Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU

Dissertations Graduate College

12-1978

A Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Modern Mathematics in Comparison with Traditional Mathematics in the American Educational System

Kuriakose K. Athappilly Western Michigan University

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations

Part of the Science and Commons

Recommended Citation Athappilly, Kuriakose K., "A Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Modern Mathematics in Comparison with Traditional Mathematics in the American Educational System" (1978). Dissertations. 2693. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations/2693

This Dissertation-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. A META-ANALYSIS OF THE EFFECTS OF MODERN MATHEMATICS IN COMPARISON WITH TRADITIONAL MATHEMATICS IN THE AMERICAN EDUCATIONAL SYSTEM

by

Kuriakose K. Athappilly

A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of The Graduate College in partial fulfillment of the Degree of Doctor of Education

Western Michigan University Kalamazoo, Michigan December 1978

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The present study as well as my entire doctoral program could not

have been accomplished without the assistance and encouragement of

many people. I wish to gratefully acknowledge my appreciation to the

following people.

Dr. Uldis Smidchens, chair of my doctoral committee, Professor,

Department of Educational Leadership, contributed most generously of

his time, energy, and expertise as an educationist, researcher, and

above all as a person of utmost understanding and singular dedication

to the cause of the students. Dr. John Kofel, my former advisor and

committee member, Director of International Education Center, had truly

been to me "a friend, philosopher, and guide" from the beginning to

the end of my doctoral program. Dr. James Riley, Professor, Department

of Mathematics, provided his professional expertise for the research.

To these members of my doctoral committee a debt of gratitude is most

willingly, but inadequately, acknowledged.

I am extremely grateful to the authorities of Western Michigan

University General Library. Although they must remain anonymous, it

must be acknowledged that this research could not have materialized

without their generous financial cooperation. I thank also my friends

and acquaintances— students, faculty members, and administrators— who

gave me their encouragement, concern, and understanding.

My mother, Mrs. Monica Athappilly, has immeasurably provided her

moral and physical support to all my educational ventures. I am

ii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. grateful for her continued interest and prayers; for, in a sense, this

work represents a fulfillment of one of her dreams.

My deepest appreciation goes to my wife, Sossa, and children, Geo,

Geena, and Geetha. Sossa has for the past few years abandoned her pro­

fession to free me for this work. In to being wife and mother,

she has most willingly assumed responsibilities that both parents should

share. The cooperation, encouragement, and sacrifice of my family made

this study a work of love.

This dissertation is dedicated to the memory of my father,

Kunjuvarkey Athappilly, 1907-1967.

Kuriakose K. Athappilly

i i i

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. INFORMATION TO USERS

This was produced from a copy of a document sent to us for microfilming. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the material submitted.

The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or notations which may appear on this reproduction.

1. The sign or “ target” for pages apparently lacking from the document photographed is “Missing Page(s)” . If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting through an image and duplicating adjacent pages to assure you of complete continuity.

2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a round black mark it is an indication that the film inspector noticed either blurred copy because of movement during exposure, or duplicate copy. Unless we meant to delete copyrighted materials that should not have been filmed, you will find a good image of the page in the adjacent frame.

3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., is part of the material being photo­ graphed the photographer has followed a definite method in “sectioning” the material. It is customary to begin filming at the upper left hand comer of a large sheet and to continue from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. If necessary, sectioning is continued again-beginning below the first row and continuing on until complete.

4. For any illustrations that cannot be reproduced satisfactorily by xerography, photographic prints can be purchased at additional cost and tipped into your xerographic copy. Requests can be made to our Dissertations Customer Services Department.

5. Some pages in any document may have indistinct print. In all cases we have filmed the best available copy.

University Microfilms International 300 N. ZEEB ROAD, ANN ARBOR, Ml 48106 18 BEDFORD ROW, LONDON WC1R 4EJ, ENGLAND

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 790861*3

ATHAPPILLY, KURIAKOSE KUNJUVARKEY A META-ANALYSIS OF THE EFFECTS OF MODERN MATHEMATICS IN COMPARISON WITH TRADITIONAL MATHEMATICS IN THE AMERICAN EDUCATIONAL SYSTEM.

WESTERN MICHIGAN UNIVERSITY, ED.D., 1978

University Microhms International 300 n. zeeb road, ann arbor, mi 4bio6

@ 1978

KURIAKOSE KUNJUVARKEY ATHAPPILLY

ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF T A B L E S ...... , . . . vl

LIST OF F I G U R E S...... viii

CHAPTER

I INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND INFORMATION .... 1

Purpose of the S t u d y ...... 1

The Evolution of Modern Mathematics .... 2

The Role of Developmental Projects ...... 4

Toward Definitions ...... 10

The Statement of the P r o b l e m ...... 11

Significance of the S t u d y ...... 12

Research Questions ...... 13

Topics Excluded ...... 14

Organization of Dissertation ...... 15

II RATIONALE FOR THE S T U D Y ...... 17

Research Studies on Modern Mathematics .. . 17

Techniques of Aggregation ...... 28

S u m m a r y ...... 35

III DESIGN AND DATA A N A L Y S I S ...... 37

Concept of Meta-analysis ...... 37

Effect S i z e ...... 39

Techniques of Measuring Effect Sizes .... 45

Multiple Effect S i z e ...... 47

Sources of the Stud i e s ...... 48

Locating and Selecting Studies ...... 49

iv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Table of Contents— continued

CHAPTER

Identifying and Coding Variables ...... 53

Analysis of Data . , ...... 55

Procedures for Judging the Magnitude and Significance of Effect S i z e ...... 56

S u m m a r y ...... 57

IV R E S U L T S ...... 59

Description of the Studies Included .... 59

Overall Effects of Modern Mathematics . . . 62

Specific Effects by Subject Characteristics ...... 64

Specific Effects by Treatment F a c t o r s ...... 69

Specific Effect Size by Study Variables...... 77

Specific Effects by Design Variables .... 85

S u m m a r y ...... 88

V INTERPRETATIONS, IMPLICATIONS, GENERAL RECOMMENDATIONS, AND SUMMARY ...... 92

Interpretations ...... 92

Implications ...... 100

General Recommendations ...... 104

S u m m a r y ...... 107

BIBLIOGRAPHY ...... 112

APPENDICES ...... 132

Appendix A: The List of the Variables Identified...... 133

Appendix B: Code Sheet of the Variables Identified ...... 137

v

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. LIST OF TABLES

1. Overview of the Development of Mathematics Programs Since 1951 ...... 5

2. Study I ...... 31

3. Study I I ...... 31

4. Studies I and II C o m b i n e d ...... 32

5. Studies Classified by Source ...... 60

6. Studies Classified by Y e a r ...... 61

7. Length of Treatment and Number of Subjects Per S t u d y ...... 62

8. Grand Mean Effect Sizes in Achievement and A t t i t u d e ...... 63

9. Effect Size by Levels of Grade and Ability in Achievement ...... 67

10. Effect Size by Socio-economic Status and Sex in A c h i e v e m e n t ...... 70

11. Effect Size by Content Area in Achievement .... 72

12. Effect Size by Type of Program in Achievement...... 75

13. Effect Size by Length of Treatment in Achievement...... 76

14. Effect Size by Number of Subjects Per Study in A c h i e v e m e n t ...... 78

15. Effect Size by Study Approach in Achieve­ ment and Att i t u d e ...... 80

16. Effect size by Source in Achievement...... 82

17. Effect Size by Subject Area of Investigation in Achievement ...... 83

18. Effect Size by Affiliation of the Researcher in A c h i e v e m e n t ...... 84

vi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. List of Tables— continued

19. Effect Size by Design Variables in Achievement...... 87

20. Effect Sizes by All Categories of Variables in Achievement and/or Attitude ...... 90

vii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. LIST OF FIGURES

1* Overview of the cluster approach ...... 34

2. Normal curves illustrating the effect of modern treatment in relation to tradi­ tional treatment— a hypothetical illustration ...... 41

3. Normal curves illustrating the effects of three different treatments of modern mathematics (modern algebra, modern geometry, and modern arithmetic) in relation to untreated traditional group— a hypothetical illustration ...... 44

4. Normal curves illustrating the aggregate effect of modern mathematics in achieve­ ment and attitude in relation to tradi­ tional mathematics group ...... 65

viii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND INFORMATION

Many innovations in the content, methodology, and materials

related to mathematics education at the elementary, secondary, and

post-secondary levels were introduced during the past three decades

in an effort to make the American mathematics curriculum relevant to

current needs. Studies on the effects of these innovative efforts

were found primarily in dissertations, journals, and technical

reports. Many curriculum changes in mathematics education were intro­

duced under a common label called "new mathematics" or "modern mathe­

matics." The terms "new mathematics" and "modern mathematics" were

used interchangeably, since most of the literature related to the

changes which occurred in mathematics curricula did not differentiate

between the two terms.

Purpose of the Study

The studies under the titles "new mathematics" or "modern mathe­

matics" were numerous and their findings were often dissimilar and

conflicting (Dieudonne, 1973; Thom, 1971). Some attempts had been

made to summarize and extract meaning from portions of the vast body

of research data in the area of new mathematics. Recently, Hartley

(1977) investigated the summative results of research pertaining to

the effects of individually paced ins* -.-uctions in mathematics, which

is a later offshoot of modern mathematics. No attempt had been made,

1

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 2 however, to summarize the body of data related to the various con­

cepts encompassed within new mathematics. The purpose of this study,

therefore, was (a) to summarize the effects which were unique to the

new mathematics curricula and (b) to examine whether or not the new

mathematics curricula led to improved academic achievement and atti­

tude over "traditional mathematics."

The Evolution of Modern Mathematics

Toward the middle of the twentieth century, educational leaders

in the colleges and universities, high school curriculum supervisors,

mathematical organizations, and other interested individuals began

challenging the traditional sequential pattern of mathematics teach­

ing. In 1940, Betz cited several factors, such as general unawareness

of the significance of mathematics, one-sided emphasis on the doctrine

of "social utility," and mechanistic methods of teaching, as contri­

buting to the poor status of mathematics in the schools.

In the same year, the Progressive Education Association (PEA)

published a report, Mathematics in General Education, which aimed to

develop a mathematics curriculum to meet the needs of the students

(Bidwell & Clason, 1970, p. 531). Another report of the same year

was the Place of Mathematics in Secondary Education, published by the

joint commission of the Mathematical Association of America (MAA) and

the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM). The report

emphasized "the goals of mathematics and the role of individual dif­

ferences among students" (Bidwell & Clason, 1970, p. 532). Reports

and publications of this nature asserted that the curriculum changes

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. in mathematics, which emphasized concepts, understanding, and insight

without neglecting basic skills, were imperative to cope with the

rapid advances in the field of science and technology.

World War II and Sputnik

Two critical events gave great stimuli for reform in mathematics

education: World War II and the Russian launching of the first satel­

lite (Sputnik) in 195 7. World War II created a perceived need for

trained manpower in scientific technology which, in turn, placed a

new emphasis on improved mathematics education. The Education for

All American Youth Report of 1945, the three reports of the Commis­

sion on Post-War Plans of the National Council of Teachers of Mathe­

matics (NCTM) (1944, 1945, 1947), and The Steelman Report of 1947 were

typical products of the times. All five reports reflected an increas­

ing dissatisfaction with the content and approach of school mathemat­

ics, while calling for innovation in mathematics education.

The launching of Sputnik by the Russians and the consequent com­

petition of the United States with Russia over space superiority

created even greater pressure than World War II for the production of

large numbers of qualified scientists, engineers, and mathematicians.

As a result, the direction of mathematical research was toward the

objective of developing mathematics curriculum adequate for the tech­

nological age rather than predictive studies toward success in the

former sequential mathematics courses (Anglin, 1966).

National foundations, the federal government, and private organ­

izations provided financial aid for curriculum improvement in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 4

mathematics. As a result, mathematics scholars— in cooperation with

educational specialists, supervisors, and teachers— began developing

new mathematics programs for elementary and secondary schools.

Sherman (1972) reviewed the purposes and accomplishments of all major

mathematics programs established since 1951. An overview of those

programs is shown in Table 1, including the year, range, and essential

characteristics of 10 prominent programs.

The Role of Developmental Projects

Most of the developmental projects as seen in Table 1 were ini­

tiated primarily to correct perceived weaknesses in school programs.

Consequently, the initial concerns were to produce adequate educa­

tional materials and to establish appropriate teacher training pro­

grams. The University of Illinois Committee on School Mathematics,

"the progenitor of all current curriculum projects in mathematics"

(NCTM, 1970, 32nd Yearbook, p. 257), was founded in 1951 to investi­

gate problems concerning the content and teaching of high school math­

ematics and to correct weaknesses in that area. As a first step

toward that goal, it produced materials which were pilot tested in

classrooms, and it was ascertained that the success of the materials

depended on the skills of the teacher.

In addition, the Madison Project placed a major emphasis on

teacher training through demonstration centers and in-service pro­

grams. The three main objectives of the Greater Cleveland Mathematics

Program were production of ample materials, teacher training, and

evaluation. The Minnesota Mathematics and Science Teaching Project

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. teachers point of view student background and experience bers Correspondence course with graduate credit for Stress Stress mathematical on ideas Intuitive and axiomatic approaches Cultural aspects man'sof experience with num­ Precise terminology Nongraded units be to used in accordancewith Deemphasis on social arithmetic Logical development Table 1 Table Support of of Illinois patterns USOE Carnegie Consistency; Early verbalization precise terminology discouraged University Logical structure mathematics; of study of Ball State Structure of mathematics NSF Learning through discovery University Interrelatedness of principles NSF Structure mathematics of from the historical Secondary Secondary Elementary Grades 7,8 Elementary 1957 1957 Secondary Year Overview of Developmentthe of Mathematics Programs Since 1951 (1953) (1953) College Started Program (BCMI) (BSU) Institute of of Illinois Committee on School Mathe­ University Mathematics Ball State University matics (UICSM) Boston College

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CTv Essentials own own solutions problems to of of arithmetic erately omitted program ously encouraged of of project the ber systems Concept of set and operations on set Integration of arithmetic, algebra and geometry Inductive and deductive reasoning Discovery of structure through finding one's Discussion through careful questioning Unstructured tasks; social applications delib­ Ungraded material intended as a supplementary Fundamental learning processes major a concern Relationship between set theory and foundations Mathematics as a language; precise terminology Unifying concepts; particular emphasis num­ on Learning through discovery Verbal and operational components simultane­ Logic Properties mathematical of a structure Precise language; mathematical laws Algebraic and geometric principles texts sale of Carnegie Carnegie USOE Funds from Table 1 1 (continued) Table NSF NSF USOE NSF Range Support Present Secondary Elementary Elementary Elementary 1959 Grades 7,8 1957 1959 Year Started Program (SUSNP) of Marylandof Stanford Sets and Project Numbers Project Madison University University Mathematics

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Essentials tion things out tant mathematicaltant ideas opportunity for conventional practice and review course of study; no grade levels mathematics not just some to subdivision mathematics concept formation K-12 Concepts mathematics of as part of whole the of Intuitive thinking; guessing; inventing; trying Search for patterns and relationships Structure mathematics of Spiral approach New content wellas as conventional topics; No strong position on verbal-operational ques­ Developing a feeling mathematicalfor ideas Topics with "travel"— an adventure Logical structure mathematicsof Emphasis a on continuous and systematic flow of No stress verbalization; on not a complete Project seeks teachable alternatives for impor­ Discovery approach Mathematical exploration Mathematical laws Support of of Illinois Carnegie Table 1 1 (continued) Table The Council NSF University NSF Year Present Program Started Range (UIAP) (SMSG) (GCMP) School Study Group 1958 Secondary Greater Cleveland 1959 Secondary Elementary Project Mathematics Elementary Arithematic 1958 Elementary Program Mathematics

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Their Origins and Evolutions, by Helene tive tive science curriculum K-9 and undergraduate courses for cultural and historical aspects of mathemat­ number system, Euclidean space, a withspace ics pre-service education of teachers measure Concepts of an algebraic structure and a deduc­ Connections between mathematics and science; Three specific mathematics structures; the real Ungraded units Working toward a coordinated mathematics-science Table 1 1 (continued) Table Range Support Essentials Present 1961 Year (1958) (1958) Elementary Started Note. Note. From Common Elements New in Mathematics Programs: Program (MINNEMAST) and and Science Teaching Project Sherman. Sherman. NewYork: Teacher's College Press, 1972. Minnesota Mathematics

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 9 started with the goal of finding out what children could learn and

then preparing teachers to teach it.

The Boston College Mathematics Institute was organized with the

purpose of reeducating high school teachers in the elements of con­

temporary mathematics. The Ball State University Project was created

to improve materials in secondary school geometry, and later in grades

7-12 and then in grades K-6. Summarily, improving materials and

training teachers were the first steps toward introducing new mathe­

matics in almost all the developmental projects.

Behavior modification in the minds of the pupils was effected by

placing emphasis on logical thinking and structure instead of drill

and rote. These concepts as described in Table 1 were introduced

through different terminologies by different projects. The UICSM

Project introduced the term "discovery approach." The University of

Illinois Arithmetic Project stressed the ideas of thinking and con­

cept development. The SMSG Project advocated a deeper understanding

of basic concepts, structure of mathematics, and precise and sophis­

ticated mathematical language.

The Madison Project recommended the unstructured task and the

discovery approach. The purpose of the Greater Cleveland Mathematics

Program was to develop a curriculum which could be presented in a

logical, articulated, and sequential manner. It focused on patterns

of relationships and the logical structure of mathematics. The BSU

Project also gave prominence to the structure of mathematics. Crea­

tive thinking and structure of mathematics went hand in hand in almost

all the developmental projects in the process of introducing new

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 10 mathematics.

There was a basic similarity in the outcomes of the various pro­

jects, although the different projects were not provided with any

common set of objectives prior to the formulating of the modern math­

ematics programs.

Toward Definitions

Schaaf (1964) made a list of factors common to all the innovative

projects and other activities: (a) increased emphasis on abstract

ideas, (b) increased attention to logical region, (c) the use of con­

temporary terminology, (d) the insistence upon precise language, and

(e) new "mathematical ideas" or "innovations of content."

The new ideas and content listed by Schaaf included the concepts

and language of sets, Venn diagrams, systems of enumeration, base

system, the real number system of algebra, modular arithmetic, ine­

qualities, relations and properties, functions, axioms and postulates

in geometry, the elements of logic, and the nature of measurement.

Ferguson (1964) pointed out that the modern mathematics programs

had built-in motivation factors in that students experience the thrill

of discovering mathematical principles, for example, in algebra seeing

structure, order, and beauty.

According to a comprehensive analysis of six major programs for

the elementary grade levels (Sherman, 1972), the common elements of

new mathematics were a numeration system, measurement, geometric

ideas, algebraic ideas, structure (laws and systems) sets, statistics,

probability, number theory, and logic. Kline (1973) identified a set

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 11 of 12 major elements pertaining to the concepts of new mathematics.

They were set theory, bases of number systems, the congruences, ine­

qualities, matrices, symbolic logic, Boolean algebra, relations and

functions, abstractions, structure, group and field, and statistics

and probability.

It was, therefore, conceivable that another analysis of modern

mathematics programs could produce another set of elements which were

not identical with the previous sets. Hence, for the purposes of this

study, the term "new mathematics" was used in referring to new devel­

opments in mathematics at the elementary, secondary, and post-secondary

levels. The term applied to both new subject matter and new approaches

to topics, such as those discussed above. "Traditional mathematics"

referred to the content and teaching method prior to the introduction

of the developmental programs.

The Statement of the Problem

"There can be little question that following a fifteen year

period of unprecedented favor and prominence in the curricula of

American schools, mathematics teaching today faces the future with

less certainty of its goals and less optimism about its potential

effectiveness," observed Hill (1969, p. 440), who chaired the

National Advisory Committee on Mathematics Education.

The uncertainty about mathematics curricula stems from strong

public criticisms and from the abundance of research findings of

which some are dissimilar while others are conflicting in nature

(Norland, 1971; Yasui, 1967). Because of the dissimilar and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 12 conflicting nature of study findings, decision making at the levels

of resource allocation, curriculum development, and classroom teaching

becomes difficult and often misguided. Moreover, the money, time, and

thought expanded on new mathematics have been considerable— perhaps

even enormous (Kline, 1973).

These facts about the conflicting nature of research findings

and public opinions about the outcomes of new mathematics, coupled

with the scarcity of summative research resolving dissimilar and con­

flicting findings, formulate the problem for which this study was

intended.

Significance of the Study

The conflicting nature of past findings made the study highly

relevant and important. The study, which was a meta-analysis— an

analysis of analyses— was precisely designed to resolve outcomes which

were not in agreement with each other. Conclusive findings about the

outcomes of modern mathematics were focused on three major areas.

These areas were (a) the subject— the importance of an appropriate

mathematics curriculum, (b) the methodology— the utilization of a

meta-analysis technique to discover the message of the new mathematics

in the midst of conflicting findings, and (c) the two levels of deci­

sion making— balanced resource allocation decisions and well-founded

curriculum decisions yielding increased learning.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 13 Research Questions

In order to summarize the effects of new mathematics and to exa­

mine whether or not the new mathematics curriculum led to improved

academic achievement, the research sought answers to the following

questions:

1. Did the aggregation of studies show any evidence of change

in academic achievement attributable to new mathematics programs?

This question was concerned with the overall impact of the modern

mathematics as evidenced by the mean effect size of achievement when

all achievement studies were combined.

2. Did the aggregation of studies show any evidence of atti-

tudinal change attributable to new mathematics when compared to tradi­

tional mathematics programs? This was concerned with the grand mean

effect size of attitude toward mathematics when all attitudinal stud­

ies were combined.

3. What was the differential effect of the new mathematics pro­

grams with respect to the following variables?

Subject characteristics:

Grade Socio-economic status Ability Sex

Treatment factors:

General content area Specific subject area Type of program Length of treatment Number of subjects per study

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 14 Study variables:

Study type Study approach Author Source of study Affiliation of researcher Involvement of researcher General subject area

Design variables:

Type of test Quality of design Novelty effect

Topics Excluded

In addition to the previously discussed projects, there were

other developmental efforts that introduced innovative concepts in

mathematics education. Some of these projects were excluded from this

study. Primarily, they fell into one of the three following catego­

ries .

1. In the first category were projects which pertained to pro­

grammed instruction, individual learning packets, tutoring, and com­

puter assisted instruction. The emphasis of these projects was

purely on the methodology of teaching with little or no relationship

to the new mathematics concepts as discussed in the definitions.

2. In the second category of excluded studies were those which

posed a problem of definition. They pervaded the area of cognitive

learning styles, pedagogical techniques, and teacher training. Exis­

tential approach, self supervision, hierarchical components, modeling

theory, and resource personnel were some of them.

3. In the third category were the studies which were strictly

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 15 within the area of new mathematics, but were excluded for technical

reasons, such as inadequate methodology, inappropriate design, and

lack of pertinent quantitative data which were necessary for the pur­

poses of comparison.

Organization of Dissertation

Chapter I discussed the purpose and significance of the study.

It traced the evolution of modern mathematics and the role of the

developmental projects. The chapter also addressed itself to the

definition of terms, statement of the problem, research questions,

and the topics excluded.

Chapter II reviews research studies on modern mathematics and

techniques of aggregation. The section on research studies on modern

mathematics has been subdivided into three categories: (a) appraisals

of modern mathematics, (b) research studies on modern mathematics by

primary analysis, and (c) research studies on modern mathematics by

aggregate analysis.

Chapter III presents detailed description of the concept of

meta-analysis, sources of the study, methods of locating and collect­

ing studies, the process of identifying and coding the variables, and

the techniques of measuring effect sizes. It also deals with the

question of multiple effect sizes and the method of analysis of data.

Chapter IV reports the results of the data analysis. The results

are in two major categories: (a) the overall effects and (b) the

specific effects by different groups of variables, such as subject

variables, treatment variables, study variables, and design variables.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 16

Chapter V interprets the results of the study reported in

Chapter IV, discusses the implications and the major recommendations

based upon the research data, and gives a summary of the study.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTER II

RATIONALE FOR THE STUDY

The purpose of the chapter is to present the logical basis for

the study through the review of the literature which focuses upon the

area of the investigation. In this light, it may be expedient to

devote a separate section of the chapter to the justification of the

methodology, since the study utilized a new technique of aggregation—

a meta-analysis procedure— as its research tool. Subsequently, the

rationale for the study focuses upon two categories of topics: (a)

research studies on modern mathematics and (b) the techniques of

aggregation.

Research Studies on Modern Mathematics

To begin then, the effects of the new mathematics curriculum

were reported in literally hundreds of research studies. A comprehen­

sive treatment of the total studies conducted was not attempted in

this chapter due to the number of studies being prohibitively large;

however, a summary description of the studies on modern mathematics

is given in the ensuing sections. The description consists of three

parts: (a) appraisals of modern mathematics, (b) studies of modern

mathematics by primary analysis, and (c) studies of modern mathematics

by aggregate analysis.

1-7

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 18 Appraisals of Modern Mathematics

Mathematics educators were divided in their opinions, points of

view, and observations regarding the efficacy of the new mathematics

programs. Supporters of modern mathematics advocated the relevance,

appropriateness, and the distinct advantages of the new programs.

Fehr (1953) urged the adoption of the new mathematics because of

its relevance to understanding the universe. In the same vein,

MacLane (1954), Hartung (1955), Price (1957), Kennedy (1961), Mueller

(1962), and Davis (1963) advocated the introduction of new mathematics

programs at the elementary and secondary levels in order to meet the

changing needs created by the advancement of science and technology.

Writers have also observed more specific advantages of the new

programs. For example, Clark (1961) pointed out that according to the

findings of Begle, Glennon, Gundlach, and others, the new mathematics

programs permitted and even encouraged children to learn more mathe­

matics at an earlier age. Similarly, Grossnickle (1964) suggested

that the advantage of modern mathematics was in the introduction of

new and more profound topics at all grade levels. Furthermore, some

writers were of the opinion that slow learners benefitted more through

increased learning when utilizing new mathematics programs rather than

the traditional programs (Cunningham, 1965).

Conversely, the opponents of the new mathematics programs

expressed different and opposing points of view. For example,

Rosskopf (1954) objected to the new movement for a complete reorgani­

zation from traditional to contemporary mathematics instruction

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 19

primarily because he believed very few teachers were competent to

teach it. Similarly, Pingry (1956) and Kline (1959) criticized the

outright revision of the mathematics content as the proper approach

for improving student learning. Additionally, Kline (1958) echoed

these sentiments when he said, "The modern mathematics stressed during

the past few years is trivial in importance compared to topics cur­

rently taught and is hopelessly beyond young people, because of its

abstractness and rigor" (p. 422).

About the same time other writers like Hannon (1959) believed

that the traditional approach to teaching and the related materials

had to form the basic core of the elementary curriculum. Several

years later Rappaport (1962) expressed alarm over the haste with which

untrained educators desired to teach modern mathematics. Also,

Rappaport held a concern for the impracticality of the new math pro­

grams for the majority of the children.

It was apparent from the preceding sampling of viewpoints that

there were conflicting appraisals regarding the worth of the modern

mathematics programs. Again, the primary purpose for the study was

to seek a resolution to the maze of conflicting and dissimilar find­

ings about modern mathematics as illustrated by the examples which

were cited.

Studies on Modern Mathematics by Primary Analysis

A review of the research on modern mathematics revealed two

important aspects pertaining to the nature of the studies. One, in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 20

attempting to study the effectiveness of new mathematics programs,

investigators have adopted diverse methodologies for trying to gain a

clearer perspective. Two, in spite of several attempts to gain a

clear perspective, the study findings showed sharp contrasts and dis­

similarities. The ensuing section seeks to point out the conflicts

and disagreements among the study findings of the several different

designs.

The most common methodologies utilized to study new mathematics

ranged from simple descriptions to true experimental research designs.

For example, Davis (1965) and Johntz (1967) used the descriptive proc­

ess to investigate the effectiveness of the Madison Project. They

concluded that the creative classroom experience of the new programs

was superior to the traditional mathematics experience. Most of the

studies compared the new programs with traditional ones by reviewing

data from tests given to both experimental and control groups.

Biddle (1967) used a course in programmed instruction and Berger and

Howitz (1967), a discovery program for general mathematics students.

After one year of study, both investigations found that there was "no

significant difference" between the experimental and control groups.

Some researchers compared the effects of the experimental and

the traditional programs over a longer period of time. Hungerman

(1967) found that the School Mathematics Study Group (SMSG) students

were superior to traditional students on a conventional test. Graft

and Ruddell (.1968) found a significant difference favoring the SMSG

students in understanding, thought process, and transfer of learning.

Several studies compared the effectiveness of the treatment at

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 21

various levels in relation to factors such as ability, intelligence,

and sex. Williams (1963) found no significant difference in achieve­

ment, in grade nine, between the traditional algebra text and the

SMSG materials when used by average, high-average, and high ability

students. Shuff (1962) noted that at the upper and middle ability

levels, the students who received the traditional mathematics treat­

ment in the seventh grade made better achievement gains than the stu­

dents who received the SMSG instruction. Conversely, at the lower

level, the SMSG students performed better than the traditional group.

Peck (1963) divided the sixth grade students studying modern

mathematics and traditional mathematics into two groups. The high

intelligent group consisted of students with IQ’s of 115 or more and

the low intelligent group consisted of students with IQ's less than

115. The findings indicated that there was no significant difference

in achievement between the students who received modern mathematics

and traditional mathematics at the high IQ levels or at the low IQ

levels. About the same time, Harshmon (1962) made a comparison of

programs, using modern manipulative materials and traditional mate­

rials. Groups receiving each program were subdivided into three

intelligence levels: (a) those with IQ's of 99 and below, (b) those

with IQ's between 100 and 125, and (c) those with IQ's of 125 and

above. The results significantly favored the traditional program in

the IQ subgroup of 100-125, but the opposite was true for the IQ

ranges of 125 and above and 99 and below.

Wozencraft (1963) carried out a comprehensive study to determine

the difference between boys and girls in arithmetic ability. She

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 22 found that there was a significant difference in arithmetic ability

which favored the girls in total and average groups of children; how­

ever, the groups with high or low intelligence quotients did not show

significant sex differences. Northcutt (1964) examined the compara­

tive achievement of fifth grade boys and girls in two teaching

approaches, a traditional approach and a programmed (modern) approach.

He found no significant difference in gain between the sexes using

either approach.

Longitudinal studies were yet another category of research in

this area. Yasui (1967) reported that the mean achievement score of

modern mathematics was significantly higher than the mean score of

students of traditional mathematics. Conversely, the findings of

Osborne (1965) revealed that the introduction of SMSG materials and

their study for increasing period of time had "no significant effect"

on arithmetic skills, algebra skills, and mathematics reasoning skills.

The findings of Austin and Prevost (1972) confirmed Osborne's findings,

but contradicted those of Yasui: After a four year experiment, the

traditional mathematics group scored higher than the transitional and

modern mathematics groups on the Metropolitan Achievement Test of

Mathematics Computation.

In another longitudinal study, Norland (1971) observed that after

four years of instruction in modern mathematics, achievement in arith­

metic computation declined significantly in four of the five schools

which participated in the study. Conversely, in 1973 the National

Assessment of Educational Progress reported quite different findings.

One question about the new mathematics programs of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 23

1960's has been their impact on pupil's ability to compute .... But the evidence, limited though it may be, sug­ gests that Johnny can add: computation with whole numbers is far from being a lost art. The performance of the thir­ teen year olds on each of the four operations with the whole numbers was almost as good as the performance of the seventeen year olds and the adults .... The thirteen year old's responses indicate that computational algorithms for all four operations are well developed by the latter part of elementary school. (p. 457)

Thus, the findings of the research studies fell into three cate­

gories: (a) findings showing no significant difference between the

traditional and modern mathematics groups, (b) findings favoring the

traditional mathematics, and (c) findings favoring the modern mathe­

matics. The findings, therefore, were so conflicting and dissimilar

in nature that one could not make a sound generalization about the

effects of new mathematics programs without a further investigation

into the results of those studies conducted with a more appropriate

technique of analysis.

Studies on Modern Mathematics by Aggregate Analysis

Studies on modern mathematics by aggregate analysis were exceed­

ingly small in number. Some attempts were made in the generic area

of mathematics to aggregate the studies and classify them under one

or two major categories according to source, topic, or period of pub­

lication; however, no analysis was made in any of the attempts to

summarize the findings or draw any conclusions. Several of these

attempts are summarized in the following paragraphs.

In the April 1957 issue of the Arithmetic Teacher, a bibliography

appeared which included six years of research on arithmetic teaching

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 24 (1951-1956). The annual listing of research pertaining to elementary

school mathematics continued in the Arithmetic Teacher for the next

14 years. In 1971 the Journal of Research in Mathematic Education

began publishing a comprehensive listing of research which had been

conducted in elementary and secondary mathematics education.

School Science and Mathematics began publishing listings of per­

tinent research from published sources and dissertations from 1965 to

1972 (Burns & Dessart, 1965, 1966; Mangram & Knight, 1970, 1971, 1972;

Mangram & Morris, 1968, 1969; Summers & Hubrig, 1965, 1968).

In 1972, Suydam published a Review of Research on Secondary

School Mathematics. The review consisted of 780 research reports and

770 dissertations published between 1930 and 1970. The combined 1,550

documents were each categorized and annotated, but no effort was made

in the reviews to summarize the findings nor draw any conclusions.

The annual updates of the review's original bibliography appeared in

the subsequent editions of the Journal for Research in Mathematics

Education (Suydam & Weaver, 1973, 1974, 1975, 1976, 1977).

Collectively, the published annual bibliographies were— for the

purposes of this study— the most valuable source for locating the

majority of previous research studies. At the same time, it should be

noted that no attempt was made in any of the publications to analyze

the aggregate results of the research.

The first known attempt to analyze and synthesize the results of

studies pertaining to selected areas in the teaching of arithmetic was

made by Bartram in 1956. Some of the relevant areas of his investiga­

tion were problem-solving procedures, general teaching methods,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 25

instructional materials, and provision for individual needs. Bartram

selected 200 studies out of the 600 which were available. Bartram's

criteria for selection of the 200 studies were (a) the restriction of

experimental factors; (b) the control of variations in pupil factors;

(c) the control of nonexperimental factors, such as instructional

methods and materials; (d) the validity and reliability of the meas­

uring instrument; (e) the adequacy of the sampling technique; and (f)

the statistical significance of the findings. The selection criteria,

employed by Bartram, especially statistical significance, resulted in

the exclusion of several studies which contained valuable information.

Bartram followed three procedures in treating each area: (a)

identification and analysis of points of view concerning the area,

(b) detailed presentation of research, and (c) forming conclusions and

suggesting teaching practices. His more significant conclusions in

these areas are summarized as follows:

1. Arithmetic problem solving was learned better through the

medium of an activity program than through one more rigidly organized;

also, computational skills were learned equally well. In addition,

activity programs contributed more to the child's social and emotional

adjustment.

2. Inductive methods were superior to deductive for teaching

process and generalizations.

3. Children profitted from meaningful practice. Such practice

should be motivated by a social need and should follow the acquisition

of understanding.

4. A combination of the activity method and grouping within the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 26 classroom appeared to be the most satisfactory means of meeting the

needs of the individual children.

Bartram's (1956) research focused on a critical examination of

teaching practices, rather than on an evaluation of a new program.

Consequently, Bartram's findings were not directly related to the

present study.

A second effort was undertaken by Hartley in 197 7, as mentioned

in Chapter I. Hartley (1977) collected and synthesized 153 experi­

mental studies which pertained to the efficacy of four techniques of

mathematics instruction. Hartley utilized a meta-analysis methodology

for conducting an aggregate analysis of the results.

The four techniques Hartley examined were computer-assisted

instruction, cross-age and peer tutoring, individual learning packets,

and programmed instruction. The effectiveness of each technique was

determined by comparing the resulting mathematics achievement of stu­

dents taught by the particular technique with the achievement of stu­

dents taught by a traditional method of instruction. In addition to

teaching methodology, other variables such as design characteristics,

subject variables, and attributes of instructional technique were

quantified. The resulting relationships to effectiveness of each

technique were also examined.

Briefly, Hartley found, on the basis of the studies collected,

that tutoring was the superior technique for increasing mathematics

achievement. Peer tutoring, in which classmates tutor one another,

was as effective as paid adult aides; however, cross-age tutoring, in

which older students tutor younger students, was slightly better than

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 27 the peer and adult tutoring. Also, Hartley discovered that intensive

supervision and instruction of the student tutors did not result in

an increase in effectiveness, although some instruction was beneficial.

In addition, Hartley noted that computer-assisted instruction was

less effective than tutoring in increasing the effectiveness of mathe­

matics instruction, although it was considerably more effective than

either individual learning packets or programmed instruction. Indi­

vidual learning packets and programmed instruction were essentially

comparable to traditional instruction; that is, these two techniques

resulted in little or no achievement gain over traditional instruction.

In many cases, traditional instruction was definitely superior to

individual learning packets and programmed instruction.

The Hartley (1977) study was a later off-shoot of the innovative

programs in mathematics and it did not strictly come under the defini­

tion of new mathematics given in Chapter I. Consequently, the results

of the study were not directly applicable to the purposes of the

present study.

It was clear, therefore, that no comprehensive attempt had been

made to review the large volume of research studies on the effective­

ness of modern mathematics by any aggregate technique of analysis.

The present study attempted to utilize an appropriate technique of

analysis to resolve the conflicting and dissimilar findings on modern

mathematics programs.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 28

Techniques of Aggregation

One of the early techniques of aggregation of studies was the

narrative. The narrative was mainly a chronologically arranged verbal

description; however, this technique failed to portray the accumulated

knowledge of large numbers of studies due to the cumbersome length of

the resulting narrative. As a consequence, the review of literature

was created as an improved technique of extracting information from

groups of studies.

The review of literature usually involved three processes. They

were: (a) gathering the relevant empirical studies, (b) discarding

studies with inadequate empirical designs, and (c) drawing conclusions

from the remaining studies. In order to draw conclusions, the

reviewers often made use of a listing of studies with a tally kept of

the number of significant and nonsignificant results. Conclusions

favored the results found in the larger number of studies in case of

dissimilar or conflicting findings.

One of the drawbacks of the review of the literature was that it

did not take into account the influence of the variables that could

affect the results of a study in a systematic way. For example, some

of these variables were the quality of design, ability level, age and

sex of subjects, length of treatment, and date and source of study. A

second drawback was that charts and tables were often used by

reviewers to differentiate the results; however, when encountering

large numbers of studies, the charts and tables proved to be inadequate

means to fulfill the purpose.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 29

A third drawback of the review of research was that it often

eliminated the "poorly done" studies. Glass (1976) said that it was

an empirical question whether the so-called "poorly done" studies

gave results significantly at variance with those of the best designed

studies. He believed that because the difference was so small, the

integration of research results through eliminating the "poorly done"

studies would result in discarding a vast amount of important data.

In addition to the narrative and the review of the literature,

other techniques of aggregation have been utilized for summarizing

accumulated data. Averaging, voting, and the cluster approach were

the three major methods Light and Smith (1971) explained in their

paper, "Accumulating Evidence." These three methods are briefly dis­

cussed in the ensuing paragraphs.

The Averaging Method

The averaging method was a more systematic approach than mere

listing and simple narratives of the studies. The approach consisted

of computing overall means for relevant statistics for a large number

of studies. The measures of central tendency were used to compute the

overall average which was used in the averaging method. The method

could be considered to be the first attempt of quantification, but the

weakness was that it threw away precisely the information needed most,

namely, a true estimate of the treatment effect.

The Voting Method

A more systematic and widely used approach to aggregation was the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 30 voting method. In the approach, all studies showing a relationship

between a particular dependent variable and an independent variable

were examined for their significance. The studies were classified

under one of three categories of relationship— significantly positive,

significantly negative, or neither significantly positive nor signif­

icantly negative. Tallies were made for each of the three categories.

The category that had the maximum number of tallies was assumed to

give the best estimate of the direction of the true relationship

between the two variables under consideration.

The weakness in the method was threefold. First, valuable

descriptive information was disregarded. Secondly, the method did not

integrate measures to determine the strength of experimental effects

or relationships among variables (according to whether the problem

was basically experimental or correlational). Third, aggregation by

voting did not take into account the possibility of Simpson’s Paradox

(Glass, 1977). Perhaps an example would illustrate the paradox.

Assume two studies on the effects of individualized instruction

compared with the traditional method were made in regard to achieve­

ment. In Study I, 50 students received individualized instruction

and 40 students received traditional instruction. In Study II 50 sub­

jects were in the individualized instruction group and 300 were in the

traditional instruction group. Tables 2 and 3 show the number of

passes, failures, and the pass rates in each study. The pass rate

for individualized instruction exceeded the traditional by 42% versus

40% in Study I. In Study II the pass rate for individualized instruc­

tion exceeded the traditional by 56% versus 55%. Utilizing the voting

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 31

method, the score would be 2-0 in favor of individualized instruction;

however, an opposite result would be attained if one aggregated the

raw data.

Table 2

Study I

Individualized Traditional Total

Pass 21 16 37

Failure 29 24 53

Total 50 40 90

Pass Rate 42% 40%

Table 3

Study II

Individualized Traditional Total

Pass 28 165 193

Failure 22 135 157

Total 50 300 350

Pass Rate 56% 55%

In Table 4 the pass rate was reversed. The traditional exceeded

the individualized instruction by 53% versus 49%. The cause of this

paradoxical situation was the problem of unbalanced experimental

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 32

design. Since the voting method did not pay attention to the sample

size, there existed the possibility of misinterpretation of results.

Table 4

Studies I and II Combined

Individualized Traditional Total

Pass 49 181 230

Failure 51 159 210

Total 100 340 440

Pass Rate 49% 53%

The Cluster Approach

As a more refined technique, the cluster approach had been elab­

orated upon by Light and Smith (1971) in an attempt to devise a tech­

nique for resolving contradictions among different studies. The

approach stemmed from a pragmatic insight that many populations could

be broken down into small, identifiable subpopulations which were

called clusters. The clusters were not random samples from a popula­

tion. Rather, they were natural aggregations within the population.

The clusters usually differed in broad and systematic ways. Thus, a

study could contain several clusters according to the various aggrega­

tions within the population.

The main difference between the cluster approach and the voting

method was in the selection of the unit of analysis. In the voting

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 33

method the unit of analysis was a complete study. Conversely, in the

cluster approach the unit of analysis was a cluster which could be a

part of a study or a complete study.

The primary steps involved in the method of aggregation consisted

of access to the original data, review of the quality of the studies,

tests for differences among clusters, and classification and combina­

tion of the studies according to the availability of the explanation

of differences.

Figure 1 gives an abbreviated overview of the basic logic of the

cluster approach. The first block identified the five kinds of cluster

differences for which one tested. If all the five tests failed to find

differences, data from the several clusters could be combined. If few

of the tests indicated differences, an explanation of these differ­

ences was sought. The data were combined after adjustments had been

made to eliminate the differences. If no explanation was found for

the differences, data could not be combined.

The cluster approach presented problems in its application.

Since the cluster approach required access to the raw data, the study

became overly restrictive. The approach also eliminated several

studies because of the statistical and methodological restrictions.

The subjects had to be selected from a known and precisely definable

population. The dependent variables and the independent variables

which were measured had to have a common measure or be subject to

suitable conversion methods. The overall quality of the studies

selected had to be comparable. Another serious problem was the

inability to draw any conclusion in the absence of explanations for

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 34

Test for differences among clusters in: (a) Means (b) Variances (c) Covariate relations (d) Subject-treatment interactions (e) Contextual effects differences found

One or more differences found

Search for explanation: (a) Selection effects (b) Amplification effects (c) Sensitization (d) Different proportions of types of subjects in clusters (e) Contextual differences (f) Unmeasured variables (g) Other

No explanation found Explanation found Adjust away explained cluster differences

Cannot Combine combine data data from from cluster clusters

Figure 1. Overview of the cluster approach. (From "Accumulating Evidence: Procedures for Resolving Contradictions Among Different Research Studies" by R. J. Light and P. V. Smith, Harvard Educational Review, 1971, _41, 463.)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 35

differences. These restrictive criteria, therefore led to a loss of

information.

Conversely, the present study attempted to include as many

empirical findings as possible in order to analyze and summarize the

voluminous body of research data pertaining to new mathematics cur­

ricula. The intended result was to determine the meaning of these

findings without the loss of information. In this regard, a meta­

analysis technique, as explained by Glass (1976), was adopted for the

study. The primary reason for utilizing meta-analysis was that it

diminished the weaknesses contained in the aforementioned techniques

in many respects. The concept of meta-analysis is thoroughly dis­

cussed in Chapter III.

Summary

Chapter II dealt with rationale for the present study. The

focus of the rationale was on research in modern mathematics and

techniques of aggregation. The section pertaining to research on

modern mathematics was subdivided into three areas: (a) appraisals

of modern mathematics, (b) studies on modern mathematics by primary

analysis, and (c) studies on modern mathematics by aggregate analysis.

The discussion on appraisals of modern mathematics revealed that there

existed conflicting viewpoints regarding the efficacy of modern math­

ematics. Since the studies on modern mathematics by primary analysis

were numerous and their findings were diverse, an aggregate analysis

was necessary to draw a conclusive meaning from the findings. The

number of studies on modern mathematics by aggregate analysis was

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 36

small. Among the few existing ones, none focused upon the area of

investigation of the present study.

In the discussion of the techniques of aggregation five methods

were identified. They were narrative, reviews, the averaging method,

the voting method, and the cluster approach. Strengths and weaknesses

of each technique were discussed. The examination of these techniques

revealed the necessity for a better technique of analysis for the pur­

poses of this study. The present attempt, therefore, was the first

aggregate analysis of the effects of modern mathematics by using a

meta-analysis technique.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTER III

DESIGN AND DATA ANALYSIS

The purpose of Chapter III is to describe the theoretical and

practical aspects of meta-analysis methodology. The theoretical

aspect consists mainly of the explanation of the effect size and the

techniques of calculating effect sizes. The practical aspect deals

with identifying the sources, locating and selecting the studies,

identifying and coding the variables, and the procedures for analyzing

the data.

Concept of Meta-analysis

An aggregate analysis was deemed to be appropriate to resolve

the dissimilar and conflicting nature of the study findings about the

new mathematics. Many of the techniques of aggregation used in the

past were inadequate for the purposes of the present study. A meta­

analysis procedure, introduced by Glass (1976), was found to be an

improvement on the techniques discussed in Chapter II.

In an attempt to explain the concept of meta-analysis, Glass

(1976) made a distinction among primary, secondary, and meta-analysis.

Primary analysis was defined as the original analysis of data in a

research study. Secondary analysis was thought of as the reanalysis

of data for the purposes of answering the original research question

with better statistical techniques. Also, secondary analysis could

be utilized for answering new questions with old data. Conversely,

37

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 38

meta-analysis was the analysis of analyses. In the present study,

meta-analysis, as defined by Glass, was adopted as an alternative

technique to analyze the aggregate findings on the effects of modern

mathematics.

The literature on meta-analysis consisted mainly of the writings

of Glass. Two of his papers which specifically focused on meta­

analysis were "Primary, Secondary, and Meta-analysis of Research"

(Glass, 1976) and "Integrating Findings: The Meta-analysis of

Research" (Glass, 1978). The former provided an overview of the meta­

analysis methodology and the latter pertained to the different tech­

niques involved in the meta-analysis procedure.

Glass explained meta-analysis as a statistical technique which

analyzed a large collection of analysis results from individual studies

for the purpose of integrating the findings. The method connoted a

rigorous alternative to the casual narrative discussions of research

studies which typified attempts to make sense out of the rapidly

expanding body of research literature. A special feature of Glass'

meta-analysis technique was that it statistically aggregated, rather

than merely listed, the results of a large number of studies. Also,

it used summary statistics instead of raw data for aggregation.

One of the advantages of meta-analysis was that conflicting or

dissimilar studies were not eliminated on the basis of a lack of

explanations for differences. Large numbers of conflicting studies

were combined to yield an overall conclusion by calculating what

Glass called the "effect size" (Glass, 1976). Theoretically, effect

size as used in this study was a standardized measure of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. effectiveness of the treatment when compared to an untreated control

group.

Effect Size

To elaborate, the key concept in Glass' meta-analysis method­

ology was the effect size. The effect sizes were calculated for each

study in a measure that was common for all studies. Then the meas­

ures were combined to yield more information than just a significant-

not-significant count. The result indicated the difference between

the effect of a treatment and its comparison treatment. Thus, the

effect size yielded information pertaining to the specific treatments

in the study. Specifically, each effect size measured how much more

or less effective the modern mathematics was than the traditional

mathematics counterpart.

Effect size was defined as the difference between the means of

the experimental and control groups, divided by the standard devia­

tion of the control group (Glass, 1978). The formula was as follows:

(mean of treatment) - (mean of control) Effect size (standard deviation of the control group) ' '

Symbolically, the formula can be written as:

X X E C ES S (2) X

where XE the mean of the experimental group

X C the mean of the control group

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 40

= the standard deviation of the control group

and ES = the effect size

A positive measure of effect size indicated that the findings

favored the treatment group while a negative measure indicated that

results supported the control group. For example, if the effect size

was k standard deviations, then an average person in the control

group was k standard deviations below the average person in the

treatment group.

By dividing the difference of means by the standard deviation,

the effect sizes were standardized and were readily converted into

z-score units. Consequently, the effect sizes were appropriate for

comparison even though they were derived from different studies.

Also, corresponding to each effect size, a percentile score could be

obtained from the table which indicated the areas and ordinates of

the unit normal distribution.

For example, let the effect size comparing the effectiveness of

two treatments— a traditional and a modern— be 0.49. Then, the value

0.49 indicated that an average person receiving the modern treatment

had been raised from the 50th percentile to the 69th percentile of

the traditional group, as depicted in Figure 2.

In Figure 2, the normal curve of the dotted lines represented

the control group or the traditional group. An average student in

this group occupied the midpoint of the baseline. This point coin­

cided with the zero point of the z-score. The average person, there­

fore, was said to be at the 50th percentile of this normal curve,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 41

CONTROL GROUP TREATMENT GROUP (TRADITIONAL) (MODERN)

50th Percentile 69th Percentile

Average Effect Size is 0.49

Figure 2. Normal curves illustrating the effect of modern treatment in relation to traditional treatment— a hypothetical illustration.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 42 which represented the traditional mathematics population. The normal

curve of the solid line represented the treatment group.

The assumed value of the effect size in Figure 2, namely 0.49,

being a measure of the difference between the means, was represented

by the distance between the midpoints of the two normal curves. The

midpoint of the curve which represented the modern group coincided

with the 69th percentile of the traditional group. (The percentile

value corresponding to the z-score of 0.49 was 69.)

Thus, given an effect size of 0.49, a person who was at the 50th

percentile of the modern group was, at the same time, at the 69th

percentile of the traditional group. In other words, the treatment

of the modern program under the average conditions could be expected

to move an average student from the 50th to 69th percentile of the

traditional mathematics population, if the mean effect size was 0.49.

The same explanation was applicable to all specific mean effect sizes,

except for the fact that the effect sizes were different in each par­

ticular case.

The mean effect size was a powerful tool in summarizing the

results. The findings could be summarized at the general as well as

specific levels into which the independent variables of this study

were categorized, by using various measures of mean effect size. At

the most general level, average of all the effect sizes were found.

This was called the "grand mean effect size" or the "overall mean

effect size." The terms "mean" and "average" were used interchange­

ably.

In the present study, modern mathematics population represented

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 43 the treatment group and the traditional mathematics population, the

control group. The outcome variable was either achievement or atti­

tude measured in terms of effect size.

The hypothetical effect sizes of three different subject areas

are compared in Figure 3. In all three cases, a modern mathematics

group was compared with the traditional mathematics group. Since the

traditional mathematics group was not given any treatment in modern

mathematics, it was considered to be an untreated control group whose

average effect size was always zero. The four normal curves, there­

fore, represented the typical three treated populations in relation

to the untreated control subjects in the subject areas of arithmetic,

algebra, and geometry. For example, 102 effect size measures from

modern algebra studies averaged about 1.18 standard deviation.

The major impression derived from Figure 3 was that modern alge­

bra and geometry were not substantially different from each other in

their average impact on learning. Also, one could deduce that both

modern algebra and geometry groups performed better than the tradi­

tional mathematics group.

In addition, data from Figure 3 illustrated that the children

who received the modern arithmetic treatment performed worse than the

traditional mathematics group. Thus, the various measures of effect

size illustrated the comparative effectiveness of different modern

mathematics treatments among themselves as well as their comparative

effectiveness with the traditional mathematics treatment.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. TRADITIONAL EFFECT = 0 MATHEMATICS 50th NUMBER OF EFFECT Percentile SIZES = 150

MODERN EFFECT =1.18 ALGEBRA NUMBER OF EFFECT centile SIZES = 102

MODERN EFFECT =1.28 GEOMETRY 90th NUMBER OF EFFECT Perce itile SIZES = 205

MODERN EFFECT =0.25 ARITHMETIC NUMBER OF EFFECT SIZES = 180 /40 th Peroentile

Figure 3. Normal curves illustrating the effects of three different treatments of modern mathematics (modern algebra, modern geometry, and modern arithmetic) in relation to untreated traditional group— a hypothetical illustration.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 45 Techniques of Measuring Effect Sizes

Different study designs required different techniques to measure

the numerator and the denominator in the equation of the effect size.

Glass (1978) discussed many techniques in his paper, "Integrating

Findings: The Meta-analysis of Research." In the ensuing sections

the techniques which were used for the present study are enumerated.

Techniques for determining the numerator were as follows.

1. The numerator of a typical experiment (in which the subjects

were randomly assigned to treatment and control groups, or the pre­

test scores indicated that the groups were equivalent) was the differ­

ence between the means of the posttests. The pretest scores did not

enter into the computation.

2. If the design made use of analysis of covariance, and if the

adjusted means were reported, then the numerator was the difference

between the adjusted means.

3. If a design reported the gain scores— pretest scores sub­

tracted from the posttest scores— then the numerator of the effect

size was the difference between the averages of the two gain scores.

4. If a design reported only grade equivalent scores from a

, then the numerator was calculated by the length of

the treatment. This procedure assumed that theoretically an untreated

average group should gain one point for each month in the school.

Thus, if a study indicated a pre-post difference of 2.0 grade equiv­

alents after the treatment of eight months, the numerator of the

effect size was 2.0 - (1 x 0.8) = 1.2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 46 Correspondingly, the methods for determining the denominator of

the effect size were as follows.

1. Whenever it was possible to obtain the standard deviation of

the control group, then that was used as the denominator of the effect

size.

2. If only the means based on scores from a standardized test

were given, the standard deviation of the norm group was taken as the

denominator of the effect size.

3. If only the t-statistic was available, then effect size was

derived from the formula:

1 1 ES n + n (3) 1 2

where n = the number of subjects in the control group 1 the number of subjects in the treatment n2 = group

The equation was derived from the formula (2) given earlier and

from the equation for the t-statistic, which was given by the formula:

X E t (4)

Ct * Formula (2) was:

ES

By combining formulas (2) and (4), formula (3) was derived.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 47 4. If a report contained only the information that the mean of

the experimental subjects exceeded the mean of the control subjects

at certain Of-level significance, then an approximate value of the

effect size was calculated, assuming the t-value for the particular

©(-level. For example, if Gf = .05, then the most conservative value

of t was equal to 1.96. The value 1.96 was obtained from the table

showing the percentile points of t-distributions in standard textbooks

dealing with statistics. By substituting this value for t in formula

(3) the effect size was obtained from the following formula:

5. If the report indicated only an F-value and there were only

two groups, the positive square root of F had to be substituted for t.

Glass (1978) discussed other techniques, such as probit analysis

and logit transformation, in his paper, "Integrating Findings: The

Meta-analysis of Research." However, these techniques were not

required in the present study since all of the data reported were

readily convertible to effect size by the previously discussed tech­

niques .

Multiple Effect Size

In the process of calculating effect sizes, care was taken to

prevent the problem of multiple effect size. The problem of multiple

effect size arose when a study had more than one effect size. When

a study had more than one effect size, certain effect sizes could be

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 48

repeated either in part or whole. For instance, if a study dealt

with two grade levels, then effect sizes could be found for each

grade level separately and for the entire group combining the two

grades. Thus, three effect sizes were possible from a study consist­

ing of two grade levels. If all three effect sizes were considered

for the grand mean effect size, the study would be contaminated by

the multiple effect size, because the effect sizes of each grade level

and the effect size of the combined group represented the same value.

The problem could be avoided by choosing either the two effect sizes

obtained from each grade levels— one from each grade— or the effect

size of the combined group, but not both sets.

The problem of multiple effect sizes became more acute when the

design utilized was a factorial study involving sex or ability. For

example, if a study included four grades and sex as a factor, then it

would have four effect sizes on grade, one for each grade, and two on

sex. In the overall analysis of studies, either the first four effect

sizes or the last two would be included, and not both sets.

Sources of the Studies

The success of meta-analysis depended on collecting large num­

bers of research and evaluation studies from as many sources as

available. The first task, therefore, was to identify the sources

where studies had been done in the area of interest. Accordingly, a

wide variety of sources were examined in the search for appropriate

studies to be utilized in the meta-analysis. Information pertaining

to potential studies to be included was obtained primarily from the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 49

reference entitled Dissertation Abstracts. Index to Education, Edu­

cation Resources Information Center (ERIC) documents, mathematics

education and psychological journals, research reviews, and govern­

ment publications were also helpful in providing information about

research studies which pertained to the area under investigation.

Locating and Selecting Studies

The sources helped advance progress toward the general goal of

conducting a meta-analysis— a quantitative aggregation of a large

number of research studies. After the primary sources of studies

were identified, the next task in the process was to locate specific

studies which pertained to new mathematics. This was accomplished

through a systematic and thorough search of the key sources— Index to

Education, Dissertation Abstracts Index, and ERIC for the years from

1951 to 1977. To systematize the process of locating and collecting

studies, the researcher adopted certain criteria for study selection.

The studies had to satisfy all criteria.

Broadly, the criteria for study selection were divided into pri­

mary and secondary. The primary set of criteria were the following:

(a) the studies had to be experimental as opposed to descriptive or

theoretical, (b) the dependent variables were to be limited to achieve­

ment or attitude, (c) tests of achievement or attitude were either

standardized or experimenter developed or teacher developed, and (d)

the area of experimentation had to be within the limits of the range

of elements discussed in the definition of new mathematics in Chapter I.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 50

The primary set of criteria was intended to select studies from

abstracts and summaries of research studies in the areas of investi­

gation; however, the criteria were not adequate to detect other

characteristics which were necessary for the final selection of

studies. Hence, a set of secondary criteria were established for the

final selection process. The secondary criteria were the following:

(a) studies comparing two or more groups had a control group which

received traditional mathematics treatment, (b) studies adopted ade­

quate method of analysis, and (c) studies related to the concepts

introduced into mathematics instruction during the 1950's and 1960's.

Once the criteria for selection were established, the search for

locating the studies started. The search was conducted in two stages.

Initially, a variety of approaches was utilized to identify studies

from different sources. For example, separate computer searches

were made of the ERIC documents and Dissertation Abstracts by using

appropriate descriptors. The searches yielded approximately 300

studies which were potentially usable.

In addition to dissertations and ERIC, a genuine attempt was

made to locate and collect studies or results of studies from text­

book publishers. Parenthetically, textbook publishers who played an

active role in the production and promotion of modern mathematics

materials frequently conducted research on the methods and techniques

incorporated within their textbooks. The publishers were identified

from the list of modern mathematics in EL-HI Textbooks in Print 1978.

Unfortunately, the search elicited responses which were predominantly

unusable.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 51 The search for studies on modern mathematics programs from

developmental projects was revealing on several dimensions. One,

many of the developmental projects were no longer in existence.

Among those which are in operation, ironically, no project indicated

that it possessed any studies which measured the effectiveness of

that particular program. A few of the research centers, such as the

Ohio State University ERIC Center for Science-Mathematics and Envi­

ronmental Education, proved to be useful in citing some of the rele­

vant studies and locating their sources. Approximately 50 studies

were located through this source.

At the end of the search through research centers, the investi­

gator made a thorough search of the Dissertation Abstracts. In the

Dissertation Abstracts more than 300 studies were identified which

pertained to the area of investigation. Ninety-two dissertations

were selected from the 300 studies for meta-analysis in accordance

with the primary criteria. Copies of the 92 dissertations were

obtained from the University Microfilms, Ann Arbor, Michigan.

As mentioned, a second search was completed for the additional

studies. The search was based on the information gathered from the

bibliographies and references of the previous set of studies. In the

second search, 68 additional dissertation studies were identified and

the copies of these dissertations were obtained from the University

Microfilms, Ann Arbor, Michigan.

Many additional studies were located through a variety of

sources. Some of them were identified in more than one source.

Eliminating the repetitious studies, more than 200 studies were

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 52 originally identified through the brief descriptions given in the

form of abstracts or summaries as potentially providing relevant data

for the present analysis. The identification of these studies was

accomplished by the primary set of criteria mentioned earlier.

While the investigator reviewed the original studies, it was

necessary to eliminate those which met all the primary criteria, but

did not satisfy some of the secondary criteria. The studies which

were excluded fell into one of the following categories.

1. Studies comparing two or more groups which received new

mathematics treatments, but did not have a control group which

received traditional treatment. For example, a study which compared

a guided discovery group with a hinted discovery group belonged to

this category.

2. Studies which adopted an inadequate method of analysis. A

study which showed the comparative effect only by the difference in

percentage of pass or failure of the two groups typified this cate­

gory.

3. Studies which belonged to the later developments in mathe­

matics instruction were also excluded. This category included studies

pertaining to the existential approach, self-supervision, individual­

ized instruction, television-aided instruction, computer-assisted

instruction, etc.

In the final screening, 134 studies were selected for meta­

analysis from three different sources. The sources were the ERIC

documents, journals, and doctoral dissertations. Of the three

sources, doctoral dissertations contributed more than three-fourths

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 53 of the studies.

Identifying and Coding Variables

After the studies were selected through the processes of locating,

collecting, and screening, information about the individual studies

was extracted. Compilation of information about the studies was

carried out through an identification and coding process for the rele­

vant variables. Since meta-analysis research is concerned with the

description of the relationships among findings and the characteris­

tics of the studies, care was taken to identify as many characteris­

tics as possible and classify them as specific variables.

Identification of the variables was a continuous process. Prior

to reviewing each study in depth, the descriptive variables, such as

number of students, grade level, type of treatment, length of treat­

ment, year of study, rating, and the quality of design were identified.

Later these variables were classified under four categories: the

subject characteristics, treatment factors, study variables, and

design variables. Identification of a variable consisted of three

steps: (a) naming the variable, (b) defining the purpose the variable

serves, and (c) assigning values to the variable.

Originally, 31 variables were identified for coding. While

reviewing the studies, it was necessary to make modifications in the

original list of variables. Major modifications were introduced in

the range of values for certain variables. Two new variables were

added to the original list: the generic content area and the sub­

achievement area. Variables such as retention, time, transfer time,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 54 the dependent variable, the length of treatment per day, the length

of the treatment per minute, subachievement area, and new versus

field-tested, were not considered for analysis, since the number of

effect sizes in those cells was too small. The list of variables

and the coding sheet are presented in Appendices A and B, respectively.

The list of variables provided the name, purpose, and range of the

values, while the code sheet consisted of the code, the name of the

variable, the columns, and the values.

Coding the necessary information from the selected studies

involved a great deal of concentration. For convenience, the col­

lected studies were first of all arranged in a chronological order.

Then, each study was reviewed in its entirity to identify and assign

the proper values for the quantitative and descriptive characteris­

tics which were listed in the code sheet. The descriptive data were

spread throughout the study, while the quantitative data were mostly

located in chapters dealing with methodology and results. Occasion­

ally, tables of basic summary statistics, such as mean, standard

deviation, and sample size, were provided in the appendix.

Each code sheet contained one effect size and all other variables

related to the effect size. Consequently, the data in one code sheet

formed the unit of analysis. From most of the studies more than one

effect size was calculated. When there were many effect sizes in one

study, the photostat copies of the pages in the original studies,

which contained the necessary data for the calculation of effect

sizes, were obtained. The copies were attached with the respective

sets of code sheets which comprised the study. The procedure

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 55 facilitated the calculation of effect sizes and further reference.

Analysis of Data

After the information about the studies was coded in a systematic

manner, the findings were analyzed. The analysis of the results of

the present study was expected to fall under four major categories.

The first category was the overall effect of new mathematics pro­

grams. This was divided into two subcategories: (a) the effect in

terms of achievement and (b) the effect in terms of attitude. The

overall effect size was measured by calculating the average effect

size of all the studies taken together.

The second category of results consisted of effect sizes which

were computed for each relevant independent variable. These effect

sizes were calculated to show the relative significance of the dif­

ferent characteristics under consideration.

The calculations involved in the computation of effect sizes

were completed with the use of a hand . The analysis of

data, such as the mean effect size, correlations, and regressions

were done by computer. The computer analysis cards were punched from

the code sheets. The data were entered into the computer from the

cards. The computer analysis was performed by using Statistical

Package and Bank Data Management Package, prepared and programmed by

Houchard (1974) for the Western Michigan University Computer Center.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Procedures for Judging the Magnitude and Significance of Effect Size

As previously discussed, the main tool for the analysis of the

results was the mean effect size. Consequently, judgments on the

results of the findings were heavily dependent upon the mean effect

sizes. In order to make sound judgments about the results, it was

necessary to have a clear perception of the magnitude and signifi­

cance of the mean effect sizes.

Procedures to judge the magnitude and significance of effect

size of a group of studies were not detailed in literature on meta­

analysis. The most obvious and direct meaning of the effect size was

the impact of a new treatment in terms of a shift from the mean point

toward the positive or negative direction of the unit normal curve.

The shift was measured in standard deviations which were readily con­

vertible to percentiles. Any effect size which corresponded to a

percentile greater than the 50th suggested a positive impact or an

improvement by the new treatment over the untreated group; con­

versely, any effect size which corresponded to a percentile less than

the 50th indicated a negative impact or a deterioration. The greater

the average effect size was, the greater the impact was.

To determine whether the effect size of a group of studies was

a chance factor or not, other procedures were necessary. In the

present study, t-values and the exact probabilities were calculated

corresponding to each average effect size of a group of studies.

This was done in order to determine the significance of the effect

size. The t-values were calculated under the assumptions that studies

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 57 on modern mathematics formed a sampling distribution and the hypo­

thetical mean of the traditional mathematics population was at the

zero point. Thus, the t-values were obtained from the formula:

(Effect size) - (Zero) t ~ /Standard error of mean of') (the effect sizes J

The degrees of freedom needed in addition to the t-value for the

calculation of the exact probability were obtained by subtracting one

from the number of effect sizes. Since the studies on modern mathe­

matics indicated positive as well as negative impacts, the probabil­

ities corresponding to the t-values were calculated for the two-

tailed test. The effect sizes were then considered to be significant

or not significant at a certain level of significance. The probabil­

ity of committing type I error for the present study was taken to be

0.05.

To help visualize more of the nature of the central tendency

and of the dispersion of the distribution of the effect sizes, the

values of the median and of the standard deviation were provided .

along with the average effect size.

Summary

The discussion of the methodology for the present study focused

mainly in two areas: the theoretical aspects of meta-analysis and

the practical steps involved in its implementation. The theoretical

section consisted mainly of an explanation of the concept of meta­

analysis, the effect size, and the description of different techniques

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 58 of measuring effect size. The practical steps involved in the imple­

mentation of meta-analysis consisted of identifying the source of the

study, locating and collecting studies, identifying and coding the

variables, and the analysis of the data.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTER IV

RESULTS

The purpose of Chapter IV is to present the results of the anal­

ysis of the data. The presentation of the results consists of a

brief description of the studies included and a detailed discussion

of the effects of the variables for which the effect sizes were cal­

culated. The effects were classified into five groups: (a) the over­

all effects of modern mathematics in achievement and attitude, (b) the

specific effects by subject characteristics, (c) the specific effects

by treatment factors, (d) the specific effects by study variables,

and (e) the specific effects by design variables.

Description of the Studies Included

A description of the studies was intended to give a picture of

the general nature of the studies included in the present analysis.

The factors that describe the general nature are the sources from

which the studies were collected, the year in which the studies were

published, the length of treatment of the studies, and the number of

subjects per study.

Source of Study

One hundred and thirty-four studies were identified from three

different sources. From the 134 studies, 810 effect sizes were cal­

culated. The exact number of studies and effect sizes corresponding

59

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 60 to each source is shown in Table 5.

Table 5

Studies Classified by Source

Number of Number of Source Studies Effect Sizes

ERIC Documents 4 39

Journal Articles 16 127

Doctoral Dissertations 114 644

Total 134 810

Year of Study

The studies utilized came from the period between 1951 and 1977.

The majority of the studies were conducted during the second half of

the 1960's. The median year for the studies and the effect sizes was

1968. In Table 6, the number of studies and the number of effect

sizes corresponding to each year are listed.

Length of Treatment and Number of Subjects Per Study

The length of treatment of studies ranged from 1 week to 144

weeks. The average length of treatment was approximately 33 weeks.

The number of subjects per study ranged from 4 to 2,862. The average

number of subjects per study was 108. In order to obtain a clearer

perspective of the central tendencies along with the mean of the two

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 61

Table 6

Studies Classified by Year

Year of Number of Number of Publications Studies Effect Sizes

1955 1 12

1956 1 4

1958 1 1

1959 6 20

1960 2 19

1961 4 11

1962 4 20

1963 5 59

1964 5 19

1965 10 47

1966 9 96

1967 13 73

1968 10 83

1969 13 70

1970 14 107

1971 8 47

1972 5 30

1973 2 9

1974 3 25

1975 6 26

1976 7 20

1977 5 12

Median Year: 1968 1968

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. variables, the measures of the median and mode are provided in Table

7. The maximum and minimum values of the variables are also provided

in the same table. This was done in order to indicate the direction

of the range of observations under consideration.

Table 7

Length of Treatment and Number of Subjects Per Study

Number of Maxi­ Mini­ Observa­ Variable Mean Median Mode mum mum tions

Length of treatment 33.84 24 36 144 1 810 (in weeks)

Number of subjects 206.90 108 24 2,862 4 810 per study

Overall Effects of Modern Mathematics

Description of the studies provided £i picture of the general

nature of the studies included. The ensuing sections will deal with

the quantified measures of the effects of modern mathematics in terms

of achievement and/or attitude.

Chapter III discussed the concept that the "effect size" was a

common measure of treatment effectiveness. This measure was defined

as the difference between the means of the modern mathematics group

and the traditional mathematics group divided by the standard devia­

tion of the traditional mathematics group. Based on the preceding

definition, 660 effect sizes in achievement and 150 effect sizes iri

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 63 attitude were calculated from 134 controlled outcome studies. It

must be remembered that in some studies more than one effect size was

calculated.

The grand mean effect size in achievement was 0.24 and the grand

mean effect size in attitude was 0.12. Table 8 shows the average

effect sizes in achievement and attitude.

Table 8

Grand Mean Effect Sizes in Achievement and Attitude

Statistic Achievement Attitude

Mean 0.24* 0.12*

Median 0.09 0.04

Standard deviation 0.77 0.56

Standard error of mean 0.03 0.05

Number of effect sizes 660 150

t-value 8.00 2.40

Probability 0.00 0.018

Note. Degrees of freedom = Number of effect sizes - 1.

*The null hypothesis, stating the mean effect size is zero, is rejected with the probability of committing type I error being less than or equal to 0.05.

This meant that the average person receiving some form of modern

mathematics treatment was 0.24 standard deviation more improved on

the achievement measure and 0.12 standard deviation more improved on

the attitude measure than the average traditional mathematics group

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 64

member (see Figure 4).

The interpretation of Figure 4 followed the reasonings presented

while discussing Figure 2 in Chapter III. The dotted lines repre­

sented the normal distribution of the traditional mathematics popula­

tion and the solid line represented the normal distribution of the

modern mathematics population. Since the effect size in achievement

was 0.24, a student at the 50th percentile of the modern mathematics

group was equivalent to a student at the 59th percentile of the

traditional mathematics group. Similarly, a student at the 50th per­

centile of the modern mathematics group was equivalent to a student

at the 55th percentile of the traditional mathematics group in atti­

tude. The effect size of attitude was 0.12. In other words, the

modern mathematics program raised an average student from the 50th to

the 59th percentile in achievement and from the 50th to the 55th per­

centile in attitude over the traditional methematics population. The

basic assumption was that an average student of the traditional math­

ematics population occupied the 50th percentile in a normal curve.

It is interesting to note that the improvements, reported in

terms of effect sizes which resulted from the treatment of modern

mathematics, indicated a great difference in scores on the achieve­

ment and attitude dimensions. In the following sections, treatment

of the specific effects are provided.

Specific Effects by Subject Characteristics

The subject variables described the important characteristics of

the students who were involved in the studies that were utilized.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 65

0.24

TRADITIONAL MODERN

50th Percentile 59th Percentile of Control of Control

Average Effect Size in Achievement: 0.24

0.12

50th Percentile 55th Percentile of Control of Control

Average Effect Size in Attitude: 0.12

Figure 4. Normal curves illustrating the aggregate effect of modern mathematics in achievement and attitude in relation to traditional mathematics group.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Four main subject characteristics were analyzed. They were the

grades which were chosen for the experiments, ability levels, the

levels according to socio-economic status, and sex. Classification

of the subjects on the basis of the variables discussed was intended

to enable the reader to form an idea of the effectiveness of modern

mathematics under varying conditions and circumstances. Since the

classification of levels within ability and socio-economic status was

arbitrary in nature, the effect sizes in those subcells were less

specific and reliable than the effect sizes of grade levels and sex.

Effect Size by Grade

Grade levels considered for the present study ranged from kinder­

garten to post-secondary. The post-secondary group consisted mostly

of students of the community colleges and their freshmen and sopho­

more counterparts at four year colleges and universities. Among the

five grade levels, the elementary had the largest average effect size

in achievement. It was 0.32. An average effect size which was close

to the elementary grades was from the senior high grades. Conversely,

the average effect size at the junior high grades was remarkably low

in comparison to the elementary and senior high grade levels. Table

9 indicates that the average effect size of the kindergarten and the

post-secondary grade levels in achievement was insignificant. This

was possibly due to the limited number of effect sizes in each cate­

gory.

An attempt was made, in observing the average effect sizes, to

find the relationship between the significant effect sizes and grade

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.095 0.001* 4.67 0.000* 3.57 t-value Probability 79 88 2.63 0.010* 404 7.33 0.000* Sizes Effect Number of 0.140.04 110.06 249 0.43 140 4.25 0.676 0.000* 0.13 89 2.69 0.009* 0.08 Error of Meanof Standard Table 9 Table 0.47 0.58 0.63 0.03 Standard Levels of Grade Deviation Levels Ability of 0.02 0.09 0.47 0.07 44 1.71 0.17 0.06 0.00 0.28 0.18 0.77 0.35 0.00 1.25 Effect Size by Levels Grade of Ability and Achievementin Mean Median *The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejected with the probability of Note. Note. Degrees of freedomNumber = of effect sizes - 1. Senior Senior high Junior high Kindergarten Post-secondary 0.12 committing type error I being less than or equal to 0.05. "J Elementary 0.32 0.14 0.96 0.07 216 4.57 0.000* Low Unspecified 0.22 0.14 MiddleHigh 0.25 0.21 0.07 0.00 0.64 0.79 0.07

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 68

levels. This was done by fitting a parabola with a downward bend.

The multiple correlation coefficient of the curvilinear regression

was only 0.09. The value of the multiple correlation coefficient

indicates that there is no significant relationship between the

levels of grades and the effect sizes in achievement.

Effect Size by Ability Level

Most of the studies which compared the effectiveness of modern

mathematics based on ability divided the subjects into three groups,

such as low, middle, and high. In a few instances, the comparison

was made between the high and low ability levels only. In this

study, all three levels of ability were considered. Effect sizes

were classified into low, middle, or high levels of ability according

to the designation in the original studies. A fourth group, desig­

nated as "unspecified" ability level, consisted of studies which did

not make a comparative analysis by ability levels.

The average effect sizes were considered to be statistically

significant at = 0.05. Table 9 indicates that the average effect

size in achievement was the largest for the low ability group, which

was 0.35. This value was substantially higher than that of the grand

mean effect size in achievement which was 0.24 (see Table 8). The

average effect sizes of the middle, high, and the unspecified ability

levels did not deviate from the grand mean effect size in any signifi­

cant manner. In general, Table 9 shows a decrease in mean effect size

as one goes from the low to the high level of ability.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 69 Effect Size by Socio-economic Status and by Sex

Socio-economic status and sex were the other two variables which

belonged to the category of subject characteristics. There were only

72 effect sizes out of 660 in achievement where the socio-economic

status of the students was identified (see Table 10). The 72 effect

sizes were not representative of the total population of studies

insofar as the mean of the 72 effect sizes was not equal to the grand

mean effect size. The only effect size which was significant at the

= 0.05 level was that of the students belonging to middle class

parents. The effect size of the middle group was 1.05 which was

conspicuously higher than the grand mean effect size of the present

study.

As in the case of the analysis of socio-economic status, the

cell sizes in achievement were quite small when the data were analyzed

by sex. As Table 10 indicated, the effect sizes of each category in

this particular subset did not represent the total population, since

the average mean for either sex was substantially lower than those

studies not grouped for sex.

Because of the small number of studies in the socio-economic

status and sex categories, caution must be exercised in the interpre­

tation of the results.

Specific Effects by Treatment Factors

The previous section dealt with the characteristics of the stu^

dents who were involved in the studies that were utilized; whereas,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 1.67 0.108 28 0.25 0.805 Sizes t-value Probability Effect Number of 0.12 0.090.31 25 33 3.39 0.002* 0.03 588 7.00 0,000* Error of Meanof Standard in in Sex Table 10 Table 1.77 Standard Deviation of of Socio-economic Status Difference Levels 0.42 Median Effect Size by Socio-economic Status and Sex Achievementin 0.15 0.01 0.47 Mean *The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejected with the probability of Note. Note. Degrees of freedom = Number of effect sizes - 1. committing type errorI being less than or equal 0.05. to Female 0.03 -0.05 0.66 Unspecified 0.21 0.78 0.66 Low High 0.22 0.35 0.50 0.13 14 1.69 0.115 Male 0.11 -0.05 0.59 0.12 26 0.92 0.366 Middle 1.05

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 71

the treatment factors described the type and content of application

of modern mathematics in the various experiments selected for the

study. Two factors which related to the content of application were

the general content area and the specific subject area. There were

three variables which described the type of treatment. They were the

type of program that had been selected for the treatment of modern

mathematics, the length of treatment, and the number of subjects per

study.

Effect Size by General Treatment Area

Table 11 reveals that 304 effect sizes in achievement were iden­

tified as belonging to one of the three selected categories. The

categories were in accordance with the emphasis given in the treat­

ment of modern mathematics in the generic area. The three areas of

emphasis were concepts, computation, and application. Studies which

could not be identified as belonging to one of these three categories

were treated in the fourth category, designated as the "combination."

Among the four categories, the highest average effect size was

that of the concepts. Almost similar achievement gain was evidenced

in computation. While the mean effect sizes of the concepts and of

computation were 0.36 and 0.31, respectively, the mean effect size of

the application was only 0.06. In other words, the improvement attrib­

utable to modern mathematics in the area of application of the mathe­

matical concepts was next to zero, whereas the improvements in the

areas of concepts and of content was quite substantial.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.006* 0.000* 0.000* 1.20 0.234 6.25 0.000* 85 1.75 0.084 Sizes t-value Probability Effect Number of . 1 0.07 134 5.14 0.04 3560.05 5.50 305 0.000* 4.20 0.000* 0.04 196 Error of Mean Standard Table 11 Table 0.88 Standard Deviation General Content Area Specific Subject Area of of effect sizes - Number Effect Size by Content Area in Achievement 0.31 0.08 1.05 0.11 95 2.82 0.21 0.05 Mean Median *The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejected with the probability of Note. Note. Degrees of freedom = Combination 0.22 0.09 0.71 Computation Concepts 0.36 0.27 0.78 committing type I error being less than or equal 0.05. to Application 0.06 0.00 0.45 0.05 75 Geometry 0.14 -0.01 0.70 0.08 Arithmetic Other 0.25 0.11 0.62 Algebra 0.43 0.31 0.68 0.08 74 5.38

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 73

Effect Size by Specific Subject Area

An attempt was made to classify effect sizes into specific sub­

ject areas, such as arithmetic, algebra, and geometry. The fourth

section consisted of treatments pertaining to sets, groups, fields,

logic, and some specific areas of mathematics, such as calculus,

statistics, and probability, which were introduced in the language of

modern mathematics.

Among the specific subject areas, algebra had the highest average

effect size with 0.43 (see Table 11) in achievement. The average

effect size in arithmetic was almost close to the grand mean effect

size of the total number of studies in achievement (0.21 versus 0.24).

Table 11 also points out the average effect size of achievement in

geometry (0.14) was lower than that of arithmetic (0.21) and of

algebra (0.43). The mean effect size in achievement of the combina­

tion group was typical of the grand mean effect size in achievement

as reported in Table 8. Generally, the mean effect sizes of arith­

metic and of the combination group represented the grand mean effect

size. The larger mean effect size of algebra was offset by the

smaller mean effect size of geometry.

Effect Size by Type of Program

Initially, an attempt was made to identify studies according to

the specific development programs discussed in Chapter I; however, the

number of studies belonging to each specific development program other

than the School Mathematics Study Group (SMSG) was either very small

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. or nil. The type of program therefore, was broadly divided into SMSG

programs and non-SMSG programs. The average effect size in achieve­

ment of studies belonging to SMSG was considerably lower than the

average effect size in achievement of all other studies put together

(see Table 12). Other studies denoted as non-SMSG, included a few

studies on modern mathematics as introduced by other developmental

programs such as the University of Illinois Committee on School Math­

ematics Program, the Ball State University Program, the Greater

Cleveland Mathematics Program, and a number of studies on modern

mathematics as introduced by innovators other than the reformers in

the developmental programs.

Effect Size by Length of Treatment

Table 13 reports the average effect size in achievement according

to the length of treatment. Studies with a length of treatment which

was less than 9 weeks and studies with a length of treatment which

was greater than or equal to 36 weeks had the same average effect

size (0.25). The average effect size reached the maximum point,

which was 0.48 in this context, when the length of’"treatment was

between 9 and 18 weeks. Studies with the length of treatment ranging

from 18 to 36 weeks had average effect sizes as low as 0.09. Thus,

the effect sizes indicated a gradual increase until the length of

treatment became 18 weeks or more. After 18 weeks, the effect sizes

gradually decreased until the length of treatment was 36 weeks or

greater. Thereafter, the effect sizes in achievement rose and

remained steady at 0.25, which was close to the grand mean effect

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 127 2.80 0.006* Sizes t-value Probability Effect Number of . 0.041 533 6.75 0.000* 0.05 Error of Mean of Standard Standard Table 12 0.52 Standard Deviation = Number of effect sizes - Effect Size by Type of Program Achievementin Degrees of freedom *The *The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejected with probability the of Note. SMSG 0.14 0.00 Othercommitting type I error being less than or equal to 0.05. 0.27 0.10 0.81 Program Mean Median

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.000* Probability 1.50 0.136 124 Sizes t-value Effect Number of . 0.05 133 5.00 1 0.04 334 6.25 0.000* Error Error of Meanof Standard Standard - - Table 13 0.57 Standard Deviation 0.17 Effect by Size Length of Treatment Achievement in Mean Median *The *The null hypothesis, stating meanthe effect size is zero, is rejected with the probability of Note. Note. Degrees of freedom = Number of effect sizes (weeks) Treatment Length of 36 more36 or 0.25 0.09 0.66 committing type I error being less than or equal to 0.05. Less Less than 9 0.25 Between 9 and 18Between 18 and 36 0.48 0.09 0.00 0.04 1.43 0.64 0.17 0.06 69 2.82 0.006*

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 77

size in achievement.

Effect Size by Number of Subjects Per Study

As indicated in Table 14, the number of subjects per study was

categorized into five groups. The upper limits of each class interval

of the distribution of number of subjects per study were in geometric

progression with common ratio equal to 2 and the base equal to 50.

With this arrangement, the effect sizes were relatively evenly dis­

tributed in each subgroup. The average effect size in achievement

was fairly high for the studies in which the number of subjects was

less than 50. Studies in which the number of subjects was greater

than 200 and less than 400 had a typical average effect size (0.26).

It was only 0.02 standard deviation greater than the grand mean effect

size. The average effect sizes in the other cells of this subgroup

ranged from 0.13 to 0.16.

Specific Effect Size by Study Variables

The study variables outlined characteristics such as the empha­

sis in the approach of the study, the source of the studies, the

affiliation of the researcher, and the author who introduced an

innovation. The variables in this category were not manipulative in

nature, but they were related to the outcomes. Collectively, they

described those characteristics of the study which were not treated

under the subject characteristics, treatment factors, or design vari­

ables .

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.002* Probability 3.75 0.000* 3.25 t-value 118 4.33 0.000* Sizes Effect Number of . 1 0.06 Error of of Mean Standard Table 14 0.67 0.53 0.04 178 Standard Deviation 0.08 0.34 0.04 68 0.07 0.05 0.78 0.06 153 2.67 0.008* Effect by Size Number of Subjects Per Study Achievementin of freedom = Number of effect sizes - *The *The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejected with the probability of Note. Note. Degrees Subjects Per Study Mean Median 50 50 or lessBetween 50 and 100 0.16 0.49 0.25 1.10 0.09 143 5.44 0.000* Between 100 and 200 0.15 Between 200 and 400 0.26 0.10 committing type I error being less than or equal to 0.05. More than 400 0.13

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 79 Effect Size by Study Approach

The variable of study approach allowed the studies to be distin­

guished according to one of four areas of emphasis: methodology

approach, content approach, materials approach, and combination

approach. The combination approach consisted of studies with no

special emphasis on any of the three areas previously discussed. The

majority of the studies which emphasized the methodology belonged to

the discovery approach, heuristic method, and deductive reasoning.

Studies which emphasized the content dealt mostly with topics such as

sets, logic, structure, and nondecimal bases. Most of the studies

which emphasized the materials belonged to experiments with manipula­

tive materials, the Cuisanaire approach, and the mathematics labora­

tory.

Table 15 shows the average effect sizes of each of the study

approaches in achievement and attitude. Obviously, there were large

achievement gains in all the approaches except that of methodology.

Conversely, there were no substantial attitudinal gains in any of the

four approaches except that of methodology. In the methodology

approach the achievement gain was only 0.04, which was the least of

all the mean effect sizes. Conversely, the attitudinal gain in the

methodology approach was 0.17, which was the largest mean effect size.

The best result in achievement was that of the materials approach

with a mean effect size of 0.51. The content approach was second

with an average effect size of 0.35.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.319 0.017* 0.001* 0.312 2.43 1.33 0.202 1.00 3.64 -1.04 19 17 229 5.83 0.000* Sizes t-value Probability Effect Effect Number of . 1 0.07 84 0.05 30 1.60 0.12Q 0.06 0.05 0.09 Error of Mean of Standard Standard - - Table 15 Table Attitude 0.26 Achievement Standard Deviation 0.16 0.36 0.00 = Number of effect sizes Effect Sizeby Study Approach in Achievement and Attitude 0.17 0.21 0.70 0.08 0.35 0.10 0.94 0.51 0.21 0.86 0.14 39 Mean Median of of freedom *The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejectedwith the probability of Note. Note. Degrees Approach Content Combination 0.25 0.05 0.61 0.04 211 6.25 0.000* Content -0.07 0.00 0.21 Combination committing type I error being less than or equal to 0.05. Material Methodology 0.04 0.08 0.59 0.04 181 Methodology Material 0.12

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 81 Effect Size by Source of the Study

Three different sources from which the studies were gathered

were the ERIC documents, journal articles, and doctoral dissertations.

Table 16 lists the effect sizes by each source. The ERIC documents

consisted of school district reports, final reports from federal

grants, and other miscellany. The studies from the ERIC documents

had the Largest average effect size in achievement— 0.77— an effect

size which was three times bigger than that of the grand mean effect

size. The journal articles had an effect size (0.42) which was about

twice the grand mean effect size in achievement. The mean effect

size of the doctoral dissertations was the least in the category of

source (0.16).

Effect Size by Other Descriptive Variables

Other descriptive variables analyzed for the present study were

the subject area of investigation, the affiliation of the researcher,

and the author. The subject area variable had two categories: (a)

mathematics alone and (b) mathematics and other subjects combined. In

Table 17, the average effect sizes by subject area are reported. The

average effect sizes in both categories were almost identical.

"Affiliation of researcher" identified the organization the

researcher had an association with while conducting the study. The

variable was broadly divided into affiliation with a university and

affiliation with any organization other than a university. Table 18

indicates that the average effect sizes in achievement were almost

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 8.40 0.000* 39 2.85 0.000* 121 500 5.33 0.000* Sizes t-value Probability Effect Number of . 0.05 1 Error of Meanof Standard - - Table 16 0.67 0.03 Standard Deviation effect sizes 0.34 0.52 Effect Size by Source in Achievement “The “The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejectedwith the probability of Note. Note. Degrees of freedom Number = of Source Mean Median dissertations 0.16 0.04 Journal articles 0.42 ERIC documentsDoctoral 0.77 0.02committing type error I being less than or equal 0.05. to 1.70 0.27

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Sizes t-value Probability Effect Number of . 1 0.03 609 8.00 0.000* Error of of Mean Standard Standard - - Table 17 Standard Median Deviation Effect Size by Subject Area of Investigation Achievementin 0.23 0.09 0.61 0.08 51 2.88 0.006* Mean *The null hypothesis, stating meanthe effect size is zero, is rejected with the probability of Note. Note. Degrees of freedom Number = of effect sizes Area of Investigation Other Mathematics 0.24 0.09 0.78 committing type I error being less than or equal 0.05. to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 583 8.00 0.000* 0.03 Standard Number of Table 18 0.79 Standard Error Effect 0.09 0.25 0.09 0.49 0.06 77 4.17 Effect Size by Affiliation of Researcher the inAchievement *The *The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejected with probability the of Note. Note. Degrees of freedom Number = of effect sizes - 1. committing type I error being less than or equal to 0.05. Other University 0.24 Affiliation Mean Median Deviation Mean of Sizes t-value Probability

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 85

identical for both the studies affiliated with a university and those

affiliated with other organizations. The similarity was also true for

the subject area of investigation.

An attempt was made to identify the author who introduced either

a new content, a new methodology, a new material, or any combination

of the three. The purpose of this variable was to identify the direc­

tion of bias in the results due to the influence of personal interest in

the topic of experimentation. For this purpose, the variable (author)

was subdivided into four categories (commercial, school districts,

experimenter, and university); however, it was impossible in most of

the cases to identify the real author of the innovations.

An attempt was made to determine the extent of each researcher's

personal involvement in the experiment, but it was uncertain in most

of the cases exactly how involved the researcher was in the results of

the study. In some cases the researcher was either the director of a

project or a member of the staff of a project; however, information

of this nature was available in only a few cases. Consequently, a

determination could not be made about the extent of the researcher’s

actual involvement in the study.

Specific Effects by Design Variables

The fourth category of the specific variables dealt with the

design variables. The function of the variables in this category was

to measure the different characteristics of the research design used

in each study. Three variables were selected for this purpose. They

were the type of test used to measure the achievement and/or attitude,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 86

the quality of the design used for each experiment, and the novelty

effect which was assumed to be a factor that depends on the length of

treatment.

Effect Size by Type of Test

Basically, there were only two categories of tests that were

used in the studies. One category consisted of a variety of standard­

ized norm-referenced mathematics tests. The other category consisted

of teacher or experimenter developed tests. Table 19 shows that about

two-thirds of the effect sizes in achievement were measured by com­

mercial tests. The average effect size in achievement of the commer­

cial test was only 0.15 which was lower than the grand mean effect

size in achievement; however, the average effect size of the experi­

menter developed test in achievement was about three times greater

than that of the commercial tests.

Effect Size by Quality of Design

The quality of design variable had two categories: studies where

subjects were randomly assigned to treatment and studies where sub­

jects were not randomly assigned to treatment. As indicated in Table

19, 552 effect sizes in achievement were obtained from studies with

random assignments. The average effect size in achievement of the

studies with random assignment was 0.26, whereas the average effect

size of studies without random assignment was only slightly more than

half (0.14) of the former.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 0.000* 0.000* 5.00 0.000* 438 108 2.80 0.006* Sizes t-value Probability Effect Number of 0.03 0.05 Error of of Mean Standard Table 19 Table 0.62 0.53 Types of Test Standard Novelty Effect Deviation Quality of Design 0.13 0.96 0.07 202 4.71 Effect by Size Design Variables Achievementin 0.15 0.05 0.14 0.05 0.33 Mean Median *The *The null hypothesis, stating mean the effect size is zero, is rejected with probability the of Note. Note. Degrees of freedom Number = of effect sizes - 1. developed 0.42 0.25 0.97 0.07 222 6.00 0.000* Commercial Experimenter assignment Random assignment 0.26 0.09 0.80 0.03 552 8.67 No random Present committing type I error being less than or equal to 0.05. Absent 0.20 0.08 0.66 0.03 458 6.67 0.000*

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Effect Size by Novelty Effect

Since the novelty effect variable depended on the length of

treatment, a critical measure was arbitrarily established to measure

the novelty effect. For the purposes of the present study, the nov­

elty effect was assumed to be present whenever the length of treatment

was less than 18 weeks. For those studies in which novelty effect

was judged to be present, the average was significantly higher than

that of the studies not having novelty effect. Table 19 lists the

effect sizes in achievement by novelty effect. About one-third of

the number of effect sizes had a novelty effect. The average effect

size in achievement of the studies without novelty effect was less

that two-thirds of the effect size in achievement of the studies with

novelty effect.

Summary

Table 20 gives an overview of the mean effect sizes in achieve­

ment and/or attitude of all the variables analyzed. In this section

only the major results are highlighted.

When the subjects were divided into five grade levels, the ele­

mentary and the senior high had the larger effect sizes in achieve­

ment, which were respectively 0.32 and 0.28. Among the low, middle,

and high ability groups, the low ability had the largest effect size

(0.35) in achievement. In a similar grouping based on the socio­

economic status, the effect size of the middle group in achievement

was 1.05 which was conspicuously higher than that of the low and high

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 89

Effect Sizes by All Categories of Variables in Achievement and/or Attitude

Variable Effect Size Variable Effect Size

Achievement by grade Achievement by number of subjects Kindergarten 0.06 50 or less 0.49* Elementary 0.32* Between 50 and 100 0.16* Junior high 0.17* Between 100 and 200 0.15* Senior high 0.28* Between 200 and 400 0.26* Post-secondary 0.12 More than 400 0.13*

Achievement by ability Achievement by study approach Low 0.35* Methodology 0.04 Middle 0.25* Content 0.35* High 0.21* Material 0.51*

Achievement by SES Attitude by study approach Low 0.15 Methodology 0.17* Middle 1.05* Content -0.07 High 0.22 Material 0.12

Achievement by sex Achievement by source Male 0.11 ERIC document 0.77* Female 0.03 Journal article 0.42* Dissertation 0.16* Achievement by general content area Achievement by area of Concepts 0.36* investigation Computation 0.31* Mathematics 0.24* Application 0.06 Other 0.23* Achievement in specific Achievement by affiliation subject area of the researcher Arithmetic 0.21* University 0.24* Algebra 0.43* Other 0.25* Geometry 0.14 Achievement by type of test Achievement by program Commercial 0.15* SMSG 0.14* Experimenter 0.42* Other 0.27* Achievement by design Achievement by length Random assignment 0.26* of treatment in weeks Other 0.14* Less than 9 0.25* Between 9 and 18 0.48* Achievement by novelty effect Between 18 and 36 0.09 Present 0.33* 36 and more 0.25* Absent 0.20*

Grand mean achievement 0.24* Grand mean attitude 0.12*

*The null hypothesis, stating the mean effect size is zero, is rejected with the probability of committing type I error being less than or equal to 0.05.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 90 groups.

In the general content area, the effect sizes of concepts (0.36)

and of computation (0.31) were very large in achievement, while that

of the application was as low as 0.06. Among the specific subject

areas, algebra had a high effect size of 0.43 in achievement; however,

geometry had a poor effect size of 0.14.

Of the three approaches such as methodology, content, and mate­

rials the effect sizes in the content was 0.35 and in the materials

0.51. The methodology had the lowest effect size (0.04) in achieve­

ment. Conversely, methodology had the highest effect size in atti­

tude .

The effect sizes when averaged according to the different sources,

studies from dissertations showed the least effect size (0.16). The

journal articj.es and the ERIC documents indicated superior effect

sizes (0.42 and 0.77, respectively).

Caution must be exercised in the interpretation of the nonsigni­

ficant results reported in the tables presented in this chapter. Two

or more effect sizes having the same magnitude may differ in their

significance. For example, in Table 20, the grand mean effect size

in attitude (0.12) is significant, whereas the mean effect size of

the post-secondary grades in achievement (0.12) is insignificant. It

must be noted that in many instances, it is very likely that the

results are insignificant due to small number of effect sizes.

Achievement by sex and by socio-economic status are other striking

instances of the previously discussed phenomenon.

In total, the grand mean of 660 effect sizes in achievement was

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 91

0.24 and the grand mean of 150 effect sizes in attitude was 0.12. In

other words, an average student who received some form of modern math­

ematics instruction was raised from the 50th to the 59th percentile

in achievement and from the 50th to the 55th percentile in attitude,

over the traditional mathematics population.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. CHAPTER V

INTERPRETATIONS, IMPLICATIONS, GENERAL RECOMMENDATIONS, AND SUMMARY

Interpretations

The interpretations generally followed the sequence of the dis­

cussion of the results in Chapter IV. Some of the mean effect sizes

reported in Chapter IV were not significant. As a rule, interpreta­

tions were focused primarily upon results which were statistically

significant.

The Grand Mean Effect Sizes

The obtained results revealed that there were improvements in

achievement and attitude which were attributable to modern mathemat­

ics. Improvement in achievement of an average student was almost

double the improvement in attitude. The discrepancy between improved

rates of achievement and attitude is typical of innovations where the

appeal is intellectual in nature and demanding in its application.

New mathematics fits this mode in that it is mostly abstract. Con­

sequently, modern mathematics requires a strict discipline of the

mind in order to comprehend its operations. It is consoling to note

that the effects of modern mathematics are worth its demands. Also,

attitude toward an innovative program is often a result of the per­

ceived success or failure of the innovation. Dissimilar findings in

the absence of summarized results helped maintain a low morale toward

92

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 93

modern mathematics for the past 30 years. People were uncertain

about whether or not "new math worked." Hopefully, this study has

revealed those areas where modern mathematics is effective. Conse­

quently, a more positive attitude toward new mathematics should occur,

an attitude which will be comparable to the achievement level.

Subject Variables

Of the four variables identified as subject characteristics,

three lend themselves to interpretations. The three variables are

grade level, ability level, and socio-economic status. In general,

the findings in this group were not in agreement with the expecta­

tions and viewpoints of the critics of new mathematics. Consequently,

interpretations of these findings must be interesting to the oppo­

nents as well as proponents.

Grade level. Among the different grade levels, the mean effect

size in achievement of the junior high pupils was the least signifi­

cant. One explanation of the low mean effect size of the junior high

grades is obvious. Confusion may have entered the minds of pupils

due to the sudden transition in their thinking patterns as they

switched from traditional mathematics in the elementary grades to

modern mathematics in the junior high.

The greater mean effect size of the elementary grades where the

students were introduced to only one kind of treatment (new mathe­

matics) explains the clear perception of new mathematics concepts in

the absence of treatments antithetical in nature. In the case of the

senior high students, the large effect size could be assumed to be

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 94 the result of a development of minds into a stage in which they could

discriminate one treatment from another without confusion.

Ability level. Summarily, the greater the effect, the lower the

ability level was. This finding explains the comparative effective­

ness of the two treatments of mathematics. High ability students,

because of their innate intellectual acumen, do not depend upon the

kind of treatment as much as the low ability students do in the

understanding of mathematics concepts. Consequently, the introduc­

tion of new mathematics effected a comparatively smaller gain in

achievement among the high ability groups while the low ability

groups experienced a large growth in achievement.

Knowing this trend, one would not be tempted to conclude that

modern mathematics was an easy approach. More likely, it was a mean­

ingful approach which produced better results than the traditional

mathematics among all ability levels, especially at the lower levels

of ability.

Socio-economic status. The striking result in the application

of modern mathematics among different levels of socio-economic status

was the outstanding effect size of the pupils coming from middle

class parents. One of the main reasons for the high achievement of

pupils from middle socio-economic levels could be parental help. It

must be pointed out that among the parents who are most attuned to

the trends in mathematics, many are also teachers who generally

belong to the middle socio-economic class. These parents are often

more aware of the need for providing help to their children in under­

standing modern mathematics.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 95

Treatment Variables

The interpretations in the ensuing sections might help to find

some answers to the heated criticisms levelled against certain

aspects of modern mathematics. These interpretations pertain to the

general content area, specific subject area, type of program, and the

length of treatment.

General content area. In the general content area, which con­

sisted of concepts, computation, and application, the highest mean

effect size was that of the concepts. Obviously this is a likely

outcome of the treatment of modern mathematics because one of the

unique characteristics of modern mathematics is the emphasis on the

understanding of concepts. The weakest score in the general content

area was in application. The poor result in this area perhaps

explains a lack of adequate time for the pupils to exercise their

minds. Possibly, a longer treatment of modern mathematics with suf­

ficient emphasis on the concrete presentation of abstract concepts

would strengthen the students in the area of application of mathe­

matical concepts.

Specific subject area. The specific content area was divided

into arithmetic, algebra, and geometry. A fourth category was

included to accommodate subject areas other than those previously

specified. Among the four subject areas, algebra had the highest

mean effect size in achievement. The explanation for this result is

subject to speculation. It could be that among all the specific sub­

ject areas investigated, algebra was the most expressive medium to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 96 bring out the message of modern mathematics. Similar results can be

expected in other subject areas, especially in geometry, if the mode

of presentation of those subject areas are more attuned to the axio­

matic approach of modern mathematics.

Type of program. The types of programs were grouped in School

Mathematics Study Group (SMSG) and non-SMSG. This was due to the

small number of studies in all developmental programs except SMSG.

The average effect size in achievement of the non-SMSG group was

almost twice that of the SMSG group. A number of studies in the non-

SMSG group were conducted by individual experimenters. In those

experiments, factors such as enthusiasm of the experimenter, attrac­

tion of a new theme, and pride of the students in belonging to a

selected group, might have been favorable for the great success in

modern mathematics treatment. Conversely, in the treatment of SMSG

programs, most of the factors mentioned did not exist because they

were generally adopted on a massive scale by school districts; there­

fore, the effect size of non-SMSG programs explains more of an initial

enthusiasm. Conversely, the results of the SMSG program indicate a

conservative, but steady and longstanding, effect of modern mathema­

tics treatment.

Length of treatment. The effect sizes, when averaged according

to the length of treatment, demonstrated an interesting pattern. The

average effect in achievement was maximum when the length of treat­

ment was between 9 and 18 weeks and minimum for a period between 18

and 36 weeks. The maximum and minimum values were 0.48 and 0.09,

respectively. In the studies with the length of treatment greater

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 97 than 36 weeks, the effect sizes were averaged to be 0.25, the mid­

value between the two extremes. Therefore, the pattern was a rise,

fall, and a gradual steadiness toward the mean of the two values.

The rise possibly demonstrates the initial enthusiasm and fascination

for the new mathematics programs. The fall manifests the setback the

pupils experienced when they were confronted with the rigor and

abstractness of the modern math treatment. Once the pupils got over

the period of a confrontation, they began making steady improvements

which were reflected in the effect sizes of treatments over 36 weeks.

Most of the criticisms that were levelled against modern mathe­

matics during the past decades could have been based on the results

obtained during the time of the initial setback. The ebb and tide is

over. Now, the "new math" flows steady. This is the time for a

mature judgment on the effect of modern mathematics.

Study Variables

The interpretations of the study variables highlight some pos­

sible explanations of the differences in effectiveness in three

major areas. The areas are the study approach, author, and source of

the study.

Study approach. A polarity existed between the scores in

achievement and attitude in the four different types of study

approach. Among the four types, namely, methodology, content, mate­

rials, and combination, all types except methodology had significantly

larger effect sizes in achievement. On the contrary, effect sizes of

all types of approaches, except that of methodology, were relatively

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 98

small in attitude.

The polarity reveals a striking point. What worked better with

modern mathematics wjs due to an appeal to reason, rather than an

appeal to the senses. The methodology approach, which mostly con­

sisted of discovery approaches and its subcategories, might have

appealed to the susceptibilities of the young minds; however, little

was accomplished in terms of achievement through the new methodologies.

What made modern mathematics effective in terms of achievement was its

content area characterized by the structure and abstractness.

The mean effect size on the materials is even more revealing.

The high achievement effect size of the materials approach makes it

explicit that the abstract concepts of modern mathematics, when pre­

sented to the students in concrete forms through adequate materials,

produced the best results in achievement.

Author. The effect sizes in this group were associated with

school districts, universities, and private experimenters according

to the affiliation of the researcher. A comparatively large mean

effect size in achievement for the experimenter group indicates, per­

haps, the direction of bias caused by self-interest.

The experimenter group consisted mainly of textbook publishers

and other commercial agencies. This group possibly tried to present

the most positive results for monetary purposes. Consequently, they

would be the most vulnerable in the direction of bias. To a lesser

degree, the school districts might have been affected by their pre­

occupation to build up an exemplar. The results of the studies whose

authors were affiliated with universities indicated the least effect

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 99

size in achievement. On the whole, effect sizes in that group repre­

sented the most conservative and most dependable figures.

Source of the study. Sources of the study revealed a similar

situation. ERIC documents which consisted of school district reports,

final reports from federal funds, etc., had the highest average effect

size in achievement. The reason for the high effect size could be

that studies from the above sources are often attempts to convince

the funding agencies to continue funding of a specific program by

presenting the results as attractively as possible. The journal

articles also had a large effect size in achievement. Perhaps the

journals reveal the increased tendency to publish studies favoring

the new approach. Conversely, dissertations with no such self-motives,

contained more balanced and neutral findings.

Since the present analysis utilized 85 percent of the total num­

ber of studies from complete dissertations, the findings presented

here would be considered to be highly conservative and most dependable

in nature.

Design Variables

Interpretations of the design variables deal with the type of

test and the quality of design. The elucidations will help one eval­

uate the comparative effectiveness of different types in tests and

designs.

Type of test. The types of tests utilized were commercial and

experimenter developed. Effect sizes in achievement of studies meas­

ured by commercial tests were very small in comparison with effect

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 100

sizes in achievement measured by experimenter developed tests. One

of the weaknesses of the standardized tests was that they were not

constructed specifically for a study to test the objectives of a par­

ticular treatment. As a rule, most of the commercial tests were more

suited to measure the objectives of the traditional treatments than

those of the modern treatment of mathematics. Consequently, the

effect size of the commercial test group did not adequately represent

the achievement of students in modern mathematics. It is obvious,

therefore, that the overall mean effect size would have been signifi­

cantly higher than the present one, if all the studies utilized tests

which adequately measured the performance according to the objectives

specified for the particular treatment.

Quality of design. In the present study, the experiments in

which the subjects were randomly assigned to treatments had a higher

effect size in achievement than the experiments without random assign­

ment. According to the methodologists, experiments with random

assignment should yield results with less bias than the experiments

without random assignment. On that basis, the greater effect size in

achievement obtained from the random assignment group should be con­

sidered to be more objective than the smaller effect size obtained

from the studies without random assignment.

Implications

The data presented in Chapter IV and their interpretations in

the early sections of Chapter V have several important implications

for sound decision making and needed improvements in mathematics

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 101

programs.

The aggregate findings of the experiments with modern mathematics

for the last 30 years show the critics and opponents wrong with regard

to the detrimental effects of the "new math." On the contrary, the

evidence shows the beneficial effects in terms of the improvement in

overall achievement and attitude. In the light of the findings of

the present study, it would be unwise to advocate the superiority of

the traditional mathematics over the new mathematics. Subsequently,

the current retrenchment in mathematics under the banner of the back-

to-basics philosophy can be regarded only as an aberration brought

about by the short sightedness of the doomsayers and the misjudgments

of the decision makers in the absence of reliable research findings

about the effectiveness of new mathematics.

The beneficial effects of new mathematics call for the promotion

of the new programs throughout the nation. The findings should

encourage the higher authorities throughout the country, on the

federal, state, and local levels of mathematics education, to re­

introduce new mathematics curriculum in order to meet the changing

needs of the society.

The overall findings also indicated that the improvement of

attitude did not compare with that of achievement. The discrepancy

between the gains in achievement and attitude has serious implications

for the implementation of the new mathematics programs. In the first

place, the decision makers should be aware of the existence of the

discrepancy. Secondly, in assessing the new mathematics programs,

the decision makers should take the achievement score as the index of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 102

effectiveness. Thirdly, in the decisions regarding the implementa­

tion of the new mathematics program, they should not be guided by the

score in attitude which partly echoes the deteriorating influence of

the back-to-basics movement.

The implications based on the results which pertained to the

specific variables considered in the study are numerous. An attempt

is made in the following sections to discuss the major considerations

of the important findings. Among the grade levels, the explicitly

superior gains in achievement in the elementary and senior high, in

contrast with the junior high, have a serious implication. The

superior scores suggest that in the process of introducing new mathe­

matics, the most appropriate grade levels are either the elementary

or the senior high. Unfortunately, in the past the majority of the

experiments conducted with modern mathematics were at the junior high

level. Much of the "anti-feelings" about new mathematics might not

have come to the surface, if the new programs were introduced either

at the elementary or senior high levels.

One of the allegations against modern mathematics has been that

it is good only for the highly motivated students who will go on to

major in mathematics in college. The findings of this study proved

the allegation wrong. The average mean scores, when categorized

according to the ability levels, show that although new mathematics

was beneficial for bright students, it was more beneficial for the

slow learners. It will be a serious mistake, therefore, to teach the

new mathematics only to a chosen ability group— a practice that has

been prevalent in the past decades. The consequences would be to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 103

deprive the slow learners of the benefits of the new mathematics.

Moreover, when a choice is available regarding the levels of ability

and introducing new mathematics, the decision should be to support the

low ability groups utilizing new mathematics.

The findings in the general content area are capable of solving

much of the heated controversy over the concept approach and the com­

putational skills. The results indicated remarkably superior achieve­

ment attributable to new mathematics in concepts and computation.

The criticism that modern mathematics is hopelessly beyond young

people because of its abstractness and rigor carries no weight in the

light of these findings. Also, computation with whole numbers is far

from being a lost art. More surprisingly, now Johnny adds better.

Knowing that Johnny adds better makes one skeptical of the back-to-

basics movement.

As the back-to-basics movement gains momentum, one should be

deeply concerned about the dangers it can hold. The movement, insofar

as it emphasizes the computational skills in isolation, might elimi­

nate teaching for mathematical understanding. What good can computa­

tional skills in isolation do, if a student does not know what to

compute when? The modern math has been addressing this problem by

placing the computational skill within a total mathematics program.

The findings of this study supported the assertion that this approach

has been successful, and that it is far superior to the traditional

approach. In reality, going back to basics demarcates a deterioration

in the learning of mathematics.

The results pertaining to application revealed that the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 104

improvement attributable to new mathematics in the area of applica­

tion was remarkably low in comparison with the scores in concepts

and computation. Unless and until mathematics can operate to clarify

and solve human problems, it has indeed a narrow value. Hence, major

emphasis must be placed to strengthen the area of application in the

new mathematics curriculum. As a result, the students should be able

to apply the concepts and skills they learned in the practical field.

The relatively low geometry score in the specific subjects

revealed another area which requires serious consideration. Since

geometry occupies a central position in mathematics, according to

many mathematicians, presenting geometry as meaningful and under­

standable as the rest of the subject areas is a proven necessity to

fulfill the goal of total mathematics program.

On the whole, the new mathematics has been proven to be superior

in all areas investigated in this paper. In a few instances, such

as geometry, application in the generic content area, and methodology

in the general approaches, the improvement attributable to new mathe­

matics is negligible; however, "new math" is still on its way. Fur­

ther, it is hoped that the leaders in the new mathematics curriculum

will recognize these areas as their unfinished task and propose

alternatives for improvement.

General Recommendations

The discussion of recommendations pertains to three points. They

are recommendations for utilization of the study findings, recommend­

ations for further meta-analyses in the field of mathematics, and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 105

recommendations for improved techniques for meta-analysis.

Utilization of the Study Findings

The present study attempted to resolve several problematic issues

pertaining to modern mathematics education. In general, the study

tried to find an answer to the central issue: whether the treatment

of new mathematics resulted in an improvement or decline of mathemat­

ical achievement. The overall analysis of 810 conflicting and dis­

similar findings showed that the "new math" raised a typical student

from the 50th to the 59th percentile of the traditional mathematics

group in achievement. Though to a lesser degree, the introduction of

new mathematics also improved the attitude toward mathematics. The

attitude of a typical student rose from the 50th to the 55th percen­

tile.

The study also showed the relative strengths and weaknesses of

new mathematics in several specific areas. These were categorized

under four sets of variables: subject characteristics, treatment

factors, study attributes, and design variables. The overall find­

ings and the findings in those specific areas will enable mathematics

educators, educational specialists, curriculum developers, classroom

teachers, commercial agencies, and other interested persons or agen­

cies to make sound decisions pertaining to such things as grade,

ability level, socio-economic status, sex, general content area,

specific content area, different subject areas, and type of program

in the process of implementing modern mathematics programs.

In summary, the authorities in mathematics education should

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 106

realize that a great deal of improvement has been made in the learning

of mathematics since World War II. Sputnik is due credit for having

the greatest influence on the new mathematics. It would be unwise to

ignore these benefits derived from new mathematics and rush "back-to-

basics" which holds a promise for only a retarded growth.

Further Meta-analyses on Mathematics

There are many areas in mathematics in which there exists an

abundance of studies with dissimilar findings. Meta-analysis would

prove to be extremely useful for extracting the message of those

diverse findings. A meta-analysis would be indicated in such ques­

tions as the relative effectiveness of the application of Piaget's

theories and the traditional treatment. Likewise, the literature on

instructional television, use of , and such other innova­

tions hold promise for statistical meta-analysis.

Improvements in the Techniques of Meta-analysis

Improvement in the techniques of meta-analysis is another area

of exploration. For example, the mean effect size as defined and

applied in the literature does not enable one to evaluate its signif­

icance. A smaller mean effect size could be of a greater significance

than a larger mean effect size if the number of effect sizes is sig­

nificantly small or the standard deviation is significantly high. An

attempt is made in the present study to evaluate the relative merits

of the mean effect sizes. The t-values and the corresponding proba­

bilities were calculated to find the significance of each mean effect

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 107

size. Since the concept of meta-analysis is relatively recent,

improvements in the techniques such as those discussed above are

quite possible and certainly rewarding.

Summary

During the last three decades many innovations in mathematics

education were introduced under a common label called "new mathemat­

ics." The studies on new mathematics were numerous and their find­

ings were conflicting and dissimilar in nature. No attempt had been

made to summarize those studies in order to extract the message of

the diverse findings about "new mathematics." The present study

attempted to summarize 134 studies on new mathematics, which were

located mainly from dissertations, abstracts, journal articles, and

ERIC documents.

In order to explain the term "new mathematics," the author

traced the origin and evolution of the movement of modern mathematics.

There were two critical events, World War II and the Russian Sputnik,

which gave great stimuli for reform in mathematics education. The

reform movement was initiated mostly by developmental projects which

emerged during the 1950's and early part of the I960's. The role of

the developmental programs was to correct perceived weakness in

school programs, such as to improve materials, to train teachers, and

most importantly to introduce new concepts in the area of content,

methodology, and materials in the treatment of mathematics. The new

concepts placed great emphasis on logical thinking, structure, and

abstractedness, instead of drill and rote of the traditional

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 108

mathematics. Innovations of these developmental programs were known

under a common label, "new math."

The facts about the conflicting nature of research findings and

public opinions about the outcomes of new mathematics, coupled with

the scarcity of summative research resolving the dissimilar and con­

flicting findings, presented the problem for which the study was

intended. Research questions were formulated to answer the problem­

atic issues in the new mathematics curriculum. The issues pertained

to the examination of overall outcome— decline or growth in the learn­

ing of mathematics— attributable to modern mathematics.

Since the study adopted a new technique for summarizing the

aggregate findings about new mathematics, the rationale for the study

focused on two categories of topics: the justification of the research

topic and the justification of the research tool. To justify the

topic, three major areas in the literature of modern mathematics were

examined. They were: (a) appraisals of modern mathematics, (b)

studies on modern mathematics by primary analysis, and (c) studies on

modern mathematics by aggregate analysis. Treatment on appraisals of

modern mathematics disclosed conflicting viewpoints regarding the

efficacy of modern mathematics. To make a sound judgment on the effi­

cacy of modern mathematics, research studies were utilized. An over­

view of the research studies on modern mathematics by primary analysis

revealed that there were numerous studies in that area; however, the

findings were diverse. Hence, an aggregate analysis was deemed to be

necessary in order to resolve the conflicting findings. The existing

aggregate analysis did not strictly pertain to the area of investigation

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 109

of the present study. Therefore, the researcher attempted to utilize

an aggregate analysis methodology specifically to resolve the con­

flicting findings about new mathematics.

The literature search for techniques of aggregation exposed five

different methodologies used in the past. They were narratives,

reviews, the averaging method, the voting method, and the cluster

approach. The drawbacks of the review methodology were: (a) it did

not evaluate the influence of the independent variables, (b) it

adopted inadequate techniques as means of aggregation, and (c) it

eliminated the so called "poorly done" studies. The weakness of the

averaging method was that it threw away precisely the information

needed most, namely, a true estimate of the treatment effect. The

defects of the voting method were: (a) it disregarded valuable des­

criptive information, (b) it did not integrate the measures of the

strength of experimental effects, and (c) it did not solve the prob­

lem of unbalanced experimental design. The major drawbacks to the

cluster approach were that the study became overly restrictive

because: (a) it required access to raw data, (b) it eliminated

studies which were not measured in a common scale, and (c) it elimi­

nated studies which lacked explanation for differences.

Because of the drawbacks mentioned previously, none of the tech­

niques was found to be adequate for the present analysis. Glass

(1976) introduced another technique of aggregation called meta­

analysis. Meta-analysis, in many respects, was an improvement on the

previously discussed techniques.

Meta-analysis methodology stood for a statistical aggregation of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 110

a large body of similar, dissimilar, and conflicting findings without

loss of information. The key concept in the technique of meta­

analysis was the effect size, which was defined as the difference

between the means of the experimental and control groups measured in

standard deviations of the control group. Since the effect sizes

were standardized measures, comparison of different effect sizes were

possible. Practical steps to conduct the present study consisted of

the identification of the sources of the study, locating and collect­

ing the studies, identifying and coding variables, and methods of

analyzing the data.

The overall mean effect sizes of 810 substudies revealed that

modern mathematics showed evidence of change attributable to new math­

ematics programs. The change was reflected in the improvement of

academic achievement and in the improvement of attitude toward math­

ematics. The results also revealed differential effects of specific

characteristics of subject variables, treatment variables, study

variables, and design variables. Spectacular improvements attribut­

able to modern mathematics have been made in the understanding of the

concepts and computational skills. Among the specific subject areas,

algebra had an outstandingly superior achievement score. Of the dif­

ferent approaches of modern mathematics, the one that emphasized the

concrete presentation of abstract ideas was indicated to be the most

rewarding. The major areas which revealed a need for substantial

improvements were geometry among the specific subjects and application

in general content area.

The findings of the study have several important implications for

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Ill sound decision making and needed improvements in many specific areas.

On the whole, the findings of the study strongly encourage the pro­

motion of the new mathematics throughout the nation. At the same

time, the back-to-basics movement should certainly be regarded as an

aberration in the advancement of the learning of mathematics.

In summary, the present study did show evidence that the experi­

ments with modern mathematics of the past three decades in its con­

tent, methodology, materials, or in any combination, have, in general,

led to improved learning of mathematics. This is revealed by the

progress in achievement and attitude toward mathematical concepts,

computation, and application on all grade and ability levels. There­

fore, findings of the present study should motivate the decision

makers to dispel their unfounded uncertainties and misguided appre­

hensions about the effectiveness of new mathematics and to bring

forth the new mathematics to a respectable place in the curriculum

for the benefit of the students and for the betterment of the

society.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. BIBLIOGRAPHY

*Adamson, G. Y. Mathematics achievement between first grade students using developing mathematical process and distar arithmetic math- matics instruction (Doctoral dissertation, Brigham Young Univer­ sity, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1976, 36, 4211A. (University Microfilms No. 76-683)

*Altizer, C. J. The role of manipulatives in learning to multiply and factor polynomials (Doctoral dissertation, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, 1977). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1977, _38, 1951A. (University Microfilms No. 77-22,048)

*Anderson, L. W. A comparison of the effects of two approaches to initial instruction in and in grade three (Doctoral dissertation, The University of Michigan, 1974). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1975, 35, 7178A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 75-10,125)

*Anderson, W. C. The development and evaluation of a unit of instruc­ tion designed to teach second grade children the concept of math­ ematical equality (Doctoral dissertation, The Florida State University, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1977, 37, 6322A. (University Microfilms No. 77-8566)

Anglin, J. B. The value of selected ninth grade tests and algebra grades in predicting success in modern geometry (Doctoral disser­ tation, Indiana University, 1966). Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, 27, 1696A. (University Microfilms No. 66-12,637)

*Archer, J. A. Effect of concrete, semi-concrete, and abstract teach­ ing methods on mathematical achievement, transfer, and retention at the college level (Doctoral dissertation, George Peabody College for Teachers, 1972). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972, ^3, 1580A. (University Microfilms No. 72-25,370)

Archibald, P. L. The status of modern mathematics in grades one through six in the public schools of Mississippi (Doctoral disser­ tation, Mississippi State University, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 2 9 _ , 52A. (University Microfilms No. 68-9414)

Ashlock, R. B., & Herman, W. L., Jr. Current research in elementary school mathematics. New York: Macmillan Co., 1970.

*Entries that are preceded by an asterisk were used as sources of data for the analyses of the present study.

112

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 113 *Ashton, M. R. Heuristic method in problem solving in ninth grade algebra (Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1962). Dissertation Abstracts, 1962, 22^, 4288A. (University Microfilms No. 62-2320)

*Austin, G. R. A longitudinal evaluation of mathematical computa­ tional abilities of New Hampshire's eighth graders. 1963-67. Final report. Durham: New Hampshire University, 1969. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 039 147)

Austin, G. R., & Prevost, F. The results of a longitudinal study in mathematics in New Hampshire. The Mathematics Teacher, 1972, 65, 746-747.

*Ballew, J. H. A study of the effect of upon critical thinking abilities of high school algebra students (Doctoral dissertation, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, 1965). Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, _26, 3775A. (University Microfilms No. 65-14,307)

Bartram, C. E. An analysis and synthesis of research relating to selected areas in the teaching of arithmetic (Volumes I and II) (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, 1956). Disserta­ tion Abstracts, 1956, 16, 2101A. (University Microfilms No. 56-3,334)

*Beckmann, M. W. Ninth grade mathematical competence— 15 years ago and now. School Science and Mathematics, 1969, 6 9 _ , 315-319.

Berger, E. J., & Howitz, T. A. Evaluation of experiences in the mathematical discovery. In J. M. Scandura (Ed.), Research in mathematics education. Washington, D. C.: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 1967.

Betz, W. The present situation in secondary mathematics, with par­ ticular reference to the new national reports on the place of mathematics in education. The Mathematics Teacher, 1940, 33 (8), 339-360.

*Bezdek, J. J. A comparison of two approaches to the teaching of plane and solid geometry (Doctoral dissertation, Cornell Univer­ sity, 1966). Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 28, 150A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 67-1420)

*Biddle, J. C. Effectiveness of two methods of instruction of high school geometry on achievement and retention: A two and one-half year study. School Science and Mathematics, 1967, 6 ] _ , 689-694.

Bidwell, J. K., & Clason, R. G. Readings in the history of mathe­ matics education. Washington, D. C.: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 1970.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 114 *Binter, A. R. A comparison of two methods of teaching the first case on percent (Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas, 1961). Dissertation Abstracts, 1962, 2 2 , 3942A. (University Microfilms No. 62-1851)

*Bluman, A. G. Development of laboratory method of instruction in mathematics at the community college (Doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, 1971). Dissertation Abstracts Inter­ national, 1971, 3 2 , 1970A. (University Microfilms No. 71-26,175)

*Bogart, C. B., Jr. The effect of a pontoon transitional design on the performance of seventh and eighth grade children in a non­ public secondary school (Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, 1971). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972, 3 2 , 4479A. (University Microfilms No. 72-6040)

*Bridges, C. M. The application of elementary statistics in analysis of data by selected secondary school students (Doctoral disserta­ tion, The University of Tennessee, 1959). Dissertation Abstracts, 1959, ^0, 1223A. (University Microfilms No. 59-4254)

*Budgy, J. V. A comparative study of the relative achievement of a first grade group using Greater Cleveland Mathematics Program material and a first grade group using traditional mathematics material (Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers State University, 1968). Dissertation Abstracts, 1969, 29, 2143A. (University Microfilms No. 69-1037)

Buffie, E. G., Welch, R. C., & Paige, D. D. Mathematics: Strategies of teaching. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968.

Burns, P. C., & Dessart, D. J. A summary of investigations relating to mathematics in elementary education: 1965. School Science and Mathematics, 1966, 6 6 , 838-851.

*Bush, W. J. An analysis of the outcomes of functional mathematics and formal mathematics as measured by certain objective tests after completion by the students of two years of study (Doctoral dissertation, University of Arkansas, 1959). Dissertation Abstracts, 1959, 29^, 920A. (University Microfilms No. 59-3036)

Cambridge Conference on School Mathematics. Goals for school mathe­ matics, the report of the Cambridge conference on school mathe­ matics . Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1963.

*Carl, J. C. Effects of the inquiry method on formal thought of community college remedial mathematics students (Doctoral disser­ tation, University of Northern Colorado, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1976, 3 7 _ , 165A. (University Microfilms No. 76-16,267)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 115

*Carlson, P. R. An investigation of the effects of instruction in logic on pupils success in proving theorems in mathematics (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1971). Disser­ tation Abstracts International, 1971, 3 2 , 3148A. (University Microfilms No. 72-342)

Carpenter, T., Coburn, T. G., Reys, R. E., & Wilson, J. W. Results and implications of the ANEP mathematics assessment: Secondary school. The Mathematics Teacher, 1975, 6>8 (6), 457.

*Caruso, G. E. A comparison of two methods of teaching the mathemat­ ical theory of groups, rings, and fields to college freshmen (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, 1966). Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 27, 3769A. (University Microfilms No. 67-4887)

*Cassel, R. N., & Jerman, M. A preliminary evaluation of SMSG instruction in arithmetic and algebra for 7th, 8th, and 9th grade pupils. California Journal of Educational Research, 1963, 14, 202-207.

*Clark, A. C. A creative vs. traditional approach to teaching story problems (Doctoral dissertation, University of Utah, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, ^8, 2929A. (University Microfilms No. 67-17,555)

Clark, J. R. Looking ahead at instruction in arithmetic. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1961, 8^, 388.

*Cronin, R. E. The effects of varying amounts of traditional and modern mathematics instruction relative to sex and intellectual ability on both the traditional and modern mathematics achieve­ ment of eighth grade pupils (Doctoral dissertation, The Catholic University of America, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 28, 2551A. (University Microfilms No. 67-17,139)

*Crowder, A. B. A comparative study of two methods of teaching arithmetic in the first grade (Doctoral dissertation, North Texas State University, 1965). Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, 26, 3778A. (University Microfilms No. 65-15,112)

*Crowder, R. P., Sr. The effect of modern mathematics program on achievement in arithmetic and on intelligence test scores of children in grades four through six in a selected Air Force dependent school (Doctoral dissertation, University of Arkansas, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, J30, 2901A. (University Microfilms No. 70-399)

*Cummins, K. B. A student experience-discovery approach to the teaching of calculus (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State Univer­ sity, 1958). Dissertation Abstracts, 1959, JL9, 2292A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 59-367)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 116

Cunningham, G. The National Observer. March 1, 1965. (Quoted in J. R. Clark, Perspective in programs of instruction in elementary mathematics, The Mathematic Teacher, 1965, _12, 609.)

*Curry, R. D. Arithmetic achievement as a function of concrete, semi-concrete, and abstract teaching methods (Doctoral disserta­ tion, George Peabody College for Teachers, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 31, 4032A. (University Microfilms No. 71-4258)

*Davidson, N. A. The small group discovery method of teaching mathe­ matics instruction as applied calculus (Doctoral dissertation, The University of Wisconsin, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, _31, 5927A. (University Microfilms No. 71-2210)

*Davidson, W. W., & Gibney, T. C. Evaluation of a modern mathematics program. School Science and Mathematics, 1969, 6£, 364-366.

*Davis, J. N. The effects of high school coordinate geometry (Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, 31, 2246A. (University Microfilms No. 70-22,218)

Davis, R. B. Math takes a new path. PTA Magazine, February 1963, 57, 9-10.

Davis, R. B. (Ed.). A modern mathematics program as it pertains to the interrelationship of mathematical content, teaching methods, and classroom atmosphere (The Madison Project). Cooperative Research Project No. D-093, 1965. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 003 371)

Deans, E. Elementary school mathematics; New directions. Washing­ ton, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1963.

DeVault, M. V., & Kriewall, T. E. Perspectives in elementary mathe­ matics. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. Merrill, 1969.

Dieudonne, J. Should we teach "modern" mathematics? American Scientist, 1973, 61, 16-19.

EL-HI text books in print 1978. New York and London: R. R. Bowker Co.

*Erlckson, G. L., & Ryan, J. J. A study of the effects of experi­ mental programs on pupil achievement observed during first three years of the project, secondary mathematics evaluation project. Interim report. St. Paul: Minnesota State Department of Educa­ tion, 1966. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 011 977)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 117

*Evanovich, G. P. A finite mathematical structure for students' secondary trigonometry. A study of the effects upon achievement of students utilizing the finite structure as an introduction to trigonometry (Doctoral dissertation, Temple Univrersity, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1975, 36, 3475A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 75-28,108)

Fehr, H. F. Reorientation in mathematics education. Teachers College Record, 1953, _54, 430-439.

Ferguson, W. E. Current reforms in mathematics curricula— A passing phase or progress? The Mathematics Teacher, 1964, _57, 143.

*Findley, R. E. An evaluation of the effectiveness of a textbook, advanced general mathematics, used by ninth grade general mathe­ matics classes (Doctoral dissertation, Colorado State College, 1966). Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 27^, 2440A. (University Microfilms No. 67-1109)

*Fleckman, B. Improvement of learning division through use of the discovery method (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1966). Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 27, 3366A. (University Microfilms No. 67-3545)

*Frand, J. L. Mathematical understanding and mathematical proficiency: An analysis of alternative goals and programs in mathematics education (Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1976, 37, 2695A. (University Microfilms No. 76-25,195)

*Friebel, A. C. Measurement understanding in modern school mathemat­ ics. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1967, 14, 476-480.

*Fullerton, C. K. A comparison of the effectiveness of two prescribed methods of teaching multiplication of whole numbers (Doctoral dissertation, State University of Iowa, 1955). Dissertation Abstracts, 1955, JJ5, 2126A. (University Microfilms No. 14-107)

*Gardner, H. F. A comparison of the modern and traditional mathe­ matics curriculums and their effects on standardized test scores (Doctoral dissertation, Southern Illinois University, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, 3^0, 2847A-2848A. (University Microfilms No. 70-428)

Glass, G. V. Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis of research. Educational Researcher, 1976, .5 (10), 3-8.

Glass, G. V. Integrating findings: The meta-analysis of research. A chapter in Review of Research in Education, in press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 118

Glass, G. V., & Smith, M. L. Meta-analysis of psychotherapy out­ come studies. Paper presented at annual meeting for Society for Psychotherapy Research, San Diego, Calif., 1976.

*Gopal Rao, G.S., Penfold, D. M., & Penfold, A. P. Modern and tradi­ tional methematics thinking. Educational Research, 1970, 13, 61-65.

*Gore, J. A. Development and preliminary evaluation of a logic unit for college freshmen mathematics courses (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts Interna­ tional, 1969, 30, 5342A. (University Microfilms No. 70-10,190)

*Grafft, W. D., & Ruddell, A. K. Cognitive outcomes of the SMSG mathematics program in grades 4, 5, and 6. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1968, 15, 161-165.

*Gravel, H. Teaching mathematical relations at the grade six level (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, 1968). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 2 3 _ , 1473A. (University Microfilms No. 68-11,133)

*Greabell, L. The effect of three different methods of implementa­ tion of mathematics programs in children's achievement in mathe­ matics. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1969, 1 6 , 288-291.

Greater Cleveland Mathematics Program. Key Topics in Mathematics for the Primary Teacher. Chicago: S.R.A. Inc., 1961-62.

*Greathouse, J. J. An experimental investigation of relative effec­ tiveness among three different arithmetic teaching methods (Doctoral dissertation, The University of New Mexico, 1965). Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, 2 6 , 5913A. (University Microfilms No. 66-4465)

*Greitzer, S. L. A comparison of the postulational approach and the traditional approach in teaching selected topics in algebra to above average students (Doctoral dissertation, Yeshiva University, 1959). Dissertation Abstracts, 1960, _21, 1137A. (University Microfilms No. 60-02108)

Grossnickle, F. E. In answer to your questions. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1964, 11, 499.

*Hall, W. D. A study of relationship between estimation and mathe­ matical problem solving among fifth grade students (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, 1976) Dissertation Abstracts International, 1977, 3 7 _ , 6324A. (University Microfilms No. 77-9014)

Hannon, H. A new look at content and its placement in elementary mathematics. School Sciences and Mathematics, 1959, 59^, 614.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. *Hanson, L. E. Inductive discovery learning, reception learning, and formal verbalization of mathematical concepts (Doctoral disserta­ tion, Florida State University, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 28, 1731A. (University Microfilms No. 67-14,451)

*Harper, E. H. The identification of socio-economic differences and their effect on the teaching of readiness for "new math concepts" in the kindergarten. Final Report, 1970, The University of Wisconsin. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 040 852)

Harper, K. J. A comparison of three elementary mathematics programs, a model for curriculum evaluation (Doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1972). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972, _33, 6059A. (University Microfilms No. 73-13,176)

Harshmon, H. W. The effects of manipulative materials on arithmetic achievement of first-grade pupils. Unpublished doctoral disserta­ tion, University of Michigan, 1962. Dissertation Abstracts, 23, (July, 1962), p. 150.

Hartley, S. S. Meta-analysis of the effects of individually paced instruction in mathematics (Doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado, 1977). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1978, 38, 4003A. (University Microfilms No. 77-29,926)

Hartung, M. L. Modern methods and current criticisms of mathematics education. School Science and Mathematics, 1955, j)5, 90.

*Heine, B. An investigation of the effect of teaching elementary mathematical logic in problem solving ability of the fifth grade students (Doctoral dissertation, Temple University, 1972) . Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972, 33, 1587A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 72-27,196)

*Herbeck, C. A. Experimental study of the effect of two proof for­ mats in high school geometry on critical thinking and selected student attitude (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, 1972). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1973, 33, 4243A. (University Microfilms No. 73-2013)

^Hershberger, L. D. A comparison of two methods of teaching selected topics in plane and solid analytic geometry (Doctoral dissertation, Florida State University, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts Inter­ national, 1971, _31, 4622A. (University Microfilms No. 71-7030)

*Higgins, J. E. The effects of non-decimal numeration instruction on mathematical understanding (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, 31, 2791A. (University Microfilms No. 70-23,366)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 120 Hill, S. Issues from the National Advisory Committee on Mathematics Education (NACOME) report. The Mathematics Teacher, 1976, 69, 440-446.

*Hollis, L. Y. A study to compare the effects of teaching first grade mathematics by the Cuisenaire-Gattegno method with the traditional method (Doctoral dissertation, Texas Technological College, 1964). Dissertation Abstracts, 1965, 2 6 , 904A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 65-1458)

*Huber, J. C. A comparative study of traditional and transformational approaches to selected topics in trigonometry (Doctoral disserta­ tion, University of Houston, 1977). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1977, _38, 2628A. (University Microfilms No. 77- 24,431)

*Hungerman, A. D. Achievement and attitude of sixth grade pupils in conventional and traditional mathematics programs. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1967, 14, 30-39.

*Jackson, R. L. Numeration systems: An experimental study of achievement on selected objectives of mathematics education resulting from the study of different numeration systems (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1965). Disserta­ tion Abstracts, 1966, 2 6 , 5292A. University Microfilms No. 65- 15,267)

*Johnson, R. E. The effect of activity oriented lessons on the achievement and attitudes of seventh grade students in mathemat­ ics (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 3 2 , 305A. (University Microfilms No. 71-18,755)

Johntz, W. F. Innovation and the new concern for the disadvantaged. California Teachers Association Journal, 1967, _32^ 63; 65-66.

*Kavett, P. F. A study of the teaching of non-decimal system of numeration in the elementary school (Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers State University, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts Inter­ national, 1969, 30, 1468A. (University Microfilms No. 69-16,492)

*Keese, E. E. A study of the creative thinking ability and student achievement in mathematics using discovery and expository methods of teaching (Doctoral dissertation, George Peabody College for Teachers, 1972). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972, 33, 1589A. (University Microfilms No. 72-25,392)

*Keller, J. R. The effect of studying a unit of finite fields on the performance in intermediate algebra at the college level (Doctorate dissertation, West Virginia University, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1977, 3 ] _ , 2038A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 76-22,435)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 121

Kennedy, J. Administrative practices in secondary mathematics curriculum experiments. School Science and Mathematics, 1961, 56, 34-35.

*King, R. The theory of denominator sets as a new mode in the presentation and explanation of (Doctoral dissertation, Saint Louis University, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts Interna­ tional, 1977, T7, 7592A. (University Microfilms No. 77-12,111)

*Kirkpatrick, J. E. The use of finite mathematical systems to achieve selected mathematics objectives in grade six (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 3 2 _, 306A. (University Microfilms No. 71-18,761)

*Kleckner, L. G. An experimental study of discovery type of teaching strategies with low achievers in basic mathematics (Doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, 1968). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1969, 30, 1075A. (University Microfilms No. 69-14,534)

Kline, M. The ancient versus the moderns, a new battle of the books. The Mathematics Teacher, (October .1958), jQ, 419.

Kline, M. The status of reform in the mathematics curriculum. California Journal of Secondary Education, (November 1958) 33, 422.

Kline, M. Why Johnny can’t add: The failure of the new math. New York: St. Martin's Press, 1973.

*Klingbeil, D. H. An examination of the effects of group testing in mathematics courses taught by the small group discovery method (Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, 1974). Disserta­ tion Abstracts International, 1975, jS6, 849A. (University Microfilms No. 75-17,886)

*Kort, A. P. Transformation vs. nontransformation of tenth grade geometry effects on retention of geometry and on transfer in eleventh grade mathematics (Doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, 1971). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 32, 3157A. (University Microfilms No. 71-30,860)

*Kratzer, R. 0. A comparison of initially teaching Greenwood algorithm with the aid of manipulative material (Doctoral disserta­ tion, New York University, 1971). Dissertation Abstracts Interna­ tional, 1972, 3 2 , 5672A. (University Microfilms No. 72-11,464)

*Krogness, J. A comparison of three sixth grade programs of mathemat­ ics instruction (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1967, 30, 1242B. (University Microfilms No. 69-15,162)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 122

*Lackner, L. M. The teaching of the limit and derivative concepts in beginning calculus by combinations of inductive and deductive approaches (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, 1968). Dissertation Abstracts, 1969, 29, 2150A. (University Microfilms No. 69-1378)

*Larsen, C. M. The heuristic standpoint in teaching of elementary calculus (Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1960). Dissertation Abstracts, 1961, 21, 2632A. (University Microfilms No. 60-6740)

Light, R. J., & Smith, P. V. Accumulating evidence: Procedures for resolving contradictions among different research studies. Harvard Educational Review, 1971, 41, 429-471.

Loman, M. L. An experimental evaluation of two curriculum designs for teaching first year algebra in a ninth grade class (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oklahoma, 1961). Dissertation Abstracts, 1961, 2 2 :, 502A. (University Microfilms No. 61-2864)

MacLane, S. The impact of modern mathematics. National Association of Secondary School Principal Bulletin, May, 1954, 38, 70.

*Maetsky, E. M. The relative merits of several methods of teaching probability in elementary statistics (Doctoral dissertation, New York University, 1961). Dissertation Abstracts, 1961, 2 2 , 503A. (University Microfilms No. 61-2559)

Mangrum, C. T., II, & Knight, C. W., II. Doctoral dissertation research in science and mathematics reported for volume 30 of dissertation abstract. School Science and Mathematics, 1972, _72, 505-534.

Mangrum, C. T., & Morris, W. Doctoral dissertation research in science and mathematics. School Science and Mathematics, 1969, 69, 431-449.

*Mastain, R. K. A comparison of the effect of two mathematics curricula on the mathematical understanding of fourth grade pupils (Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, 1964). Dissertation Abstracts, 1964, 25, 3439A. (University Microfilms No. 64-13,531)

*McAloon, M. D. An exploratory study on teaching units in logic to grades three and six (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1969, 30, 1918A. (University Microfilms No. 69-18,055)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 123 ^McLaughlin, J. P. Mathematics achievement in relation to textbook communication style and ability group membership: A comparison of two seventh grade mathematics textbooks (Doctoral dissertation, The University of Texas at Austin, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, 30, 2911A. (University Microfilms No. 69-21,859)

*McLure, C. W. Effectiveness of laboratories for eighth graders (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, 1971). Disserta­ tion Abstracts International, 1972, 32., 4078A. (University Microfilms No. 72-4565)

Meril, D. M., & Wood, N. D. Journals for Research in Mathematics Education, The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Inc. 1973, 1974, 1975, 1976, and 1977.

Midlands Mathematical Experiment Report 1963-65. London: George G. Harrap & Co., 1967.

*Miller, H. F. The comparison of guess-and-check and multi-equation methods for deriving the equations for verbal problems in ele­ mentary algebra (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, 1959). Dissertation Abstracts, 1959, 20, 2180A. (University Microfilms No. 59-05866)

*Moliver, M. A program in probability for non-college bound students in the tenth grade general mathematics (Doctoral dissertation, Temple University, 1977). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1977, J38, 1955A. (University Microfilms No. 77-21,777)

*Moray, J. Effects of curriculum on mathematics achievement in grades six (Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, JL8, 4538A. (University Microfilms No. 68-5681)

*Morris, J. K. A comparison of an inductive and deductive procedures of teaching in a college mathematics course for prospective elementary teachers (Doctoral dissertation, North Texas State University, 1973). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1974, 35, 308A. (University Microfilms No. 74-14,824)

Mueller, F. J. The forest or the trees. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1962, 9, 306-307.

*Muller, D. J. Logic and the ability to prove theorems in geometry (Doctoral dissertation, The Florida State University, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1975, 3 6 , 51A. (University Microfilms No. 75-17,944)

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. An analysis of new mathematics programs. Washington, D. C.: NCTM, 1963.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 124

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. 32nd Yearbook: A education in the United States and Canada. Washington, D. C.: NCTM, 1970.

*Nichols, D. E. Comparison of two approaches to the teaching of selected topics in plane geometry (Doctoral dissertation, Univer­ sity of Illinois, 1956). Dissertation Abstracts, 1956, 1(5, 21906A. (University Microfilms No. 18,178)

Norland, C. R. Mathematics achievement: Changes in achievement scores for grades six and eight after instruction in modern mathematics programs for four years or more, 1969 (Doctoral dissertation, Northern Illinois University, 1971). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, _32, 2363A. (University Microfilms No. 71-29,821)

Northcutt, M. P. The comparative effectiveness of classroom and programmed instruction in the teaching of decimals to fifth grade students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, George peabody College for Teachers, 1963. Dissertation Abstracts, (June, 1964) 24, pp. 5091-5092.

*Nystrom, N. K. An experimental study to compare the relative effects of two methods of instruction on learning of intermediate algebra (Doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, 1969). Disserta­ tion Abstracts, 1969, 2 9 _ , 3532A. (University Microfilms No. 69- 5715)

*0'Brien, T. C. An experimental investigation of a new approach to the teaching of decimals (Doctoral dissertation, New York Univer­ sity, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 2 8 , 4541A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 69-6164)

Organization for European Economic Cooperation, Office for Scientific and Technical Thinking Personnel. New thinking in school mathe­ matics . May, 1961.

Osborne, K. H. A longitudinal study of achievement in and attitude toward mathematics of selected students using school mathematics study group materials (Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley, 1965). Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, 26, 7119. (University Microfilms No. 66-3531)

*Pate, R. T. A study of transactional pattern differences between school mathematics study group classes and traditional mathemat­ ics classes (Doctoral dissertation, The University of Oklahoma, 1964). Dissertation Abstracts, 1964, _25, 3442A. (University Microfilms No. 64-13,333)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 125

*Pavlick, F. M., Jr. The use of advanced sets in the tea-hing of limits: A comparative study (Doctoral dissertation, Florida State University, 1968). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 2 9 , 518A. (University Microfilms No. 68-11,683)

*Payne, H. I. A study of students using SMSG and conventional approaches in first year algebra (Doctoral dissertation, Oklahoma State University, 1963). Dissertation Abstracts, 1965, 25, 4481A. (University Microfilms No. 64-8941)

*Peck, H. I. An evaluation of topics in modern mathematics. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1963, 10, 277-279.

*Peterson, J. M. A comparison of achievement in traditional mathe­ matics skills of seventh grade students using three different types of materials— traditional, transitional, and modern (Doctoral dissertation, Utah State University, 1966). Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, T j _ , 2790B. (University Microfilms No.

*Pettofrezzo, A. J. A comparison of the relative effectiveness of two methods of teaching certain topics in solid analytic geometry to college freshmen (Doctoral dissertation, New York University, 1959). Dissertation Abstracts, 1960, 2 0 , 4604A. (University Microfilms No. 60-1096)

*Phelps, J. A study comparing attitude towards mathematics of SMSG and traditional elementary school students (Doctoral dissertation Oklahoma State University, 1963). Dissertation Abstracts, 1964, 25, 1052A. (University Microfilms No. 64-8942)

*Phillips, J. W. Small group laboratory experiences as an alternative to total group instruction for college low achievers in mathe­ matics (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, _31, 5945A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 71-11,406)

Pingry, R. E. For a better mathematics program in the junior high school. The Mathematics Teacher, 1956, _49, 120.

Pollack, H. A film-film text-study. SMSG Report No. 3 . Stanford: School Mathematics Study Group, Stanford University, 1967.

*Possien, W. M. A comparison of the effects of three teaching methodologies on the development of the problem solving skills of sixth grade children (Doctoral dissertation, University of Albama, 1964). Dissertation Abstracts, 1965, 2 ! 5 , 4003A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 64-12,774)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 126

*Post, T. R. The effects of the presentation of a structure of the problem solving process upon problem solving ability in seventh grade math (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, 1967) . Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 28, 4545A. (University Microfilms No. 68-4746)

Price, H. V. Needed research in the teaching of science and mathe­ matics. School Science and Mathematics, 1957, _57, 389.

*Price, J. S. Discovery: The effect on the achievement and critical thinking abilities of tenth grade general mathematics students (Doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1965). Disserta- tion Abstracts, 1966, 2 f i , 5304A. (University Microfilms No. 66-1246)

Rappaport, D. Mathematics— logical, psychological, pedagogical. The Arithmetic Teacher, 1962, j), 67-70.

Regional Orientation Conferences in Mathematics. The revolution in school mathematics. Washington, D. C.: NCTM, 1961.

*Reimer, D. D. The effectiveness of a guided discovery method of teaching in college mathematics course for non-mathematics and non-science majors (Doctoral dissertation, North Texas State University, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1969, 30, 626A. (University Microfilms No. 69-13,220)

*Retzer, K. A. The effect of teaching certain concepts of logic on verbalization on discovered mathematical generalization (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 28, 1351A. (University Microfilms No. 67-11,909)

*Rich, L. W. The effects of manipulative instructional mode in teaching mathematics to selected 7th grade inner city students (Doctoral dissertation, Temple University, 1972). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972, J33, 330A. (University Microfilms No. 72-20,209)

*Robertson, H. C. The effects of the discovery and expository approach of presenting and teaching selected mathematical principles and relationships to fourth grade pupils (Doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, _31, 5278A. (University Microfilms No. 71-8785)

Rosskopf, M. F. Trends in content of high school mathematics in the United States. Teachers College Record, December 1954, 56^, 137.

*Rowlett, J. D. An experimental comparison of direct detailed and direct discovery methods of teaching orthographic projection principles and skills (Doctoral dissertation, University of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Illinois, 1960). Dissertation Abstracts, 1960, 2 0 , 4589A. (University Microfilms No. 60-1684)

*Roy, G. R. The effect of the study of mathematical logic on student performance in proving theorems by mathematical induction (Doctoral dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, _31, 4045A. (University Microfilms No. 70-22,109)

*Ruddell, A. K. The results of a modern mathematics program. The Arith- metic Teacher, 1962, 9 _ , 330-335.

Schaaf, W. L. How modern is modern mathematics? The Mathematics Teacher, February 1964, 5_7, 90.

*Schlinsog, G. W. The effects of supplementing sixth grade arith­ metic instruction with a study of other number bases (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, 1965). Dissertation Abstracts, 1966, 2 6 , 5307A. (University Microfilms No. 66-629)

Scott, L. Trends in elementary school mathematics. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1966.

*Scott, L. F. Summer loss in modern and traditional elementary school mathematics programs. California Journal of Educational Research, 1967, 18, 145-151.

*Scrivens, R. W. A comparitive study of different approaches to teaching the Hindu-Arabic numeration system to third grades (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1968). Disserta­ tion Abstracts, 1968, _29, 839A. (University Microfilms No. 68- 13,400)

*Settle, M. G. The relative effects of introduction in the guess and test procedure on writing relevant equations to verbal problems in algebra I (Doctoral dissertation, The Florida State University, 1977). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1977, 38.> 2633A. (University Microfilms No. 77-24,804)

*Shelton, R. M. A comparison of achievement resulting from teaching the limit concept in calculus by two different methods (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, 1965). Dissertation Abstracts, 1965, 2 6 , 2613A. (University Microfilms No. 65-11,868)

Sherman, H. Common elements in new mathematics programs: Their origins and evolutions. New York: Teachers' College Press, 1972.

*Sherril, J. M. The effects of differing presentations of mathematics word problems upon the achievement of tenth grade students (Doc­ toral dissertation, The University of Texas at Austin, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, _31, 3427A. (University Microfilms No. 71-191)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 128

Shuff, R. V. A comparative study of achievement in mathematics at the 7th and 8th grade levels under two approaches, School Mathe­ matics Study Group and traditional (Doctoral dissertation, Univer­ sity of Minnesota, 1962). Dissertation Abstracts, 1962, 2 3 , 558A. (University Microfilms No. 62-3100)

Shulte, A. P. The effects of a unit in probability and statistics on students and teachers on ninth grade general mathematics (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1967). Disserta­ tion Abstracts, 1968, ^8, 4962A. (University Microfilms No. 68- 7726)

*Simmons, S. V. A study of two methods of teaching mathematics in grades five, six, and seven (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1966). Dissertation Abstracts, 1965, J26, 6566A. (University Microfilms No. 66-2,500)

*Smith, C. W. A comparison of the effect of the activity approach and the lecture approach used in teaching general college mathe­ matics to risk students and the relationship between the two methods and learner characteristics (Doctoral dissertation, Temple University, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1975, J)6, 3476A. (University Microfilms No. 75-28,299)

*Smith, M. A. Development and preliminary evaluation of a unit on probability and statistics at the junior high school level (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1966). Disserta­ tion Abstracts, 1966, 2 J _ , 1723A. (University Microfilms No. 66- 13,620)

*Smith, M. L. A comparison of three methods of teaching freshman mathematics: Lecture, guided discovery and programmed (Doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1976, 3 6 ^ , 5879A. (University Microfilms No. 76-5479)

*Smith, Q. C. A comparison of a heuristic and traditional method of teaching a preparatory course in mathematics to college freshmen and sophomores (Doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri at Kansas City, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, ^8, 3573A. (University Microfilms No. 68-3577)

*Solheim, J. H. The effect of the study of the transformations of the plane on the attitudes of secondary school geometry students (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, 1971). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1972, _32, 3165A. (University Microfilms No. 72-1572)

*Steadman, D. G. An evaluative study comparing achievement and atti­ tude between the SSMGS unified mathematics programs and tradi­ tional accelerated mathematics programs in the Utah county public

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 129

schools, 1974-75 (Doctoral dissertation, Brigham Young University, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1976, _3(5, 4186A. (University Microfilms No. 76-676)

*Strickland, J. F., Jr. A comparison of three methods of teaching selected content in general mathematics (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1968) . Dissertation Abstracts, 1969, 29, 4392A. (University Microfilms No. 69-9525)

*Summers, E. G., & Hubrig, B. Doctoral dissertation research in mathematics reported for 1963. School Science and Mathematics, 1965, 65, 505-528.

Suydam, M. N. A review of research on secondary school mathematics. Columbus, Ohio: ERIC Information Analysis Center for Science, Mathematics and Environmemtal Education, March 1972.

Suydam, M. N. A categorized listing of research on mathematics education (K-12, 1964-1973. Columbus, Ohio: ERIC Information Analysis Center, The Ohio State University, 1974.

Suydam, M. N., & Weaver, J. F. Research in mathematics education reported in 1975. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 1976, I, 193-257.

Thom, R. "Modern" mathematics: An educational and philosophic error? American Scientist, 1971, 5£, 695-699.

*Threadgill, J. M. The relationship of analytic global cognitive style and two methods introduction in mathematics concept attachment (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1977, 17, 5664A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 77-4766)

Thwaites, B. The school mathematics project: The first ten years (1961-1971). Cambridge: University Press, 1972.

*Trafton, P. R. The effects of two initial instructional sequences on learning of the subtraction algorithm in grade three (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, _31, 4049A. (University Microfilms No. 71-4753)

*Tryon, L. A. A comparison of a modern mathematics program and traditional arithmetic in the intermediate grades (Doctoral dissertation, University of Arizona, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 2^8, 3792B. (University Microfilms No. 68-3581)

*Tucker, J. A junior high school level experiment on developing algebra readiness by the use of finite non-numerical algebraic systems (Doctoral dissertation, Auburn University, 1969)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 130

Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, JJ1, 1154A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 70-15,274)

UNESCO. New trends in mathematics teaching. Vol. I, 1966; Vol. II, 1970; Vol. Ill, 1972.

*Uprichard, A, E., & Collura, C. The effect emphasizing mathematical structure in the acquisition of whole number computation skills (addition and subtraction). University of South Florida, 1974. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 088 716)

*UrWiller, S. L. A comparative study of achievement, retention, and attitude towards mathematics between students using spiral home­ work assignments and students using traditional homework assign­ ments in second year algebra (Doctoral dissertation, University of Nebraska, 1971) . Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 32, 845A. (University Microfilms No. 71-19,521)

*Usiskin, Z. P. The effects of teaching Euclidean geometry via transformation on student achievement and attitude in tenth grade geometry (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, _31, 688A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 70-14,670)

*Volchansky, P. R. The effects of two mathematical instruction approaches on analytical cognition (Doctoral dissertation, The University of New Mexico, 1968). Dissertation Abstracts, 1969, 29, 4396A. (University Microfilms No. 69-9273)

*Wajeeh, A. The effect of a program of meaningful and relevant mathe­ matics with achievement of the ninth grade general mathematics students (Doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1976, 3 ] _ , 280A. (Univer­ sity Microfilms No. 76-26,189)

*Walker, J. D. The value of non-Euclidean geometry as an enrichment for superior high school geometry students (Doctoral dissertation, George Peabody College for Teachers, 1973). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1974, 34, 5002A. (University Microfilms No. 74-4635)

*Walton, G. A. A followup study df two methods of teaching mathe­ matics: Traditional versus new math. School Science and Mathe­ matics, 1977, 77_ (3), 251-255.

*Weiner, M. A comparison of the effect of two teaching techniques in developing the functional competence of college students in a first semester course in mathematics (Doctoral dissertation, New York University, 1961). Dissertation Abstracts, 1962, 22, 4298A. (University Microfilms No. 62-1434)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 131

*Wilkinson, G. G. The effect of supplementary materials upon academic achievement in and attitude toward mathematics among eighth grade students (Doctoral dissertation, North Texas State University, 1971). Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 32, 1994A. (University Microfilms No. 71-25,378)

Williams, E. D. Comparative study of SMSG and traditional mathemat­ ics . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1962, pp. 129-130.

^Williams, E. D., & Shuff, R. V. Comparative study of SMSG and tradi­ tional mathematics text material. The Mathematics Teacher, 1963, 56, 499-504.

*Williams, J. D. Modern mathematics, intelligence and basic conceptual skills. Journal of Structural Learning, 1973, 3> (2), 79-88.

*Woodall, P. G. A study of pupils' achievements and attitudes in the school mathematics study group and the traditional mathematics programs of the Lewiston School District, 1960-65 (Doctoral dissertation, University of Idaho, 1966). Dissertation Abstracts, 1967, 2]_, 4040B. (University Microfilms No. 67-5389)

Wooten, W. S.M.S.G. the making of a curriculum. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1965.

*Wozencraft, M. Are boys better than girls in arithmetic? The Arith­ metic Teacher, December 1963, 10, 486-490.

*Wright, R. E. Something old, something new. School Science and Mathematics, 1970, 70, 707-712.

*Yasui, R. V. An analysis of algebraic achievement and mathematical attitude between the modern and traditional mathematics programs in senior high school: A longitudinal study (Doctoral disserta­ tion, University of Oregon, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 28, 4967A. (University Microfilms No. 68-4017)

*Ziebarth, R. A. The effect of experimental curricula on mathematics achievement in high school (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1963). Dissertation Abstracts, 1964, 2 A , 4593A. (University Microfilms No. 64-4114)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. APPENDICES

132

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 133

Appendix A

The List of the Variables Identified

Variable Name Purpose

1. Study number To group the effect One assigned to each sizes by category study

2. Study approach To specify the mod­ 1 = methodology ern mathematics 2 = content treatment in accord­ 3 = materials ance with the emphasis 4 = combination placed on the approach

3. Study type To differentiate 1 = achievement between achievement 2 = attitude and attitude studies

To identify the commercial innovator school district experimenter university other

5. Year of study To determine the year of publication

6. Source of study To determine where : public school report study was located ‘ ERIC : journal articles : dissertations

7. Affiliation of To specify the organi- : staff of a project researcher zation the researcher : school district worked for evaluation staff ; university ; other

8. Involvement of To determine researcher’s : evaluating on researcher personal dependency on material or project the outcome of the study

9. Area of study To define the area of mathematics investigation other subject areas included

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 134

Appendix A— continued

Variable Name Purpose Values

10. Specific sub­ To describe what spe­ 1 = arithmetic ject area cific subject was taught 2 = algebra 3 = geometry 4 = sets 5 = logic 6 = statistics 7 = calculus 8 = trigonometry 9 = other

11. Type of pro­ To identify the spe­ 1 = UICSM gram cific innovative pro­ 2 = SMSG gram 3 = GCMP 4 = MADISON 5 = BCMI 6 = BSU 7 = UMMaP 8 = MINNEMAST 9 = OTHER

12. Length of To determine the dura­ Number of weeks until treatment tion of the treatment posttest

13. Length of To determine the dura­ Days per week treatment tion of the treatment

14. Length of To determine the dura­ Minutes per day treatment tion of the treatment

15. Time To find the total num­ Weeks x Days x Minutes ber of instruction 60 hours

16. Number To find the sample The number of subjects size that means are based on

17. New versus To determine if the 1 = new— never used field-tested treatment was field- before tested or new 2 = field tested

18. Grade To determine the grade 1 = KG or subjects 2 = Elementary 3 = JH 4 = SH 5 - Post-secondary

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 135

Appendix A— continued

Variable Name Purpose Values

19. SES To determine the 1 =unknown socio-economic status 2 = low of subjects 3 = middle 4 = high

20. Ability To determine the ability 1 = unknown levels of subjects 2 = low 3 = middle 4 = high

21. Sex To determine the sex 1 = unspecified of subjects 2 = female 3 male

22. Test To determine the type 1 = commercial of test 2 = experimenter developed 3 = teacher developed 4 = other

23. Retention To identify the length Weeks between end of of retention treatment and testing

24. Effect size To calculate measures effectiveness

25. Method for To determine the tech­ 1 = control group effect sizes nique to be used to 2 = ANOVA measure effect size 3 = gain scores 4 = grade equivalent scores 5 = standardized tests 6 = t-test 7 = F-test 8 = ANCOVA

26. Random assign­ To determine the 1 = random assignment ment quality of design 2 = no random assignment

27. Novelty effect To determine the 1 - present novelty effect 2 = not present

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 136 Appendix A— continued

Variable Name Purpose Values

28. Dependent vari­ To identify overlap­ 1 = unique data able ping studies 2 = subtests when total included 3 = subtests when total not included 4 = total when subtests included

29. General content To determine the gen­ 1 = concepts area eral content area 2 = computation emphasized 3 = application 4 = combination

30. Nature of test To determine the cate­ 1 = retention gory of test 2 = recall 3 = transfer

31. Pool To determine the num­ ber of effect sizes in each study

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 137

Appendix B

Code Sheet of the Variables Identified

Reference

CODE COLUMN VARIABLE VALUE

1- 4 STUDY NO 5 STUDY TYPE 1 Met 2 Cont 3 Mat 4 Comb 6 SUBSTUDY NO 1 Ach 2 Att 7 AUTHOR 1 Com 2 Sc D 3 Exp 4 Uni 5 Ot 8- 9 YEAR OF STUDY 10 SOURCE 1 P Sc 2 ERIC 3 JA 4 Diss 5 Ot 11 STUDY RATING 1 Rand 2 ECG 3 NECG 4 N CG 12 AFFI OF RES 1 SP 2 Sc ES 3 Uni 4 Ot 13 INV OF RES 1 0 wn 2 None 14 SUB AREA 1 Math 2 Sc 3 SS 4 Lang 5 Comb 15 METHOD 1 Reas 2 Act 3 Gru 4 Ot 16 CONTENT 1 Arit 2 Alg 3 Geo 4 Set 5 Log 6 Stat 7 Cal 8 Trig 9 Ot 17 TYPE OF PGM .1 UICSM 2 SMSG 3 GCMP 4 MAD 5 BCMI 6 BSU 7 UMMaP 8 MIN 9 Ot 18-20 LENGTH OF TREATMENT WEEKS 21 LENGTH OF TREATMENT DAYS 22-24 LENGTH OF TREATMENT MINUTES 25-27 TIME WEEKS X DAYS X MINUTES 60 28-31 NUMBER AVERAGE 32 FIELD-TESTED 1 New 2 Field T 3 Unk 33 GRADE 1 KG 2 Ele 3 JH 4 SH 5 Post Sec 6 Ot 34 SES 1 Unk 2 L 3 M 4 H 35 ABILITY 1 Unk 2 L 3 M 4 H 36 SEX 1 Both 2 Male 3 Female 37 TEST 1 Comm 2 Ex D 3 Tea D 4 Ot 38-40 RETENTION TIME WEEKS 41-48 EFFECT SIZE 49 METOD ES 1 CT 2 ANOVA 3 GAIN SC 4 GES 5 ST 6 t-T 7 F-T 8 ANCOVA 9 Ot 50 ASSIGNMENT 1 Rand 2 No Rand 51 NATURE OF TEST 1 Ret 2 Rec 3 Tran 52 NOVELTY EFFECT 1 Yes 2 No 53 DEP VAR 1 Uni 2 STWTI 3 STWTNI 4 TWSI 54 CONT GEN 1 Con 2 Comp 3 Appl 4 Gen 55 TRANSFER TIME 56-60 POOL

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.