in addition to more self- criticalfactors including shame. It issuggested that these results add to the existing literatureon shame by illustrating the interpersonal effects of shame in a clinical sample. There are many unresolved issues regardingthe denition of shame. Ina recent review of theories Gilbert (1998)argues that adenition which captures shame most closely is that it is ‘an inner experience ofself as an unattractive social agent,under pressure to limit possible damage to self via escape orappeasement’(p. 22). Shame can bedifferentiated from guilt where the action tendency promotes reparation andthe focus ofattention is outside the self (Lewis, 1971;T angney,Miller,Flicker, &Barlow,1996).Shame can be associated either with aconsciousness ofhowone is seen byothers (Sartre, 1943)or with a negative self-evaluation, andthese two different facets ofshame have been termed external shame and internalizedshame respectively (cf. Gilbert, 1998).While in practice these two cognitive domains are likely to behighlycorrelated (Lewis, 1971)this is notnecessarily the case (Gilbert, 1998).Perhaps the least controversial characteristic of shame is that it is *Requestsfor reprintsshould be addressedto James Macdonald,Salomons Centrefor AppliedSocial andPsychological Development, David Salomons Estate,Broomhill Road, Southborough, T unbridgeWells, Kent, TN3 0TG, UK. 2 JamesMacdonald andIan Morley associated with adesire to hide or conceal the self oraspects of the self (Barrett, 1995; Lindsay-Hartz, 1984;Mollon, 1984). Inrecent years studies have linked atendency toexperience shame with the incidence ofpsychopathology(Andrews, 1995, 1997; Gilbert, Pehl, &Allan, 1994;Harder, 1995; Sanftner, Barlow,Marschall, &Tangney,1995;T angney,Burggraf,& Wagner,1995; Tangney,Wagner,&Gramzow,1992);however Alexander, Brewin,V earnals, Wolff, and Leff (1999)found to the contrary that pronenessto guilt rather thanshame was associated with depression.Most of this research has been cross-sectional andquestionnaire-based andto date there appears tohave been noattempt to examine the role shame plays in the interpersonal lives of psychologically distressed people. Anaspect of psychologically distressed people’s interpersonal lives that seems especially likely tobeaffected bythe experience of shame, andthe accompanying impulse of hidingor concealment, is that of emotional disclosure. Bothcommon sense andempirical evidence suggestthat emotional disclosure is an important componentof clinical practice. Stiles (1995)has reviewed anumberof studies which suggestthat all types of psychotherapyare characterized byhigh levels of personal disclosure byclients andthat personal disclosure bythe client is in turncorrelated with measures of goodpsychotherapy process. Aconsumer studyby the Mental Health Foundation(1997) indicated that aprimary need of mental-health patients whenin distress is to have ‘someone to talk to’. Thisis consistent with the protective effect of havinga close conding relationship in the presence of other factors conferring vulnerability to psychopathology(Brown & Harris, 1978).Pennebaker andcolleagues (e.g. Pennebaker,1993) have demonstrated that the disclosure of traumatic experiences appears to have physical-health benets (including decreases in visits tophysicians andan enhancement ofimmune function) whendisclosure includes the emotional response to the experience. There is already some evidence that shame may beassociated with emotional secrecy in non-clinical populations.Research byRime ´,Finkenauerand colleagues has revealed that disclosure of everyday emotional experiences occurs approximately 90%of the time (Rime´,Mesquita,Philippot, & Boca, 1991;Rime ´ et al.,1994). Follow-up research designedto identify characteristics of the 10%of emotions that were notdisclosed suggestedthat ‘secret’emotions are associated with the desire to avoid shame andother unpleasant social emotions suchas guilt andembarrassment (Finkenauer &Rime´, 1996; Finkenauer,Rime ´,&Lerot,1996). Finkenauer and her colleagues have notfocused onthe role ofshame per se in the non-disclosure of emotional experiences. Nor didthey examine the disclosure decisions of‘normally occurring’emotions. Finally,as notedabove, their workwas conductedin anon-clinical sample. Thecurrent studyaims to extend the literature onshame andpsychopathology and shame anddisclosure byexamining the impact of shame onan aspect of the daily interpersonal lives of psychologically distressed adults—namely their non-disclosure of specic negative emotional experiences. Given the lack of research in this area, it was decided that aspecic focus onthe role of shame shouldbe accompanied bya more open-endedinquiry into factors associated with non-disclosure.This, it was hoped, wouldresult in aholistic andcontext-sensitive view of the role ofshame in emotional non-disclosure. Thedesire to develop an inclusive andcontext-sensitive understandingof participants’ Shameand non-disclosure 3 decisions notto disclose emotions led to the adoptionof aqualitative research method based onthe ‘grounded’techniques described byPidgeon and Henwood (1996). This procedure enables the researcher to develop atheoretical understandingwhich is based closely onparticipants’ own accounts andwhich can encompass unforeseen factors which mightotherwise beobscuredby the researchers’ a priori constructs. Method Materials Amodied form of Oatleyand Duncan’ s (1992)emotion diary was used to obtain accounts of shame,guilt, hatredand disgust. This method is based on the assumption that, while not all affective experiences are accessibleto introspection, people are able to identify accurately many of their more salient everyday emotionalexperiences (see Oatley and Duncan (1992) for further discussion of thisissue) including shame. A reviewby Macdonald (1998) suggests that, in spite of the fact that few instruments intended to measure shameactually ask participants directly about their experiences of shame (Andrews, 1998), the available evidencesuggests that both clinical and non-clinical samples will voluntarily report experiences of shamein a condential research setting. Oatley and Duncan’ s (1992)development of emotion diaries represented an attemptto obtain more reliable accounts of subjectiveemotional experiences by minimizing retrospective biases,which would seem more likely in studies where people are simply asked to recall an instance of the emotionof interest,as forexamplein Wicker,Payne, and Morgan (1983). Oatley and Duncan (1992) provide someevidence for the validity of thismethod in a studycomparing participants’ diary record of theirown emotionswith an independent record of their emotions provided by their partners. This demonstrated an 84%agreement as totheoccurrence of incidents. Thediary was structured and asked participants to recordspeci c detailsabout each emotion as soonas possibleafter the emotion had been experienced. Participants were asked to llin the diary questions for the rstinstance of anyof thefour emotions of shame,guilt, hatred and disgust that they experienced in the7 daysafter they had been given the diary. The diary included a numberof questionsabout the disclosure or non-disclosureof each emotion that had been recorded. Of relevanceto the current report was the question ‘Didyou tell anyone about this?’ and two follow-up questions asked if thereply was negative ‘ Wereyou afraid abouthow others might see you if you told them?’ and ‘ Didthe thought of tellinganybody make you feelany shame?’Full details and ndingsfrom the diary are reported in Macdonald (1999). Afollow-upsemi-structured interview was carried out in which participants were asked to provide informationabout (1) the reasons for the non-disclosure or disclosureof eachemotion they recorded in the diary,(2) shame and related feelings associated with anticipated disclosure of eachemotion, and (3) theeffects ofdisclosure or non-disclosure of each emotion on their relationships with other people. The interview protocolfor non-disclosed emotions is included in Appendix1. Thesequestions were designed as afollow-up tothe ‘ yes/no’questions about non-disclosure included in the diary.
Recommended publications
  • An Examination of Emotion Dysregulation in Maladaptive Perfectionism T ⁎ Bailee L
    Clinical Psychology Review 71 (2019) 39–50 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Clinical Psychology Review journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinpsychrev Review An examination of emotion dysregulation in maladaptive perfectionism T ⁎ Bailee L. Malivoire, Janice R. Kuo, Martin M. Antony Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, 350 Victoria Street, Toronto, Ontario M5B 2K3, Canada HIGHLIGHTS • Little is known about emotion dysregulation in maladaptive perfectionism. • We review evidence of emotion dysregulation and propose a conceptual model. • Key processes include heightened negative emotions and unhelpful regulation strategies. • Low emotional awareness and unhelpful emotion regulation goals are also discussed. • Targeting these processes may enhance treatment for maladaptive perfectionism. ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Maladaptive perfectionism has been shown to be associated with undesirable outcomes, such as elevated ne- Perfectionism gative emotions and psychopathological traits. Perhaps unsurprisingly, there is preliminary evidence that ma- Emotion regulation ladaptive perfectionism is also related to emotion dysregulation. However, the nature of emotion dysregulation Emotion dysregulation in perfectionism has not been characterized. In this review, Gross and Jazaieri's (2014) clinically-informed Affect framework of emotion dysregulation is used to review the evidence of emotion dysregulation in maladaptive perfectionism. Specifically, this paper reviews evidence of problematic emotional experiences and unhelpful
    [Show full text]
  • Term Toxic Shame Being Mirrored by One
    Donald Bradshaw Nathanson Coined the The compass of term toxic shame. shame Four universal Mark Epstein, Pema Chodron, Being behaviors to Kevin Griffin Karen Horney mirrored defend against Abiding difficult emotions to observe and learn . Four major by one shame. The Idea of PRACTICE and Right View being wise idea that when aempts of Gershen Kaufman non- or attuned. Led to Present with Self and Present with avoiding shame Find the entrances to shaming the neuro2c Others and Wise-Self you are bigger individual to governing scenes. person than or less than Whenever we are makes all come to others. Says, to soluon able to observe upon the learn from our our experience, we difference shame and "Just immediately detach love yourself." from it. Brene Brown Silvan Thomas Tony Webb Empathy opposite of Scheff/Helen Tomkins Virginia Satir The social aspects of shame; judge in Lewis Block Emotions the compass of Four coping areas most Disrupts bond are shame -- aggression, vulnerable to shame; motivators. stances: depression, isolation, Humiliated Placating, judging numbs-easier Affect and addiction. fury. Blame, Being than loss/grief; pre- Acknowledge theory: Alienation and Super- aggression broader frontal cortex off in shame then Scripts are shame. connection to begun as Reasonable, social results from Perfectionism. others soon as we Being Irrelevant avoiding shame. 'Good' shame as restored. are born. humility. Show deference to others. What does acknowledged shame look like? What is attunement? Shame-anger spirals. Governing Scenes Gershen
    [Show full text]
  • Psychological Consequences of Social Isolation During COVID-19 Outbreak
    PERSPECTIVE published: 09 September 2020 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02201 Psychological Consequences of Social Isolation During COVID-19 Outbreak Giada Pietrabissa1,2* and Susan G. Simpson 3,4 1Department of Psychology, Catholic University of Milan, Milan, Italy, 2 Psychology Research Laboratory, IRCCS, Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy, 3 Department of Justice and Society, University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA, Australia, 4 Regional Eating Disorders Unit, NHS Lothian, Edinburgh, United Kingdom Perceived social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic significantly has had an extraordinary global impact, with significant psychological consequences. Changes in our daily lives, feeling of loneliness, job losses, financial difficulty, and grief over the death of loved ones have the potential to affect the mental health of many. In an atmosphere of uncertainty, it is essential that clear and precise information is offered about the problem and how to manage it. In this contribution, a rationale is provided for an urgent call for a rapid response to the mental health impacts of COVID-19. Moreover, suggestions for individuals to regulate their emotions effectively and appropriately are provided. Edited by: Andrea De Giorgio, Keywords: social isolation, COVID-19, psychological consequences, loneliness, depression, clinical psychology eCampus University, Italy Reviewed by: Antonio Iudici, University of Padua, Italy INTRODUCTION Michelle Semonella, Bar-Ilan University, Israel The mental health consequences of COVID-19 are already visible and even by conservative estimates they are yet to reach their peak and likely to considerably outlive the current pandemic. *Correspondence: Giada Pietrabissa The most common psychological disorders emerging are anxiety and panic, obsessive-compulsive [email protected] symptoms, insomnia, digestive problems, as well as depressive symptoms and post-traumatic stress (Rogers et al., 2020).
    [Show full text]
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • Exaggerated Envy and Guilt Measured by Economic Games in Japanese
    Isobe et al. BioPsychoSocial Medicine (2018) 12:19 https://doi.org/10.1186/s13030-018-0138-8 SHORT REPORT Open Access Exaggerated envy and guilt measured by economic games in Japanese women with anorexia nervosa Masanori Isobe1, Michiko Kawabata1, Ema Murao1, Tomomi Noda1, Noriko Matsukawa1, Ryosaku Kawada1, Teruhisa Uwatoko1,2, Toshiya Murai1, Shun’ichi Noma1 and Hidehiko Takahashi1* Abstract Background: Anorexia nervosa (AN) patients are assumed to express high levels of guilt and envy. Ultimatum game (UG) is a standard behavioral task that focuses on interpersonal behavior when splitting a sum of money between two players. UG studies consistently demonstrate that people tend to decrease their inequity in outcomes, one explanation being that economically irrational decision-making may partly arise from the emotions guilt and envy. We assumed that AN patients would perform excessively fair in UG, reflecting high guilt and envy. Methods: We utilized UG to investigate the characteristics of guilt and envy among 24 Japanese AN patients and 22 age-matched healthy controls (HC). The relation between the outcome of UG and decision strategy confirmed by post-experimental questionnaires was analyzed. Results: As proposer, AN offered a larger amount to the responder compared with HC (p = 0.002) while, on the other hand, as responder, AN demanded much higher allocation to accept the offer compared with HC (p = 0.026). Regarding the strategy as responder, AN put more emphasis on fairness and less emphasis on monetary reward compared with HC (p = 0.046, p = 0.042, respectively). Conclusions: The results indicate that Japanese AN patients demonstrate strong preference for fairness, with high guilt and high envy.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Opioids on Emotional Reactivity
    The Effect of Opioids on Emotional Reactivity Steven M. Savvas, BHSc (Hons) Discipline of Pharmacology, School of Medical Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences University of Adelaide August, 2013 A thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy i Steven M. Savvas, PhD Thesis, 2013 TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract .................................................................................................................................... xi Declaration ............................................................................................................................ xiii Acknowledgements ............................................................................................................... xiv CHAPTER 1 - INTRODUCTION ...................................................................................... 1 1.1 OPIOIDS AND OPIOID MAINTENANCE TREATMENT ...................................... 1 1.1.1 A BRIEF HISTORY OF OPIOIDS .......................................................................... 1 1.1.2 OPIOID RECEPTORS ............................................................................................ 1 1.1.3 ADAPTATION TO OPIOIDS.................................................................................. 3 1.1.3.1 Tolerance ........................................................................................................ 4 1.1.3.2 Withdrawal ...................................................................................................... 4 1.1.3.3 Dependence
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Social Isolation and Loneliness in Children With
    brain sciences Review Effects of Social Isolation and Loneliness in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disabilities: A Scoping Review Celia Kwan 1, Mojgan Gitimoghaddam 1,2 and Jean-Paul Collet 1,2,* 1 Department of Pediatrics, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada; [email protected] (C.K.); [email protected] (M.G.) 2 BC Children’s Hospital Research Institute, Vancouver, BC V5Z 4H4, Canada * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-604-999-6860 Received: 19 October 2020; Accepted: 25 October 2020; Published: 28 October 2020 Abstract: Loneliness and social isolation have negative consequences on physical and mental health in both adult and pediatric populations. Children with neurodevelopmental disabilities (NDD) are often excluded and experience more loneliness than their typically developing peers. This scoping review aims to identify the type of studies conducted in children with NDD to determine the effects of loneliness and/or social isolation. Three electronic databases (Ovid MEDLINE, EMBASE, PsychINFO) were searched from inception until 5 February 2019. Two independent reviewers screened the citations for inclusion and extracted data from the included articles. Quantitative (i.e., frequency analysis) and qualitative analyses (i.e., content analysis) were completed. From our search, 5768 citations were screened, 29 were read in full, and 12 were included. Ten were case-control comparisons with cross-sectional assessment of various outcomes, which limited inference. Autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and learning disorder were the most commonly studied NDD. This review showed that loneliness among children with NDD was associated with negative consequences on mental health, behaviour, and psychosocial/emotional development, with a likely long-term impact in adulthood.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Patterns and Coping Mechanisms in the Context of Internet Use
    applied sciences Article Cognitive Patterns and Coping Mechanisms in the Context of Internet Use 1 1 1 2,3, 4 Cristina Costescu , Iulia Chelba , Adrian Ros, an , Attila Kovari * and Jozsef Katona 1 Special Education Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Babes-Bolyai University, Sindicatelor Street no 7, 400029 Cluj-Napoca, Romania; [email protected] (C.C.); [email protected] (I.C.); [email protected] (A.R.) 2 Department of Natural Sciences and Environmental Protection, Institute of Engineering, University of Dunaujvaros, Tancsics M 1/A, 2400 Dunaujvaros, Hungary 3 Alba Regia Technical Faculty, Obuda University, Budai u 45., 8000 Szekesfehervar, Hungary 4 Institute of Computer Engineering, University of Dunaujvaros, Tancsics M 1/A, 2400 Dunaujvaros, Hungary; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +36-25-551-635 Abstract: Recent research indicates there are different cognitive patterns and coping mechanisms related to increased levels of Internet use and emotional distress in adolescents. This study aims to investigate the relationship between coping mechanisms, dysfunctional negative emotions, and Internet use. A total of 54 participants aged between 14 and 19 years old completed a questionnaire containing several measures and demographics information. We measured participants’ coping strategies, emotional distress, social and emotional loneliness, and their online behavior and Internet addiction using self-report questionnaires. In order to identify the relation between the investigated variables, we used correlation analysis and regression, and we tested one mediation model. The results showed that maladaptive coping strategies and Internet use were significant predictors of dysfunctional negative emotions. Moreover, passive wishful thinking, as a pattern of thinking, was Citation: Costescu, C.; Chelba, I.; associated with anxious and depressed feelings.
    [Show full text]
  • Empathy and Feelings of Guilt Experienced by Nurses: a Cross-Sectional Study of Their Role in Burnout and Compassion Fatigue
    Empathy and feelings of guilt experienced by nurses: a cross-sectional study of their role in burnout and compassion fatigue symptoms Abstract Aims: The main goal of this study was to explore the relationships between empathy, empathy-based pathogenic guilt and professional quality of life (burnout and compassion fatigue). We aim to test a model in which we hypothesize that when empathic feelings are related to pathogenic guilt, burnout and compassion fatigue symptoms may be increased. Background: Empathy is at the core of nursing practice, and has been associated with positive outcomes not only for the healthcare provider but also for the patient. However, empathy is also at the core of guilt feelings that, when excessive and misdirected, can lead to pathogenic guilt beliefs. We focused on two types of empathy-based guilt characterized by excessive responsibility over others’ well-being and how these can be related to professional quality of life. Methods and Participants: This study is a cross-sectional self-report survey. Data were collected during 2014 and 2015. 298 nurses from public hospitals in Portugal’s north and center region were surveyed. Professional quality of life (ProQoL), empathy (IRI), and empathy-based guilt (IGQ) were measured using validated self-report measures. Results: Correlation analyses showed that empathy-based guilt was positively associated with empathy, and with burnout and compassion fatigue. Results from multiple mediation models further indicated when empathy is associated with empathy-based guilt, this leads to greater levels of burnout and compassion fatigue. Conclusions: Given the nature of their work, proneness to experience pathogenic guilt feelings may compromise nurses’ well-being, and this should be addressed in training programs aiming at preventing or treating burnout and compassion fatigue.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: SHAME 1 the Significance of Shame: an Adlerian Perspective a Literature Review Presented to the Faculty of The
    Running head: SHAME 1 The Significance of Shame: An Adlerian Perspective A Literature Review Presented to The Faculty of the Adler Graduate School _____________________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirement for the Degree of Master of Arts in Adlerian Counseling and Psychotherapy ______________________ By John R. Nord ______________________ Chair: Meg Whiston, PhD Reader: Rachelle J. Reinisch, DMFT _____________________ October, 2017 SHAME 2 The Significance of Shame: An Adlerian Perspective Copyright © 2017 John R. Nord All rights reserved SHAME 3 Abstract Shame is a universal affect and emotion which has application within cultures and to individuals throughout the world. It can be considered an aid to learning, teaching, or punishing, and it can also be imposed to control or defeat others. Shame refers to a reaction experience of having violated cultural, community, familial, or individual norms in an unacceptable way and having the hidden, vulnerable self exposed to others against our will. For some individuals, shame can represent a minor impact to their lives and well-being. For others, it can be an all-encompassing, life-threatening problem. Shame can appear as an affect during the course of a child’s normally healthy learning. Problematic shame can originate from a number of sources resulting in unmediated mistaken beliefs from dysfunctional infant/caregiving which are never adequately resolved. Traumatic shame can result from multiple sources including family or peer relationships with repetitive abuse. Any repetitive shaming can unconsciously become an internalized secret. An understanding of pathological shame is indeed critical for evaluating client functioning. Either shame or shame proneness within any societal, familial, or occupational relationship or manifesting within an individual can have far reaching implications and long-term consequences.
    [Show full text]
  • Day 2014 Guilt.Pdf
    Day, M.V. (2014). Guilt. In T. R. Levine (Ed.), Encyclopedia of deception (pp. 427-429). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781483306902.n164 Guilt The emotion of guilt is a negative feeling that people can experience for a wrongdoing, such as being untruthful or deceptive to others. This differs from the legal meaning of guilt, which refers to a person's culpability for an offense that violates a particular law. It is also useful to distinguish guilt, which stems from a negative evaluation of a wrongful behavior, from shame, a related emotion that arises from a negative evaluation of the self. The capacity to experience guilt differs from person to person, with some individuals feeling guilty more often than others for a variety of interpersonal and private misdeeds. Individuals can feel guilty for offenses in the past, present, or that are anticipated in the future, as well as for violations committed by close others or by one's group. Overall, years of research evidence suggests that moderate feelings of guilt are adaptive and important for social functioning. Feelings of guilt can occur following a focus on a specific action or nonaction that goes against personal or societal standards. Lying, cheating, and stealing are some of the most common examples of acts that can elicit feelings of guilt. Guilt is considered to be a self-conscious and moral emotion because it involves an evaluation of the self, and it plays a crucial role in guiding moral behavior. Beyond feeling bad, guilt is also characterized by feelings of regret and tension.
    [Show full text]
  • Guilt and Shame Appeals in Advertisements for Conspicuous Consumption Goods
    Guilt and shame appeals in advertisements for conspicuous consumption goods Bachelor thesis Huijgen, Lotte Student number: 940325379110 Date: 26-11-2015 Supervisor: Dr. Ilona E. de Hooge Second reader: Dr. ir. Arnout Fischer Wageningen University Table of contents Introduction ............................................................................................................................................. 2 Literature review ..................................................................................................................................... 4 Conspicuous consumption .................................................................................................................. 4 Emotions and conspicuous consumption ............................................................................................ 4 Guilt ..................................................................................................................................................... 5 Shame .................................................................................................................................................. 6 Framing ................................................................................................................................................ 7 Method .................................................................................................................................................... 9 Introduction ........................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]