Race, Iq and Jensen

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Race, Iq and Jensen WEST LOS ANGELES COLLEGE LIBRARY RACE, IQ AND JENSEN JAMES R. FLYNN Professor of Political Studies University of Otago Routledge & Kegan Paul London, Boston and Henley TO NATALIE t First published in 1980 by Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd 39 Store Street, London WC1E 7DD, 9 Park Street, Boston, Mass. 02108, USA and Broadway House, Newtown Road, Henley-on-Thames, Oxon RG9 1EN Printed and bound in Great Britain by Thomson Litho Ltd, East Kilbride Copyright © James R. Flynn 1980 No part of this book may be reproduced in any form without permission from the publisher, except for the quotation of brief passages in criticism British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data Flynn, James Robert Race, IQ and Jensen. 1. Intellect 2. Ethnopsychology I . T i t l e 153.9'2 BF432.A1 80-49972 ISBN 0 7100 0651 9 CONTENTS PREFACE AND ACKNOWLEDGMENTS v i 1 THE RACIST AND HIS NEED FOR EVIDENCE 1 2 JENSEN AND HIS CRITICS 25 3 DIRECT EVIDENCE AND INDIRECT 72 4 A PROBLEM FOR THE SOCIAL SCIENCES 115 5 A PROBLEM FOR BLACKS 159 6 CONCLUSION AND SUMMARY 213 APPENDIX A BLACK SOLDIERS AND WHITE SOLDIERS 219 APPENDIX B JENSEN VERSUS SANDRA SCARR 262 NOTES 271 AUTHOR INDEX 303 SUBJECT INDEX 307 v PREFACE AND ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This book deals with areas in which research and publica­ tions accumulate swiftly. The reader has a right to know the cut-off date of the author's research: the author stopped reading books on race and IQ, with some pleasure, on 1 January 1980 and published research, with a few ex­ ceptions, on about the same date. No doubt I have missed something of significance and of course I have not used everything read. As for thanking others for their help, I owe much to many but w ill lim it myself to expressing my gratitude to those whom I bothered the most: Prof. John C. Loehlin, Department of Psychology, University of Texas; Dr G.F. Liddell, Mr J.A. Harraway, and Mr B.F.J. Manly, Department of Mathematics, University of Otago; and Mr F.W. Pernell of the National Archives and Records Service, Suitland, Maryland. Prof. Loehlin read a complete draft of the manuscript and I owe him a particular debt for saving me from a number of mistakes and omissions. Arthur R. Jensen was generous enough to grant me access to material from his own most recent book, 'Bias in Mental Testing', prior to its publication. Both Sandra Scarr and Jensen extended the same courtesy by sending me drafts of material to be included in her forthcoming book, 'IQ: Race, Social Class, and Individual D ifferences'. v i C h a p te r 1 THE RACIST AND HIS NEED FOR EVIDENCE Before we get to Jensen and race and IQ, some philosophy and some history. The racist must choose between two options: that people of a certain colour (say black) or appearance ('looks' Jewish) are to be despised or feared or exploited or reJected simply because o f their colour or appearance; that they merit such treatment because their colour or appearance is correlated with certain personal traits. Philosophical analysis shows that the first option is non-viable; history shows that every racist ideologue of any sophistication has seen this and chosen the second. The second option forces him to assert pro­ positions which can be falsified by evidence - and th u s engenders a powerful need for evidence he can use in his own d e fe n c e . Philosophers have argued that whenever a man claims to hold a principle or ideal, we can ask him to universalize it. This sounds very profound, but for our purposes we can treat it as meaning no more than that whenever people use words or make Judgments they must do so with logical consistency. This is true not Just when we talk ethics but also of everyday life. If someone picks up a copy of the London 'Times' and says 'the newspaper has come' and then, the next day says 'the cat is on the mat', he had 1 2 Chapter 1 better explain himself. Unless he does, says something like 'the "Times" is so catty in its editorials that I have a special name for i t 1, he pays the price of confus­ ing whoever hears him. If a man tells his fiancé that he loves steak and then turns up his nose when she cooks it for him, he too had better have an explanation, perhaps that he is feeling ill or is too upset to eat. Then we understand: to like steak one day and not like it the next seemed inconsistent, but now his criteria for liking steak have been elaborated - 'I like it when I am in good health, have not already had it for lunch, it is cooked to my taste, and so forth.' Now that his criteria have been spelled out, his Judgments are at least logically consis­ tent and we can understand him, which is a prerequisite for deciding whether or not we agree with him. IWhen a racist makes Judgments, we can use logic as a powerful weapon to force him to make his ideals clear. When he says black men deserve to be excluded or kept in their place or exterminated, we can ask him whether this is true simply because they are black. For example, if it were a Nazi talking we might say this: assume that through industrial pollution a chemical got in the water supply which turned the skins of a ll Germans permanently black; would they then deserve to be exploited or exter­ minated? He can of course answer in the affirmative with complete logical consistency but this merely shows that while logic is important, it is not everything. The reason a Nazi could not answer as above has nothing to do with logic but with the fact that such an answer carries with it an unacceptable price. It is important to be clear about that price. It is not that only a 'fanatic' could bear to imagine the people he admires dying. Our Nazi might well be proud to see 3 Chapter 1 every last German die in the service of his ideals, let us say to avoid being dominated by an 'inferior' race. The real price he would pay i s that of denying the universal experience of mankind, including himself and his fellow Germans, about what is important in relations between human beings, namely, such things as intelligence, charac­ ter and personal traits. When we interact with people, we do so in terms of whether they are honest or dishonest, generous or miserly, courageous or cowardly, witty o r boring. Our Nazi admires his fellow Germans not simply because they are white but because they are the 'master race', they are (he thinks) more creative, courageous, regal, etc., than the rest of us. In this he is like everyone else: everyone reacts to people in terms of per­ sonal traits; and to deny that these are relevant to our Judgments about the merits of men, to say that colour un­ correlated with personal traits is our criterion, is to treat as unimportant what everyone finds to be important in his everyday experience. There is of course no such thing as an absolute price and there are no logical lim its on human psychology. We may someday find a racist who says that colour uncorrela­ ted with personal traits is his criterion of merit. Let him: his 'ideology' is logically flawless but he has ren­ dered it completely non-viable. A man who tries to build an ideology on a distaste for colour has put himself in the same position as a book reviewer who tells us that his criterion for Judging books is the colour of the binding. You cannot build an ideology on a brute psychological fact. Booth Tarkington speaks of a Frenchman who went beserk every time he heard the word 'camel'. Interesting psychologically but it does not have much ideological potential. I hope no one thinks I am denying any of the 4 Chapter 1 facts of racist psychology. The racist may well hate people simply because they are black and want to see them go u n d er simply because they a r e black; that i s n o t un­ usual but quite common among racists. However, this com­ plex of emotions adds to my thesis rather than the re­ verse: they virtually impel the racist to claim that blackness is correlated with unlovely personal traits. The more he hates black men the more he finds it impos­ sible to say, ' I hate them with all my soul, but they are wiser, stronger, more courageous, more generous than we are.' Hatred of colour for its own sake tends to liqui­ date itself as a criterion of human worth. As a matter of historical record, there has never been a racist ideologue who merely asserted that he hated a particular colour or race. Whether we read Drumont, Gobineau and Treitschke in the nineteenth century, o r Chamberlain, Rosenberg and H itler in the twentieth, their books are full of the connection between race and personal traits. To take H itler as an example, we find that Jews are selfish, filthy, dishonest, cowardly (1) - a l s o greedy, heartless, and unscrupulous. (2) They tend to be bow-legged and have a distinctive body odour repugnant to Gentiles, although they often conceal this with per­ fumes. (3) They take a special delight in raping Gentile girls so as to defile the race.
Recommended publications
  • Future Efforts in Flynn Effect Research: Balancing Reductionism with Holism
    J. Intell. 2014, 2, 122-155; doi:10.3390/jintelligence2040122 OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Article Future Efforts in Flynn Effect Research: Balancing Reductionism with Holism Michael A. Mingroni Newark, Delaware, USA; E-Mail: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-302-753-3533 External Editor: Joseph L. Rodgers Received: 13 February 2014: in revised form 2 October 2014 / Accepted: 2 October 2014 / Published: 15 October 2014 Abstract: After nearly thirty years of concerted effort by many investigators, the cause or causes of the secular gains in IQ test scores, known as the Flynn effect, remain elusive. In this target article, I offer six suggestions as to how we might proceed in our efforts to solve this intractable mystery. The suggestions are as follows: (1) compare parents to children; (2) consider other traits and conditions; (3) compare siblings; (4) conduct more and better intervention programs; (5) use subtest profile data in context; and (6) quantify the potential contribution of heterosis. This last section contains new simulations of the process of heterosis, which provide a plausible scenario whereby rapid secular changes in multiple genetically influenced traits are possible. If there is any theme to the present paper, it is that future study designs should be simpler and more highly focused, coordinating multiple studies on single populations. Keywords: Flynn effect; intelligence; secular trend; heterosis 1. Introduction It has been nearly thirty years since James Flynn brought widespread attention to the occurrence of rapid gains in IQ test scores [1,2]. However, in honestly assessing the situation today one would have to conclude that we are not much closer to identifying the cause than we were three decades ago.
    [Show full text]
  • The Black-White Test Score Gap: an Introduction
    CHRISTOPHER JENCKS MEREDITH PHILLIPS 1 The Black-White Test Score Gap: An Introduction FRICAN AMERICANS currently score lower than A European Americans on vocabulary, reading, and mathematics tests, as well as on tests that claim to measure scholastic apti- tude and intelligence.1 This gap appears before children enter kindergarten (figure 1-1), and it persists into adulthood. It has narrowed since 1970, but the typical American black still scores below 75 percent of American whites on most standardized tests.2 On some tests the typical American black scores below more than 85 percent of whites.3 1. We are indebted to Karl Alexander, William Dickens, Ronald Ferguson, James Flynn, Frank Furstenberg, Arthur Goldberger, Tom Kane, David Levine, Jens Ludwig, Richard Nisbett, Jane Mansbridge, Susan Mayer, Claude Steele, and Karolyn Tyson for helpful criticisms of earlier drafts. But we did not make all the changes they suggested, and they are in no way responsible for our conclusions. 2. These statistics also imply, of course, that a lot of blacks score above a lot of whites. If the black and white distributions are normal and have the same standard deviation, and if the black-white gap is one (black or white) standard deviation, then when we compare a randomly selected black to a randomly selected white, the black will score higher than the white about 24 percent of the time. If the black-white gap is 0.75 rather than 1.00 standard deviations, a randomly selected black will score higher than a randomly selected white about 30 percent of the time.
    [Show full text]
  • The Flynn / Non-Flynn Phenomenon and the Digitized Educational System
    ERD 2016: Education, Reflection, Development, Fourth Edition The Flynn / Non-Flynn Phenomenon and the digitized Educational System Claudia Talasman-Chioreana* * Corresponding author: Claudia Talasman-Chiorean, [email protected] aPhD Research Assistant, College of Political Sciences, Babes-Bolyai University, str. Traian Mosiu, nr. 71, Cluj-Napoca, Romania Abstract http://dx.doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2016.12.12 Forced to adapt to new realities, the human mind has evolved in a qualitatively superior way from a historical stage to another. This principle is affirmed and demonstrated in several studies known as The Flynn Phenomenon. As a response to these principles, The Non-Flynn Phenomenon claims that in our modern heavily digitized society, IQ has decreased due to the minimal contribution of the human mind in dealing with the new existential problems of humanity. The current trend of substitution of specific human elements by gadgets, software tools or created digital spaces is increasingly obvious. Between The Flynn and non-Flynn Phenomenon, education plays an important role. Education aims efficient use innovative technical means. And this trend may be the solution for the continuous development of IQ. The new technology involves increasing interest, maintaining motivation, widening of the informational horizon, if used properly, without excesses, in combination with other methods and modern and applied educational resources. © 2016 Published by Future Academy www.FutureAcademy.org.uk Keywords: The Flynn Phenomenon, The Non-Flynn Phenomenon, digitized society, IQ, education. 1. IQ and Human Evolution Throughout human evolution, progress has been one of the functional, adaptive constants that man has consistently promoted. Whether we talk about progress at the technical, cultural, social or civic level, the human being has constantly been trying to find new, useful, easy resources in order to get a ratio of maximum benefit and minimal effort.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Intelligence?
    WHAT IS INTELLIGENCE? By one of the most creative and influential psychologists in the field of intelligence comes an unrivalled ‘must-read’ …a brilliant interweaving of data and argument. Intrigued? Read on to find out more about Flynn’s inspiring new book October 2007 • 226 pages • 5 tables • 7 figures Hardback 978-0-521-88007-7 c. £14.99 Do IQ gains mean that each generation is more intelligent than the last? Do they suggest how each of us can enhance our own intelligence? For over twenty years, psychologists have struggled to understand the implications of IQ gains. Professor James Flynn, one of the most creative and influential psychologists in the field of intelligence, is finally ready to give his own views. His answer is both surprising and illuminating. Flynn’s unrivalled book makes an important contribution to our understanding of human intelligence. Contents 1. A bombshell in a 6. IQ gains can kill letter box 7. What if the gains are 2. Beyond the Flynn Effect over? 3. Towards a new theory 8. Knowing our ancestors of intelligence Appendix I: Tables; 4. Testing the Dickens/ Appendix II: Declaration Flynn model in a capital case. 5. Why did it take so long? Find out more or order online at www.cambridge.org/flynn WHAT THE EXPERTS SAY James Flynn has been a saintly anomaly in Flynn provides the first satisfying explanation this contentious debate. His latest book is a classic of the massive rise in IQ test scores... expression of the man – advancing a particular Professor N. J. Makintosh,University of Cambridge point of view, but committed to following the data, responding to criticism with reason and evidence, and unfailingly good-humored.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Ability William T
    Forthcoming in The New Palgrave Dictionary of Economics, Steve Durlauf ed. Cognitive Ability William T. Dickens Senior Fellow The Brookings Institution ABSTRACT Modern psychological theory views cognitive ability as multidimensional while acknowledging that the many different abilities are themselves positively correlated. This positive correlation across abilities has led most psychometricians to accept the reality of a general cognitive ability that is reflected in the full scale score on major tests of cognitive ability or IQ. This entry provides an introduction to the history of cognitive testing and some of its major controversies. Evidence supporting the validity of measures of cognitive ability is presented and the nature and implications of group differences is discussed along with evidence on its malleability. Some people obviously and consistently understand new concepts quicker, solve unfamiliar problems faster, see relationships that others don’t and are more knowledgeable about a wider range of topics than others. We call such people smart, bright, quick, or intelligent. Psychologists have developed tests to measure this trait. Originally called IQ tests (for Intelligence Quotient because the measures were constructed as the ratio of mental age to chronological age multiplied by 100) that name has fallen out of favor. Instead, such tests are now often referred to as tests of cognitive ability. Although the term IQ is still sometimes used to refer to what such tests measure, none construct a ratio. History Spearman (1904) first popularized the observation that individuals who do well on one type of mental task also tend to do well on many others. For example, people who are good at recognizing patterns in sequences of abstract drawings are also good at quickly arranging pictures in order to tell a story, telling what three dimensional shapes drawn in two dimensions will look like when rotated, tend to have large vocabularies and good reading comprehension, and are quick at arithmetic.
    [Show full text]
  • The G-Factor of International Cognitive Ability Comparisons: the Homogeneity of Results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-Tests Across Nations
    European Journal of Personality Eur. J. Pers. 21: 667–706 (2007) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/per.634 The g-Factor of International Cognitive Ability Comparisons: The Homogeneity of Results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-Tests Across Nations HEINER RINDERMANN* Institute of Psychology, Otto-von-Guericke-University Magdeburg, Germany Abstract International cognitive ability and achievement comparisons stem from different research traditions. But analyses at the interindividual data level show that they share a common positive manifold. Correlations of national ability means are even higher to very high (within student assessment studies, r ¼ .60–.98; between different student assessment studies [PISA-sum with TIMSS-sum] r ¼ .82–.83; student assessment sum with intelligence tests, r ¼ .85–.86). Results of factor analyses indicate a strong g-factor of differences between nations (variance explained by the first unrotated factor: 94–95%). Causes of the high correlations are seen in the similarities of tests within studies, in the similarities of the cognitive demands for tasks from different tests, and in the common developmental factors at the individual and national levels including known environmental and unknown genetic influences. Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Key words: intelligence; knowledge; education; student assessment; educational achievement; OECD; IEA INTRODUCTION The past century of empirical research on cognitive abilities has convincingly demonstrated their relevance for both individuals and societies across a wide variety of criteria. For individuals, both IQ (Schmidt & Hunter, 2004) and literacy (OECD, 2000) have been linked with job performance, longevity (Gottfredson & Deary, 2004; OECD, 2000), and low criminality (Herrnstein & Murray, 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • Malcolm Gladwell Looks at Intelligence and IQ Tests
    Malcolm Gladwell Looks at Intelligence and I.Q. Tests In this fascinating New Yorker article, author Malcolm Gladwell begins by describing what he calls the I.Q. fundamentalists – those who believe that (a) intelligence tests measure an identifiable mental trait that predicts the quality of our thinking, and (b) that intelligence is innate – it’s determined by our genes and can’t be changed after birth. Among the fundamentalists are H. H. Goddard (who coined the word moron and wrote, “The people who are doing the drudgery are, as a rule, in their proper places”); Lewis Terman (who predicted that California children with the highest I.Q. scores would grow up to be the most successful in every profession); Arthur Jensen (who said that Head Start wouldn’t work because I.Q. was mostly genetic); Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray (authors of The Bell Curve); Richard Lynn (who believes that Asians are the most intelligent race because of the Himalayas, really cold weather, pre-modern hunting practices, brain size, and specialized vowel sounds); and James Watson (who recently told an English newspaper that he was “inherently gloomy” about the prospects for Africa because its people consistently score lower than Europeans on intelligence tests). But those who believe that intelligence is inborn and fixed, says Gladwell, haven’t contended with some stubborn facts. The most important is that I.Q. improves steadily from one generation to the next. For example, at the beginning of the nineteenth century, the children of U.S. immigrants from southern Italy scored in the high 70s and low 80s on I.Q.
    [Show full text]
  • Iq on the Rise: the Flynn Effect in Rural Kenyan Children
    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article IQ ON THE RISE The Flynn Effect in Rural Kenyan Children Tamara C. Daley,1 Shannon E. Whaley,2 Marian D. Sigman,1,2 Michael P. Espinosa,2 and Charlotte Neumann3 1Department of Psychology, 2Department of Psychiatry, and 3School of Public Health, University of California, Los Angeles Abstract—Multiple studies have documented significant IQ gains dren in eastern Tennessee (Wheeler, 1970). Specifically, youths’ expo- over time, a phenomenon labeled the Flynn effect. Data from 20 in- sure to a more technological and complex visual world through toys, dustrialized nations show massive IQ gains over time, most notably in games, television, and computers may affect performance on IQ tests. culturally reduced tests like the Raven’s Progressive Matrices. To our Images typically seen on cereal boxes, restaurant placemats, and com- knowledge, however, this is the first study to document the Flynn effect puter games resemble the patterns on tests such as the Raven’s matri- in a rural area of a developing country. Data for this project were col- ces, and exposure to these materials may improve performance on lected during two large studies in Embu, Kenya, in 1984 and 1998. tests of visual-spatial abilities (Williams, 1998). Most recently, Flynn Results strongly support a Flynn effect over this 14-year period, with (2000) has argued that environmental and social factors serve as multi- the most significant gains found in Raven’s matrices. Previously hy- pliers of certain cognitive skills and that a model of reciprocal causal- pothesized explanations (e.g., improved nutrition; increased environ- ity therefore best accounts for the massive gains on IQ tests.
    [Show full text]
  • The Theory of Intelligence and Its Measurement
    Intelligence 39 (2011) 171–177 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence The theory of intelligence and its measurement A.R. Jensen ⁎ Univ. of California, Berkeley and The Institute of Mental Chronometry, 3330 Southlake Drive, Kelseyville CA 95451, United States article info abstract Article history: Mental chronometry (MC) studies cognitive processes measured by time. It provides an Received 8 January 2011 absolute, ratio scale. The limitations of instrumentation and statistical analysis caused the early Received in revised form 11 February 2011 studies in MC to be eclipsed by the ‘paper-and-pencil’ psychometric tests started by Binet. Accepted 11 March 2011 However, they use an age-normed, rather than a ratio scale, which severely limits the ability of Available online 22 April 2011 IQ tests to probe the physical basis of differences in cognition. For this reason, Arthur Jensen reinitiated mental chronometry in the 1970s. He designed an apparatus that measures reaction Keywords: time to a task known as the Hick paradigm that requires the testee to respond to a display of 1 Flynn Effect (FE) to 8 lights. Faster decision times were related to psychometric g, with theoretically important Galton Intelligence consequences. He was able to do this, where many other studies had failed, mainly because his IQ apparatus clearly separated movement (MT) from reaction time (RT, also called ‘decision Mental chronometry time’.) Interestingly, while RT is clearly related to IQ, MT is not. Principal components analysis Movement time (MT) reveals RT to be a cognitive variable and MT a motor variable. Failure to distinguish between Nisbett them drastically obscures the correlation between composite RT (i.e., RT+MT) and cognitive Psychometrics variables.
    [Show full text]
  • Flynn Et Al. 2014
    Intelligence 44 (2014) 1–10 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses James R. Flynn a,⁎,1, Jan te Nijenhuis b,1, Daniel Metzen b a Departments of Political Studies and Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand b Work and Organizational Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands article info abstract Article history: When IQ tests are ranked by the magnitude of their score gains over time, this hierarchy lacks a Received 20 October 2013 positive correlation with the same tests ranked by their g loadings. Therefore, Jensen declared Received in revised form 29 January 2014 IQ gains “hollow” and, by implication, extended this judgment to score gains that indicated Accepted 30 January 2014 that blacks had made IQ gains on whites. We offer four exploratory meta-analyses that apply Available online 25 February 2014 Jensen's method to the subtest score differences between normal subjects and those suffering from certain afflictions: iodine deficiency (K =6,N = 196), prenatal cocaine exposure (K =2, Keywords: N = 215), fetal alcohol syndrome and degree of fetal alcohol syndrome (respectively, K =1, Flynn effect N =110;andK =3,N = 125), and traumatic brain injury (K =14,N = 629). All of these Jensen effect create a substantial cognitive deficit in those afflicted. However, the correlations between Method of correlated vectors subtest score differences and g loadings run from −0.23 to +0.12, with an unweighted Significance of IQ gains over time Significance of black IQ gains on whites average of 0.00.
    [Show full text]
  • Life, Death, and IQ
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Cleveland-Marshall College of Law Cleveland State University EngagedScholarship@CSU Cleveland State Law Review Law Journals 2011 Life, Death, and IQ: It's Much More than Just a Score: Understanding and Utilizing Forensic Psychological and Neuropsychological Evaluations in Atkins Intellectual Disability/Mental Retardation Cases John Matthew aF bian William W. Thompson Jeffrey B. Lazarus Follow this and additional works at: https://engagedscholarship.csuohio.edu/clevstlrev Part of the Criminal Law Commons, and the Law and Psychology Commons How does access to this work benefit oy u? Let us know! Recommended Citation John Matthew Fabian, William W. Thompson, and Jeffrey B. Lazarus, Life, Death, and IQ: It's Much More than Just a Score: Understanding and Utilizing Forensic Psychological and Neuropsychological Evaluations in Atkins Intellectual Disability/Mental Retardation Cases, 59 Clev. St. L. Rev. 399 (2011) available at https://engagedscholarship.csuohio.edu/clevstlrev/vol59/iss3/7 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Law Journals at EngagedScholarship@CSU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Cleveland State Law Review by an authorized editor of EngagedScholarship@CSU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. LIFE, DEATH, AND IQ: IT’S MUCH MORE THAN JUST A SCORE: UNDERSTANDING AND UTILIZING FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGICAL AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EVALUATIONS IN ATKINS INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY/MENTAL RETARDATION CASES JOHN MATTHEW FABIAN, PSY. D., J.D, ABPP* WILLIAM W. THOMPSON, IV, ASSISTANT PUBLIC DEFENDER+ JEFFREY B. LAZARUS, ATTORNEY AT LAW∇ I. STANDARDS FOR ATKINS HEARINGS....................................
    [Show full text]
  • Appendix to the Petition for a Writ of Certiorari
    No. ------ 3Jn tbe ~upreme <!Court of tbe fflniteb ~tate~ KENNETH DARCELL QUINCE A/K/A RA.SIKH ABDUL-HAKIM, Petitioner, V. STATp OF FLORIDA, Respondent. ON PETITION FOR A WRIT OF CERTIORARI TO THE SUPREME COURT OF FLORIDA APPENDIX TO THE PETITION FOR A WRIT OF CERTIORARI DEA TH PENALTY CASE RAHEELA AHMED COUNSEL OF RECORD FLORIDA BAR No. 0713457 [email protected] MARIA CHRISTINE PERINETTI FLORIDA BAR No. 0013837 [email protected]. US LISA MARIE BORT FLORIDA BAR NO; 0119074 [email protected] ASSISTANT CCRCs LAW OFFICE OF THE CAPITAL COLLATERAL REGIONAL COUNSEL - MIDDLE REGION 12973 NORTH TELECOM PARKWAY, TEMPLE TERRACE, FLORIDA 33637-0907 (813) 558-1600 Counsel for the Petitioner, Kenneth D. Quince. CONTENTS OF APPENDIX INDEX TO APPENDICES Appendix A. Revised opinion of the Supreme Court of Florida affirming the lower court's denial of relief. Quince v. State, Appeal No. SCI 7-127 (April 12, 2018). Appendix B. Order of the Supreme Court of Florida granting the State's motion for clarification and denying Quince's motion for rehearing. Quince v. State, Appeal No. SCI 7-127 (April 12, 2018). Appendix C. Quince's motion for rehearing filed before the Supreme Court of Florida. Quince v. State, Appeal No. SCI 7-127 (February 1, 2018). Appendix D. State of Florida's motion for clarification filed before the Supreme Court of Florida. Quince v. State, Appeal No. SCI 7-127 (January 23, 2018). Appendix E. Initial opinion of the Supreme Court of Florida affirming the lower court's denial ofrelief. Quince v.
    [Show full text]