Case and Cause in Icelandic: Reconstructing Causal Networks of Cascaded Language Changes

Fermín Moscoso del Prado Martín and Christian Brendel University of California, Santa Barbara Department of , South Hall 3521 Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA [email protected] and [email protected]

Abstract been effected there is very little chance that the language will ever return to its original state be- Linguistic is a process that produces fore the change, in the same way that a diffu- slow irreversible changes in the grammar sion process in a very high dimensional space is and function of a language’s construc- never going to return to the exact same point in tions. Importantly, changes in a part of the space. Drift in language is in this respect a language can have trickle down effects, reminiscent of random genetic drift from evolu- triggering changes elsewhere in that lan- tionary biology (Wright, 1955). However, Sapir’s guage. Although such causally triggered idea of drift goes further in that he viewed it as chains of changes have long been hypoth- a directional process, more similar to Wright’s esized by historical linguists, no explicit (1929) concept of a directional drift related to se- demonstration of the actual causality has lectional pressures. In Sapir’s view, “language has been provided. In this study, we use co- a ‘slope”’; the small changes that accumulate in occurrence statistics and machine learning linguistic drift are not fully random, but rather they to demonstrate that the functions of mor- reflect the speakers’ unconscious cognitive ten- phological cases experience a slow, irre- dency to increase the consistency within their lan- versible drift along history, even in a lan- guages. This idea is currently challenged by some guage as conservative as is Icelandic. Cru- researchers (Croft, 2000; Lupyan and Dale, 2015), cially, we then move on to demonstrate who are of the opinion that purely random drift – –using the notion of Granger-causality– of the same type as that found in genetics–, when that there are explicit causal connections coupled with adequate selection mechanisms, is between the changes in the functions of sufficient to account for the diachronic changes the different cases, which are consistent observed in the world’s languages. Sapir moti- with documented processes in the history vated the need for directional change in what he of Icelandic. Our technique provides a saw as apparent causal chains in language change, means for the quantitative reconstruction which he illustrated with the progressive loss and of connected networks of subtle linguistic functional shift of English oblique case markers, changes. into an absolutive case-free system encoding ani- macy and position relative to the head noun. 1 Introduction ‘Chain reactions’ along the history of a lan- Sapir (1921/2014, p. 123) noticed that “Language guage are particularly well-studied in phonology. moves down on a current of its own making. It Chain shifts (Martinet, 1952) are processes by has a drift” (emphasis added). In Sapir’s view, which the position in perceptual/articulatory space the formation of different dialects requires that the of a phoneme changes in response to the change in small changes constantly being introduced by the position of another phoneme (either moving away speakers are not just plain white noise, but rather from the second phoneme, in a ‘push’ chain, or random walks in which minute changes accumu- moving to occupy the space left void by the other, late over time. The very high dimensionality on in a ‘pull’ chain). A famous example of a chain which languages operate makes cumulative lin- shift is the Great English Vowel Shift. In a similar guistic changes irreversible. Once a change has fashion, one could think of functional chain shifts

2421 Proceedings of the 54th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, pages 2421–2430, Berlin, Germany, August 7-12, 2016. c 2016 Association for Computational Linguistics in , by which a certain morphological nally, in Section 5, we discuss the theoretical im- category takes over some of the functions of an- plications of our results for theories of language other, triggering a chain of ‘push’ and/or ‘pull’ change, as well as the possibilities offered by the movements in other categories. Such cascaded technical innovations presented here. changes have often been reported in diachronic linguistics (Biberauer and Roberts, 2008; Fisiak, 2 Corpus Processing 1984; Lightfoot, 2002; Wittmann, 1983). 2.1 Corpus Icelandic is a famously conservative language. We used the Icelandic Parsed Historical Corpus Compared to most other languages, its grammar (Wallenberg et al., 2011), a sample of around has experienced remarkably little change since one million word tokens of Icelandic texts that the high middle ages. For instance, Barðdal and have been orthographically standardized, man- Eythórsson (2003) argue that the changes it has ually lemmatized, part-of-speech tagged, and experienced from its old phase (Old West Norse; parsed into context-free derivation trees. An ex- mid XI century to mid XIV century) to its cur- ample of the lemmatization and part-of-speech rent phase are comparable to the slight changes oc- tagging for a sentence is shown in Fig. 1. The curring from early Modern English (late XV cen- dating of the text samples ranges from 1,150 CE tury into early XVIII century) into Modern En- to 2,008 CE, covering most of the history of Ice- glish (from early XVIII century). In terms of landic (from its origins in Old West Norse, to the inflectional morphology, change in Icelandic has current official language of Iceland). The corpus been minimal. For instance, one finds that the is divided into 61 files of similar sizes (around nominal paradigms of Old West Norse, are mostly 18,000 words per file), each file corresponding to the same as those of Modern Icelandic. Notwith- a single document. The documents were chosen standing the apparent formal stability of Icelandic to cover the period in a roughly uniform manner, cases, there is evidence that they are experiencing sampling from similar genres across the periods. subtle changes in their functions (Barðdal, 2011; Barðdal and Eythórsson, 2003; Eythórsson, 2000). 2.2 Preprocessing In particular, Barðdal argues that an accumulation We collapsed into a single file all documents that of small syntatico-semantic shifts has finally re- were dated on the same year. This left us with sulted in a shift in the Icelandic dative’s functions 44 files containing texts from distinct years. From (i.e., ‘dative sickness’), possibly triggered by ear- each of the files, we discarded all tokens that lier changes in nominatives and accusatives (e.g., contained anything but valid Icelandic alphabetic ‘nominative sickness’). characters or the dollar sign (used for marking en- In this study, we investigate whether one can re- breaks within a word, such as the clitic deter- liably detect a drift in the functions of Icelandic miner in krossins from the example in Fig. 1). All case and –crucially– whether there is evidence for remaining word tokens were lower-cased, and the causal chain shifts in these functions. In Sec- ‘$’ character was removed from the stem elements tion 2, we describe the processing of a diachronic of broken stem plus clitic pairs (e.g., kross$ was corpus of Icelandic to obtain co-occurrence repre- changed into kross). sentations of the functions of case types and to- kens. Section 3 uses machine learning on the co- 2.3 First Order Co-occurrence Vectors occurrence vectors of tokens to demonstrate that Ideally, for constructing co-occurrence vectors, it the usage of Icelandic cases has been subject to is best to choose as features those words with high- a constant drift along history, a drift that is dis- est overall informativity, which in fact tend to be tinguishable from the overall changes experienced those words with the highest occurrence frequen- by the language in this period. We then go on –in cies (Bullinaria and Levy, 2007; Bullinaria and Section 4– to demonstrate using Granger-causality Levy, 2012; Lowe and McDonald, 2000). In our (Granger, 1969) that there are causal relations be- case, however, using plain token frequencies runs tween the changes in the different cases, and it the risk of creating a representational space that is is possible to reconstruct a directed network of strongly uninformative about particular periods in chain shifts, which is consistent with the directions the history of the language. Instead, we used docu- of causality hypothesized by Barðdal (2011). Fi- ment frequencies, as these still provide a measure

2422 En armar kross- -ins merkja ást við Guð og menn en armur krosur hinn merkja ást við guð og maður CONJ NS-N N-G D-G VBP N-A P NPR-A CONJ NS-A

Figure 1: Tagging and lemmatization of the Old West Norse sentence (number 819) from the Íslensk Hómilíubók (“Icelandic Book of Homilies”; late XII century): En armar krossins merkja ást við Guð og menn. (“But the arms of the cross mark the love of God and man.”) of word frequency (and therefore informativity), Prado Martín, 2007). The second order vectors while at the same time ensuring that those words were computed by passing a symmetrical sliding chosen as features are most representative across window including, for each token, the immedi- the history of the language. We selected as fea- ately preceding and following word. The vector tures all word types that occurred in at least 75% for each token was computed as the average be- of the 44 by-year files, that is, all 529 distinct (un- tween the first order vectors (of Subsection 2.3) lemmatized) word forms that had a document fre- of the preceding and following words. If no first quency in the corpus of at least 33 documents. order vector was available for either the preced- For each (unlemmatized) word type (w) oc- ing or the following word, the second order vec- curring at least three times in the whole corpus tor directly corresponded to the plain first order (17,741 distinct word types), we computed its vector of the word for which there was a first or- co-occurrence frequency with each of the feature der vector. We excluded those tokens for which words (t). In this way, we obtained a matrix of we had first order vectors for neither the previ- 17,741 529 word by feature co-occurrence fre- ous nor the following word type. We computed × quencies (f[w, t]) within a symmetrical window in- such second order vectors for all tokens in the cor- cluding the two preceding and following words.1 pus that had been tagged for grammatical case (a The plain co-occurrence matrix was converted into total of 419,910 vectors, on average 9,453 vec- a matrix of word-feature pointwise mutual infor- tors per year, of which 38.14% were nominatives, mations M = (mi,j), such that, 10.91% were genitives, 26.38% were accusatives, and 24.56% were datives). N f[w , t ] m = log · i j , i,j (W 1) f[w ] f[t ] 1 − · i · j 2.5 Representation of the Case Prototypes where N = 899,763 tokens is the total num- In order to represent the prototypical usages each ber of tokens in the corpus, W1 = 5 is the to- grammatical case (i.e., nominative, genitive, ac- tal sliding window size considered, and f[wi] and cusative, and dative) in a given year, we used f[tj], are the overall corpus frequencies of words the first order co-occurrence technique. For each wi and tj, respectively. In this manner, the row of the 44 distinct years –using the same features Mi, = (mi,1, . . . , mi,529) represents the contexts identified in Subsection 2.3– we computed first · in which the word type wi is found across the order co-occurrence vectors collapsing all word whole corpus. tokens in each grammatical case, and using a reduced window size including just the preced- 2.4 Second Order Co-occurrence Vectors 2 ing and following words (i.e., W2 = 3). For The co-occurrence vectors computed above pro- each year (y) we obtained a 4 529 element × vide representations for the average contexts in matrix of co-occurrence frequencies (fy[c, t]), in- which a given word type is found. In order to rep- dicating the number of times that each case (c) resent the specific context of each word token, we used second order co-occurrence vectors (Schütze 2The optimal window sizes for the first order co- and Pedersen, 1997). These provide important in- occurrence vectors for words and for case prototypes were different because they were chosen to optimize different formation about the aspects of a context that are tasks. The window size for first order vectors for words were relevant for inflectional morphology (Moscoso del chosen to optimize the machine learning algorithm for iden- tifying case identities of second order vectors (Section 3), 1To avoid log 0 values, all frequency counts in this paper whereas the first order vectors for case prototypes were opti- were increased by one. mized for clustering cases across the years (Section 4).

2423 was found to co-occur (within the specified win- barely –if at all– changed along the history of Ice- dow) with feature (t). These matrices were trans- landic. On the basis of this fact alone, one could formed into case to feature pointwise mutual in- conclude that the grammatical case system is not formations, resulting in a series of 44 matrices actually experiencing any linguistic drift, but has (M[y] = (m[y]i,j) such that, rather remained basically static throughout the last millennium. There is, however, another possibil- N f [c , t ] m[y] = log · y i j , ity. Linguistic drift could have been affecting the i,j (W 1) f[c ] f[t ] 2 − · i · j functions of grammatical cases in Icelandic. If this where N is the total number of tokens in the cor- were the case, one would expect to observe a slow pus, f[ci] is the number of instances of case ci, and –constant rate– diachronic change in the contexts f[tj] denotes the number of instances of word tj in which each of the four cases is used. in the corpus. In this way, the rows of the M[y] To investigate this latter possibility, for each matrices provided a representation of the contexts of the 44 years documented in the corpus, we in which each grammatical case was used in each trained a basic logistic classifier in the task of as- year. signing grammatical case to the second order co- occurrence vectors developed in Subsection 2.4. (a) (b) Once each of the classifiers had been trained, we Nominative Accusative Nominative Accusative Genitive Dative Genitive Dative tested the classifiers’ performances on the vectors 2.0 2.0

1.5 1.5 obtained from each of other 43 years on which 1.0 1.0 they were not trained. On the one hand, if the func- 0.5 0.5

0.0 0.0 tions of the cases have indeed remained constant 0.5 0.5 along the history of the language, one would ex- 1.0 1.0

1.5 1.5 pect that the performance of a classifier tested on 2.0 2.0 the data from a given year, should remain approx- 2.5 2.5 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 imately constant when tested on vectors from all other years. If, on the other hand, the functions of Figure 2: (a) Representation of the raw case vec- Icelandic grammatical cases have been subject to tors in SVD-reduced space (i.e., SVD dimension linguistic drift, the irreversible and cumulative na- 1 vs. dimension 2). (b) Representation of the ture of the drift (Sapir, 1921/2014) implies that the case vectors after discounting the average vector classifier error should grow –if only so slightly– for each year (i.e., SVD dimension 1 vs. dimen- with each year passed. The reason for this is that sion 2). the contexts in which one would use each case should be slightly different from year to year. One Fig. 2a plots the spatial organization of the re- should then predict that the error of the classifier sulting vectors (after reducing to a bidimensional should depend on the temporal distance between projection using Singular Value Decomposition; the year of the testing vectors and that of the train- SVD). Notice that the prototypes for each case ing ones. Furthermore, the change in error should very naturally cluster together across the years. be of a linear nature, with a very slight slope. The scatter is however asymmetric, hinting at a When tested on the same years in which they process of change along the years common for all had been trained, the classifiers performed rather four cases. If we compute a yearly overall proto- well in inferring the case to which each of the type vector as the average vector for the cases in context vectors belonged (the distribution of errors each year, and we substract it from the correspond- across the 44 years was well approximated by a ing case prototypes, we find that the case ide- normal distribution with a mean error of 26.67%, tities become clearly differentiated in space (see a standard deviation of 1.99%, and best and worst Fig. 2b), demonstrating that the case prototype classification errors of 22.17% and 30.39%, re- vectors do indeed capture the contextual proper- spectively).3 ties of all four cases, which are highly distinctive. 3Although we chose the best model among different 3 Functional Drift in Icelandic Cases learning algorithms, including multiple versions of Support Vector Machines, Classification Trees, and a Softmax Classi- As was discussed in the Introduction, the inflec- fier, we have no doubt that the learning performance can be tional paradigms marking case and number have improved upon. For our purposes, however, it was sufficient

2424 β = . 002, z = 5. 009, p < . 001 β = . 001, z = 4. 661, p < . 001 β + = 0. 002, p + < 0. 001, β = 0. 002, p < 0. 001 42 − − − 40 45 38

36

40 34

32

Classification Error (%) 30 Classification Error (%) 35 28 0 200 400 600 800 1000 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800 900 Cross-Entropy (nats) Distance in Years 30 Classification Error (%) Figure 4: Independent effects of cross-entropy 500 0 500 Temporal Distance (years) (left panel) and distance in years between the training and testing sets (right panel) as estimated Figure 3: Correlation between the classifier er- by the linear mixed-effects regression model. ror and the temporal distance from the year from which the training vectors were obtained to the time points have diverged depends on the amount year when the testing vectors were obtained. The of time that has intervened, irrespective of whether solid lines plots a linear regressions. it was the training or the testing set that was col- lected before. One could argue that the slow drift observed We then tested the classifiers on the vectors ob- may not be really due to changes in the functions tained from different years. The results are plot- of the grammatical cases themselves, but just to ted in Fig. 3. The scatter plots the difference overall changes either in the overall language, or in in years (i.e., the difference values are positive the very topics that are addressed (e.g., one might when the classifier was tested on vectors obtained guess that talk of swords, slaves, and longships after those used for training, and negative when was more frequent in XII century Norse than it testing with vectors obtained before the training is in Modern Icelandic). To investigate this pos- ones). When testing on data different from the sibility we used an information-theoretical mea- training sets, there is a logical loss in performance sure of the prototypicality of a set of second order (of about 8%) from the baseline of testing on the vectors for a particular year (based on that used same training set. We fitted two linear regressions, in Moscoso del Prado Martín, 2007). From the one to the positive differences and another to the vectors of each year, irrespective of their case, we negative differences (plotted by the solid lines in fitted a 529-dimensional Gaussian distribution (by Fig. 3). The first thing that stands out is that the estimating the mean vector for that year, µ , and performance of the classifier is remarkably good y the covariance matrix, Σ ). The average inade- when tested on vectors obtained at considerable y quacy of a given set of vectors v ,..., v ob- temporal distances from the time when the train- { 1 n} tained in year z to the distribution fitted to the vec- ing vectors were obtained. While the error of the tors obtained in year y is measured by the cross- classifier is of about 34% when tested on vectors entropy, a Monte Carlo estimator of which is given from the year after or before the training vectors, by,4 the error remains at 35% for vectors originating n from texts that are five centuries apart. Once again, 1 1 T 1 H(z, y) K + log Σy + (vi µy) Σ− (vi µy), this speaks to the remarkable conservativeness of ≈ 2 | | 2n − y − Xi=1 the Icelandic language. However, these small dif- where K is a constant.5 In addition, one should ferences are in fact reliable: There are significant also take into account the fact that, for some years, slopes in both regressions (positive differences: the classifier might generalize better or worse than R+ = .161, p+ < .001; negative differences: for others (due to irrelevant idiosyncrasies of one R = .164, p < .001). A second remark- − − − specific text used for training), which could lead able thing is that both regressions are substantially to a distortion of the results. symmetrical, in fact their slopes are basically iden- To investigate whether, after discounting for the tical ( β+ = β = .002). This indicates that the | | | −| inadequacy of the vectors to the overall distribu- degree to which the usages of the cases at different tion of those in which the classifier was trained,

4 to have a classifier with a decent performance, as our goal Assuming Σy is definite positive so that its inverse exists. 5 529 was showing that the error is time-dependent. K = 2 log 2π.

2425 there was still evidence for drift in the functions predictive power that can be found on y’s own of the cases, while also accounting for the dif- past. This idea has proven of great value to inves- ferent generalization powers of the classifiers, we tigate the causal connections between economic fitted a linear mixed-effects model to the classi- variables, sequences of behavioral responses, neu- fier errors, including fixed-effect predictors of the ral spikes, or electroencephalographic potentials. cross-entropy described above, and the absolute Often, the technique is used to reconstruct direc- value of the difference in years between the train- tional networks of variables and processes that ing set and testing set dates (as indicated above, have causal connections. the effects were equivalent for positive and neg- If x and y are stationary time sequences on time ative values in years), and the dating of the test- (τ), in order to test whether x Granger-causes y, ing set as a random effect. As expected, we found one begins by fitting autoregressive models (AR) that the cross-entropies had a significant positive that predict the values of y from its own n values effect (β = .002, z = 5.009, p < .001; left panel lagged into the past. This consists on finding val- if Fig. 4), indicating that the performances of the ues a1, a2,..., an that minimize the error ε in the models were indeed worse for less adequate sets equation, of testing vectors, irrespective of any aspect of grammatical case. However –crucially– even af- past of y y[τ] = a + a y[τ 1] + a y[τ 2] + ... + a y[τ n] +ε[τ]. ter considering the effect of cross-entropy, there 0 1 − 2 − n − remained a significant positive effect of the tempo- z }| { One then augments the autoregression by includ- ral distance (β = .001, z = 4.661, p < .001; right ing m lagged values of x, with additional parame- panel if Fig. 4).6 This result therefore supports the ters b ,..., b to be fitted, hypothesis that the function of grammatical case 1 m has been subject to a slight constant change dur- past of y ing the history of the language: a functional drift. y[τ] =a + a y[τ 1] + a y[τ 2] + ... + a y[τ n] + 0 1 − 2 − n − + b1xz[τ 1] + b2x[τ 2]}| + ... + bmx[τ m] +{ ε[τ]. − − − 4 Functional Chain Shifts in Case past of x | {z } In the previous section we have demonstrated that where the ε sequences are uncorrelated (white) the functions of Icelandic cases have been subject gaussian noises, reflecting the fully random or to slow linguistic drift. The question now arises of chaotic part of the system, which cannot be pre- whether this drift is purely random, or rather it has dicted from its past (i.e., the error, that is termed by some degree of directionality arising from endoge- some the ‘creativity’ of the model). If the second nous linguistic factors. It is possible that changes regression is a significant improvement over the in the functions of some cases caused changes in first, then it can be said that x Granger-causes y, the functions of others. We investigate this possi- indicating that past values of x significantly pre- bility using the notion of Granger-causality dict future values of y over and above any predic- tive power of y’s own past values. This is tested 4.1 Granger-causality using an F -test, with the null hypothesis being that Granger-causality (Granger, 1969) is a powerful the second model does not improve on the first technique for assessing whether one time series one. The selection of the autoregressive order pa- can be said to be the cause of another one. The ba- rameters n and m is achieved by model compar- sic idea is that one time series x is said to Granger- isons using information criteria. cause another series y if the past of series x pre- When one is interested in reconstructing a net- dicts the future of series y over and above any work of causal relations between multiple vari-

6 ables, one can use a mutivariate generalization of The estimated covariance matrices were not definite pos- itive for two of the years, which were excluded from the anal- the AR model, the vector autoregressive model yses. In addition, in 552 out of the remaining 1,849 estimates, (VAR). The VAR model consists of mutiple AR the cross-entropy took unusually large values, orders of mag- nitude larger than the rest (likely reflecting inadequacy of the equations (one for each variable in the model). If multidimensional Gaussian approximation for these cases), we consider an autoregressive order of one (i.e., which distorted the effect estimates. The analyses reported m = n = 1), when we are simultaneously consid- exclude these 552 points. However, keeping these outlying ering p variables = y ,..., y , the VAR[1] values in the regression, both key effects remained signifi- Y { 1 p} cant, but the slope estimates were less trustworthy. model to be fitted can be expressed in matrix no-

2426 tation as, this differentiation, plotted in Fig. 5c, removed the

y1[τ] a1 A1,1 ...A1,p y1[τ 1] ε1[τ] non-stationary trends from the original series...... − .  .  =  .  +  . .. .   .  +  .  . y [τ] a A ...A y [τ 1] ε [τ]  p   p  p,1 p,p  p −   p            Table 1: Results of the Granger-causality analyses. This model enables testing for Granger-causality Causality directions that remained significant after between any pair of variables y and y i ∈ Y j ∈ FDR correction are highlighted in bold. , after partialling out the possible confounding Direction F [1, 144] p p (FDR) Direction F [1, 144] p p (FDR) Y Nom. Gen. 2.614 .108 .184 Gen. Nom. 5.618 .019 .046 effects of y , t = i, t = j, 1 t p . y is said → → t j Nom. Dat. 8.295 .005 .018 Dat. Nom. 3.834 .052 .104 → → { 6 6 ≤ ≤ } Gen. Acc. .566 .453 .454 Acc. Gen. 2.408 .123 .184 to Granger-cause yi if the model coefficient Ai,j → → Acc. Nom. 6.802 .010 .030 Nom. Acc. .644 .424 .454 → → Acc. Dat. 10.249 .002 .018 Dat. Acc. .563 .454 .454 is significantly different than zero, and the reverse → → Dat. Gen. 1.354 .246 .329 Gen. Dat. 9.034 .003 .018 → → holds if Aj,i significantly different than zero (i.e., yi Granger-causes yj). We fitted a VAR[n] model to the four dif- 4.2 Granger-causality in Case Drift ferentiated time-series. The autoregressive order To investigate whether the pattern of change in found to maximize Akaike’s Information Crite- 8 one case triggers (i.e., Granger-cause) the pattern rion (Akaike, 1974) was n = 1. The F statis- of change in another, we made use of the pro- tics and significance values for the coefficients in totype vectors for the cases in each of the years the resulting VAR[1] model are given in Tab. 1. developed in Subsection 2.5. As a measure of In order to reconstruct the causality network, we the amount of contextual change for a given case also need to consider that we started out with only in a given year, we computed the city-block dis- very vague predictions on the possible directions tances between the case prototypes from each year of causality. As the model involved twelve sepa- to the next available time point, which are plot- rate p-value tests, the p-value estimates need to be ted in Fig. 5a. Notice that there is an overall pat- corrected for multiple comparisons. This correc- tern of change equally affecting all cases, and the tion was done using the false discovery rate (FDR) changes are therefore strongly correlated. This re- method (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995), result- flects the overall pattern of historical changes af- ing in the corrected p-value estimates listed in the fecting Icelandic as a whole, as well as changes last column of Tab. 1. in the topics that would be discussed in the differ- The Granger-causality analysis leads us to re- ent time periods, as was documented in Subsec- construct the causality network depicted in Fig. 6. tion 2.5 and Section 3. Considering the changes It appears that the drift in the functions of Ice- in each case as a component in a four-dimensional landic case is not plainly random. Instead, we find vector, the modulus of this vector (plotted by the evidence that changes in the functions of the ac- dashed orange line in Fig. 5a) gives the overall cusatives and genitives have had a domino effect, magnitude of the changes that are unspecific to triggering further changes in the functions of nom- the cases themselves. To remove this component inatives. Finally, changes in all other three cases from the changes, we fitted a linear regression to result in changes in the functions of the dative. the sequence of changes in each case, using the In summary, the changes observed are consistent overall pattern of change as a predictor. Fig. 5b with the idea discussed in the Introduction of a plots the resulting residuals, indicating the amount functional chain shift affecting the morphological of change that was specific to each case, over and case system of Icelandic. above the overall pattern.7 5 Discussion A precondition for testing for Granger-causality is that the time series under consideration are sta- We have presented evidence for a steady drift –of tionary. In our case, the series depicted in Fig. 5b the precise kind advocated by Sapir (1921/2014)– are significantly non-stationary; they exhibit, for even in a language as remarkably conservative as instance, significant temporal trends. In order to is Icelandic. This supports the claim that hu- remove the non-stationarities, the series were dif- man languages are in a state of ‘perpetual mo- ferentiated (i.e., we considered the difference be- tion’ (Beckner et al., 2009; Dediu et al., 2013; tween each two consecutive points). The result of Hawkins and Gell-Mann, 1992; Hopper, 1987; 7Negative values in this figure indicate changing less than 8In fact n = 1 was also found to maximize both Akaike’s the average, rather than ‘negative change’. Final Prediction Error and Hannan-Quinn Criteria.

2427 (a) (b) (c)

nominatives accusatives nominatives accusatives Overall pattern nominatives accusatives genitives datives genitives datives genitives datives 100 1000 80 80 900 60 60 800 40 40 700 20 20 600 0

500 20 0

400 20 Change in Usage 40

40 300 Residual Change in Usage 60 Difference in Residual Change 200 80 60

100 100 80 1200 1300 1400 1500 1600 1700 1800 1900 2000 1200 1300 1400 1500 1600 1700 1800 1900 2000 1300 1400 1500 1600 1700 1800 1900 Year (CE) Year (CE) Year (CE)

Figure 5: (a) Overall value of the city-block distances between the prototypical case vectors for consecu- tive years. (b) Residual value of the distances specific to each case after residualizing the overall pattern of change. (c) Differentiated values of the residualized distances, removing non-stationarities.

between them. Traditionally, historical linguists p=.018 have focused on ‘narrative’ accounts of the the Accusative p=.030 chains of change within a language. Although such type of accounts are extremely useful, the ' p=.018 # Nominative / Dative often very subtle changes in usage that can occur 7 ; p=.046 from one time-point to another cannot always be described with such clearcut patterns. Neverthe- Genitive p=.018 less, we have shown that those very small changes do accumulate in meaningful ways. Figure 6: Reconstructed network of Granger- causal connections between diachronic changes An important question addressed by this study in case functions. The p-values indicated on the is the presence of endogenous causal chains in causal arrows are FDR-corrected. language change. Lupyan and Dale (2015) argue that languages are constrained by their ‘ecolog- ical niches’, the communities in which they are Larsen-Freeman and Cameron, 2007; Niyogi and spoken, and the extralinguistic properties of those Berwick, 1997). Although we have found that niches can trigger exogenous change in the mor- functional change in Icelandic case has proceeded phology of the languages. Following on Lupyan at a constant rate, we do not think, as argued by and Dale’s ecosystem analogy, one should see that, Nettle (1999a, 1999b), that this rate of change as well as being part of ecosystems, languages are needs to be constant across languages. There also ecosystems in themselves, in a nesting similar are strong arguments suggesting that in other lan- to that found in natural ecosystems (i.e., an animal guages such rates might be different (Wichmann, is part of a particular ecosystem, and its body is 2008; Wichmann and Holman, 2009). an ecosystem in itself). Sounds, words and con- The crucial innovation presented in this paper structions have their own ecological niches within is the reconstruction of the causality network link- the language, and disturbances in the system can ing the changes in the four cases. Previous ap- trigger cascaded changes, leading to readaptation plications of the notion of Granger-causality to (evolution) of the constructions. This contrasts diachronic language change (Moscoso del Prado with the view of changes in the function of Ice- Martín, 2014) have focused on the macroscopic landic cases expressed by Eythórsson (2000). He relation between sudden changes in syntax and showed that whose arguments exhibit ‘nom- morphology. Here, we have demonstrated that inative sickness’ and ‘accusative sickness’ tend to Granger-causality can also be used to reconstruct be clustered along certain syntactic and seman- detailed networks of slow changes within the mor- tic lines. That it is in these particular niches that phological system, at a more microscopic scale. accusatives and datives ended up settling is not, The techniques developed offer a mechanism for however, the cause of the language changes. As investigating subtle changes in the functions of we have shown, the case system was subject to a linguistic constructions, and the causal relations string of cascaded pressures. That the cases ended

2428 up settling in new syntactico-semantic niches was Adam Fisiak, editor. 1984. Historical Syntax. de the result, rather than the cause of the changes. Gruyter, Berlin. Clive W. J. Granger. 1969. Investigating causal re- lations by econometric models and cross-spectral References methods. Econometrica, 37:424–438.

Hirotogu Akaike. 1974. A new look at the statistical John Hawkins and Murray Gell-Mann, editors. 1992. model identification. IEEE Transactions on Auto- The Evolution of Human Languages. Santa Fe Insti- matic Control, 19:716–723. tute Studies in the Sciences of Complexity. Addison Wesley, Reading, MA. Jóhanna Barðdal and Thórhallur Eythórsson. 2003. The change that never happened: the story of Paul J. Hopper. 1987. Emergent grammar. Proceed- oblique subjects. Journal of Linguistics, 39:439– ings of the Berkeley Linguistic Society, 13:139–157. 472. Diane Larsen-Freeman and Lynne Cameron. 2007. Jóhanna Barðdal. 2011. The rise of dative substitution Complex Systems and Applied Linguistics. Oxford in the history of Icelandic. Lingua, 121:60–79. University Press, Oxford, UK.

Clay Beckner, Nick C. Ellis, Richard Blythe, John David Lightfoot, editor. 2002. Syntactic Effects of Holland, Joan Bybee, Jinyun Ke, Morten H. Chris- Morphological Change. Oxford University Press, tensen, Diane Larsen-Freeman, William Croft, and Oxford, UK. Tom Schoenemann. 2009. Language is a complex adaptive system: Position paper. Language Learn- Will Lowe and Scott McDonald. 2000. The direct ing, 59:1–26. route: Mediated priming in semantic space. In Lila Gleitman and Aravind K. Joshi, editors, Proceedings Yoav Benjamini and Yosef Hochberg. 1995. Control- of the XXII Annual Conference of the Cognitive Sci- ling the false discovery rate: a practical and pow- ence Society, pages 806–811, Austin, TX. Cognitive erful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Science Society. Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 57:289–300. Gary Lupyan and Rick Dale. 2015. The role of Theresa Biberauer and Ian Roberts. 2008. Cascad- adaptation in understanding linguistic diversity. In ing parameter changes: internally driven change in Rik De Busser and Randy J. LaPolla, editors, Lan- Middle and Early Modern English. In Thórhallur guage structure and environment: Social, cultural, Eythórsson, editor, Grammatical Change and Lin- and natural factors, pages 289–316. John Benjamins guistic Theory: The Rosendal Papers, pages 79– Publishing Company, Philadelphia, PA. 114. John Benjamins, Philadelphia, PA. André Martinet. 1952. Function, structure, and sound John A. Bullinaria and Joseph P. Levy. 2007. Ex- change. Word, 8:1–32. tracting semantic representations from word co- occurrence statistics: A computational study. Be- Fermín Moscoso del Prado Martín. 2007. Co- havior Research Methods, 39:510–526. occurrence and the effect of inflectional paradigms. Lingue e Linguaggio, 6:247–263. John A. Bullinaria and Joseph P. Levy. 2012. Ex- Fermín Moscoso del Prado Martín. 2014. Grammat- tracting semantic representations from word co- ical change begins within the word: Causal model- occurrence statistics: Stop-lists, stemming and ing of the co-evolution of Icelandic morphology and SVD. Behavior Research Methods, 44:890–907. syntax. In Paul Bello, Marcello Guarini, Marjorie McShane, and Brian Scassellati, editors, Proceed- William Croft. 2000. Explaining Language Change: ings of the XXXVII Annual Conference of the Cogni- An Evolutionary Approach. Longman, London, tive Science Society, pages 2657–2662, Austin, TX. England. Cognitive Science Society.

Dan Dediu, Michael Cysouw, Stephen C. Levin- Daniel Nettle. 1999a. Using social impact theory to son, Andrea Baronchelli, Morten H. Christensen, simulate language change. Lingua, 108:95–117. William Croft, Nicholas Evans, Simon Garrod, Rusell D. Gray, Anne Kandler, and Elena Lieven. Daniel Nettle. 1999b. Is the rate of linguistic change 2013. Cultural evolution of language. In Peter J. constant? Lingua, 108:119–136. Richerson and Morten H. Christensen, editors, Cul- tural Evolution: Society, Technology, Language, and Partha Niyogi and Robert C. Berwick. 1997. A dy- Religion, pages 303–331. MIT Press, Cambridge, namical systems model for language change. Com- MA. plex Systems, 11:161–204.

Thórhallur Eythórsson. 2000. Dative vs. nominative: Edward Sapir. 2014. Language: An Introduction to the changes in quirky subjects in Icelandic. Leeds Work- Study of Speech. Dover Publications, Mineola, NY. ing Papers in Linguistics, 8:27–44. (Original work published 1921).

2429 Hinrich Schütze and Jan O. Pedersen. 1997. A cooccurrence-based thesaurus and two applications to information retrieval. Information Processing & Management, 33:307–318. Joel C. Wallenberg, Anton Karl Ingason, Einar Freyr Sigurðsson, and Eiríkur Rögnvaldsson. 2011. Ice- landic parsed historical corpus (IcePaHC – v. 0.9). Søren Wichmann and Eric W. Holman. 2009. Pop- ulation size and rates of language change. Human Biology, 81:259–274. Søren Wichmann. 2008. The emerging field of lan- guage dynamics. Language & Linguistics Compass, 2:1294–1297. Henri Wittmann. 1983. Les réactions en chaîne en morphologie diachronique (“Chain reactions in di- achronic morphology”). In Actes du colloque de la Societé Internationale de Linguistique Fonction- nelle, volume 10, pages 285–292, Québec, Canada. Université Laval. Sewall Wright. 1929. The evolution of dominance. The American Naturalist, 63:556–561. Sewall Wright. 1955. Classification of the factors of evolution. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quan- titative Biology, 20:16–24.

2430